Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-5xszh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T18:28:47.367Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Twelve-month-olds learn novel word–object pairings differing only in stress pattern*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 March 2009

SUZANNE CURTIN*
Affiliation:
University of Calgary
*
Address for correspondence: Suzanne Curtin, Department of Psychology, 2500 University Drive NW, University of Calgary, Calgary, ABCanada, T2N 1N4. tel: 403-220-7670; fax: 403-282-8249; email: scurtin@ucalgary.ca

Abstract

Infants at 1 ; 2 demonstrate difficulty in accessing subtle phonetic information about newly learned word–object pairings (Stager & Werker, 1997). In this study, we examined whether or not infants can access subtle prosodic information such as lexical stress in a word learning task. We tested infants younger than 1 ; 2 to see if they could learn two new word–object associations that differ only in stress pattern (Sww versus wSw). Our results are the first to demonstrate that, even without contextual support, infants at 1 ; 0 succeed at this task, suggesting that the salient acoustic properties associated with lexical stress facilitate word–object associative learning.

Type
Brief Research Report
Copyright
Copyright © 2009 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was supported by a University of Calgary Research Grant (F1031006113). Thanks to Natasha Nickel and especially to Susan Graham for their assistance with this research. Thanks also to the families who participated in this research.

References

REFERENCES

Bailey, T. M. & Plunkett, K. (2002). Phonological specificity in early words. Cognitive Development 17, 1265–82.Google Scholar
Clopper, C. G. (2002). Frequency of stress patterns in English: A computational analysis. Indiana University Linguistics Club Working Papers 2, 19.Google Scholar
Cohen, L. B., Atkinson, D. J. & Chaput, H. H. (2004). Habit X: A new program for obtaining and organizing data in infant perception and cognition studies (Version 1.0). Austin: University of Texas.Google Scholar
Curtin, S., Fennell, C. T. & Escudero, P. (in press). Weighting of vowel cues explains patterns of word–object associative learning. Developmental Science.Google Scholar
Curtin, S., Mintz, T. H. & Christiansen, M. H. (2005). Stress changes the representational landscape: Evidence from word segmentation. Cognition 96, 233–62.Google Scholar
Feldman, H. M., Dollaghan, C. A., Campbell, T. F., Kurs-Lasky, M., Janosky, J. E. & Paradise, J. L. (2000). Measurement properties of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories at ages one and two years. Child Development 71(2), 310–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fennell, C. T. & Werker, J. F. (2003). Early word learners' ability to access phonetic detail in well-known words. Language and Speech 46(2), 245–64.Google Scholar
Hollich, G. J., Hirsh-Pasek, K. & Golinkoff, R. M. (2000). Breaking the language barrier: An Emergentist Coalition Model for the origins of word learning. Monographs for the Society for Research in Child Development 63, Serial Number 262.Google Scholar
Johnson, E. K. & Jusczyk, P. W. (2001). Word segmentation by 8-month-olds: When speech cues count more than statistics. Journal of Memory and Language 44(4), 548–67.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., Hohne, E. A. & Bauman, A. (1999). Infant's sensitivity to allophonic cues for word segmentation. Perception & Psychophysics 61, 1465–76.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., Houston, D. M. & Newsome, M. (1999). The beginnings of word segmentation in English-learning infants. Cognitive Psychology 39, 159207.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W. & Thompson, E. J. (1978). Perception of a phonetic contrast in multisyllabic utterances by two month-old infants. Perception & Psychophysics 23, 105109.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K. (1987). The special-mechanisms debate in speech research: Categorization tests on animals and infants. In Harnad, S. (ed.) Categorical perception: The groundwork of cognition, 355–86. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mani, N. & Plunkett, K. (2008). 14-month-olds pay attention to vowels in novel words. Developmental Science 11(1), 5359.Google Scholar
Mattys, S. L. & Jusczyk, P. W. (2001). Phonotactic cues for segmentation of fluent speech by infants. Cognition 78, 91121.Google Scholar
Pater, J., Stager, C. L. & Werker, J. F. (2004). The lexical acquisition of phonological contrasts. Language 80(3), 361–79.Google Scholar
Polka, L., Sundara, M. & Blue, S. (2002). The role of language experience in word segmentation: A comparison of English, French, and bilingual infants. Paper presented at the 143rd meeting of the Acoustical Society of America: special session in memory of Peter Jusczyk, Pittsburgh, PA, June.Google Scholar
Pruden, S. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M. & Hennon, E. A. (2006). The birth of words: ten-month-olds learn words through perceptual salience. Child Development 77(2), 266–80.Google Scholar
Stager, C. L. & Werker, J. F. (1997). Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word learning tasks. Nature 388, 381–82.Google Scholar
Swingley, D. & Aslin, R. N. (2002). Lexical neighborhoods and the word-form representations of 14-month-olds. Psychological Science 13, 480–84.Google Scholar
Thiessen, E. D. & Saffran, J. R. (2007). Learning to learn: Infants' acquisition of stress-based strategies for word segmentation. Language Learning & Development 3, 73100.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., Cohen, L. B., Lloyd, V., Stager, C. L. & Cassasola, M. (1998). Acquisition of word-object associations by 14-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology 34(6), 1289–309.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F. & Curtin, S. (2005). PRIMIR: A developmental framework of infant speech processing. Language Learning and Development 1(2), 197234.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., Fennell, C. T., Corcoran, K. & Stager, C. L. (2002). Infants' ability to learn phonetically similar words: Effects of age and vocabulary size. Infancy 3, 130.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F. & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development 7, 4963.Google Scholar