Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-qsmjn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T00:42:07.455Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Voice onset time in consonant cluster production by children and adults*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Paula Menyuk
Affiliation:
School of Education, Boston University, and Research Laboratory of Electronics, Massachusetts Institute of Technology
Mary Klatt
Affiliation:
School of Education, Boston University, and Research Laboratory of Electronics, Massachusetts Institute of Technology

Abstract

Voice onset time (VOT) characteristics of stops in initial clusters in American English words produced by children and adults were studied. Words were spoken in isolation and in sentence context by eleven three-and four-year-old children, and by a male and female adult. Spectrograms were made and VOT duration measurements taken. Three experienced listeners transcribed the isolated words and sentences. Analyses showed that overall timing characteristics were similar for children and adults. Speakers differed in their [±voice] boundary and there was no absolute time distinction between [±voice] stops; [+voice] stops showed less variability than [−voice]. VOT generally increased from labial to dental to velar clusters, and was shorter in sentence context and longer in clusters than in singletons. Children's VOT averages were generally, but not significantly, longer than adults' in all contexts, and coarticulation constraints affected the accuracy with which children produced the stop and liquid portion of a particular cluster.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1975

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Klatt, D. H. (1973 a). Durational characteristics of prestressed word initial consonant clusters in English. QPR, Research Laboratory of Electronics, M.I.T. 108. 253–60.Google Scholar
Klatt, D. H. (1973 b). Voice onset time, frication and aspiration in word-initial consonant clusters. QPR, Research Laboratory of Electronics, M.I.T. 109. 124–36.Google Scholar
Kornfeld, J. R. (1971). What initial clusters tell us about a child's speech code. QPR, Research Laboratory of Electonics, M.I.T. 101. 218–21.Google Scholar
Lisker, L. & Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: acoustical measurements. Word 20. 384422.Google Scholar
Lisker, L. & Abramson, A. S. (1971). Distinctive features and laryngeal control. Lg 47. 767–85.Google Scholar
McCarthy, D. (1959). Research in language development: retrospect and prospect. Monogr.Soc.Res.Ch.Devel. 24. 324.Google Scholar
Menyuk, P. (1972). Clusters as single underlying consonants: evidence from children's production. In Rigault, A. & Charbonneau, R. (eds), Proc. 7th mt. Cong. Phon. Sd. (The Hague: Mouton). 1161–5Google Scholar
Menyuk, P. & Klatt, D. H. (1968). Child's production of initial consonant clusters. QPR, Research Laboratory of Electronics, M.I.T. 91. 205–13.Google Scholar
Port, D. K. & Preston, M. S. (1972). Early apical stop production: a voice Onset time analysis. Haskins Laboratories Status Report 29/30. 125–49.Google Scholar
Stevens, K. N. & Klatt, D. H. (1974). Role of formant transitions in the voiced–voiceless distinction for stops. JAcS 55. 653–9.Google Scholar