Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T02:25:56.428Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Children's phonological neighbourhoods: half empty or half full?*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Christine A. Dollaghan*
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
*
Department of Communication, 3347 Forbes Avenue, Pittsburgh, PA 15260, USA.

Abstract

Charles-Luce & Luce (1990) found smaller phonological similarity neighbourhoods in five- and seven-year-old children's expressive lexicons than in an adult receptive lexicon, a finding they interpreted as evidence that children need not employ fine-grained auditory perceptual analyses in lexical processing. In the present investigation, neighbourhood sizes were calculated for an expressive lexicon derived from two vocabulary lists representative of children aged 1;0 to 3;0 (Rescorla, 1989; Reznick & Goldsmith, 1989). Over 80% of the words in these early lexicons had at least one phonological neighbour; nearly 20% had six or more phonological neighbours. Very young children must have access to reasonably detailed phonological information in order to create and distinguish among such phonologically similar lexical entries.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

I would like to thank Thomas Campbell, Amanda Walley and an anonymous reviewer for their comments and suggestions.

References

REFERENCES

Aslin, R. & Smith, L. (1988). Perceptual development. Annual Review of Psychology 39, 435–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barton, D. (1980). Phonemic perception in children. In Yeni-Komshian, G., Kavanagh, J. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Child phonology, Vol 2. Perception. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Braine, M. (1974). On what might constitute a learnable phonology. Language 50, 270–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carey, S. (1981). The child as word learner. In Halle, M., Bresnan, J. & Miller, G. (eds), Linguistic theory and psychological reality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chapman, R., Dollaghan, C., Kenworthy, O. T. & Miller, J. (1983). Microcomputer testing and teaching of verb meaning: what's easy and what's hard? In Wilkinson, A. (ed.), Classroom computers and cognitive science. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Charles-Luce, J. & Luce, P. (1990). Similarity neighbourhoods of words in young children's lexicons. Journal of Child Language 17, 205–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cluff, M. & Luce, P. (1990). Similarity neighbourhoods of spoken two-syllable words: retroactive effects on multiple activation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16, 551–63.Google ScholarPubMed
Cutler, A. & Norris, D. (1988). The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 14, 113–21.Google Scholar
Eilers, R. (1980). Infant speech perception: history and mystery. In Yeni-Komshian, G., Kavanagh, J., & Ferguson, C. (eds), Child phonology, Vol 2. Perception. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Elliott, L., Hammer, M., Scholl, M. & Wasowicz, J. (1989). Age differences in discrimination of simulated single-formant frequency transitions. Perception and Psychophysics 46, 181–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garnica, O. (1973). The development of phonemic speech perception. In Moore, T. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gleitman, L. & Wanner, E. (1982). Language acquisition: the state of the state of the art. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. (eds), Language acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. & Gee, J. (1987). Prosodic structure and spoken word recognition. Cognition 25, 135–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jusczyk, P. (1992). Developing phonological categories from the speech signal. In Ferguson, C., Menn, L. & Stoel-Gammon, C. (eds), Phonological development: models, research, implications. Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. (1986). Towards a model for the development of speech perception. In Perkell, J. & Klatt, D. (eds), Invariance and variability in speech processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P., Bertoncini, J., Bijeljac-Babic, R., Kennedy, L. & Mehler, J. (1990). The role of attention in speech perception by young infants. Cognitive Development 5, 265–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, P. (1987). Perceptual constancy for speech-sound categories in early infancy. In Yeni-Komshian, G., Kavanagh, J. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Child phonology, Vol. 2. Perception. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ladefoged, P. (1975). A course in phonetics. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Miller, J. & Yoder, D. (1984). Miller– Yoder language comprehension test. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Nittrouer, S. & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1987). The role of coarticulatory effects in the perception of fricatives by children and adults. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 30, 319–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, L. (1989). The language development survey: a screening tool for delayed language in toddlers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 54, 587–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reznick, J. & Goldsmith, L. (1989). A multiple form word production checklist for assessing early language. Journal of Child Language 16, 91100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shriberg, L. & Kent, R. (1982). Clinical phonetics. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Smith, N. (1973). The acquisition of phonology: a case study. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Walley, A. (1987). Young children's detections of word-initial and -final mispronunciations in constrained and unconstrained contexts. Cognitive Development 2, 145–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walley, A. (1988). Spoken word recognition by young children and adults. Cognitive Development 3, 137–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walley, A. (1993). The role of vocabulary development in children's spoken word recognition and segmentation ability. Developmental Review 13, 286350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walley, A. & Metsala, J. (1992). Young children's age-of-acquisition estimates for spoken words. Memory and Cognition 20, 171–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walley, A., Smith, L. & Jusczyk, P. (1986). The role of phonemes and syllables in the perceived similarity of speech sounds for children. Memory & Cognition 14, 220–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werker, J., Gilbert, J., Humphrey, K. & Tees, R. (1981). Developmental aspects of crosslanguage speech perception. Child Development 52, 349–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed