Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-8mjnm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T14:33:28.383Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Acquisition of English grammatical morphology by internationally adopted children from China*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 September 2012

LARA J. PIERCE*
Affiliation:
McGill University, Montreal, Canada
FRED GENESEE
Affiliation:
McGill University, Montreal, Canada
JOHANNE PARADIS
Affiliation:
University of Alberta, Canada
*
Address for correspondence: Lara Pierce, McGill University – Psychology, 1205 Dr Penfield, Montreal, Quebec H3A 1B1, Canada. e-mail: lara.pierce@mail.mcgill.ca

Abstract

Acquisition of English grammatical morphology was examined in five internationally adopted (IA) children from China (aged 0;10–1;1 at adoption) during the first three years' exposure to English to determine whether acquisition patterns were characteristic of child second language (L2) learners or monolingual first language (L1) learners. Results from spontaneous and elicited speech showed that IA children acquired grammatical morphemes similarly to L1 learners; namely, (1) non-tense-marking morphemes were acquired earlier than tense-marking morphemes; (2) BE was acquired in synchrony with other tense-marking morphemes; and (3) a high percentage of omission errors and a low percentage of commission errors were observed.

Type
Brief Research Reports
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This work was supported by funding from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC) to Fred Genesee and an NSERC fellowship to Lara Pierce. The authors wish to acknowledge Kristina Maiorino and Sonia Guerrero for help with data collection and transcription, Alexia Sawyer for help with transcript and coding reliability, and Tamara Sorenson Duncan for assistance with coding procedure. We would also like to thank the children and their parents for their generous time and effort.

References

REFERENCES

Abrahamsson, N. & Hyltenstam, K. (2009). Age of onset and nativelikeness in a second language: Listener perception versus linguistic scrutiny. Language Learning 59(2), 249306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birdsong, D. (2006). Age and second language acquisition and processing: A selective overview. Language Learning 56, 949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, N. J., Lojkasek, M., Zadeh, Z. Y., Pugliese, M. & Kiefer, H. (2008). Children adopted from China: A prospective study of their growth and development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 49, 458–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delcenserie, A., Genesee, F. & Gauthier, K. (2012). Language abilities of internationally adopted children from China during the early school years: Evidence for early age effects? Applied Psycholinguistics 128.Google Scholar
Gauthier, K. & Genesee, F. (2011). Language development in internationally-adopted children: A special case of early second language learning. Child Development 82(3), 887901.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geren, J., Snedeker, J. & Ax, L. (2005). Starting over: A preliminary study of early lexical and syntactic development in internationally adopted preschoolers. Seminars in Speech and Language 26(1), 4453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glennen, S. (2005). New arrivals: Speech and language assessment for internationally adopted infants and toddlers within the first months home. Seminars in Speech and Language 26(1), 1021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glennen, S. (2009). Speech and language guidelines for children adopted from abroad at older ages. Topics in Language Disorders 29(1), 5064.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glennen, S. & Masters, G. (2002). Typical and atypical language development in infants and toddlers adopted from Eastern Europe. American Journal of Speech and Language Pathology 11, 417–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glennen, S., Rosinsky-Grunhut, A. & Tracy, R. (2005). Linguistic interference between L1 and L2 in internationally adopted children. Seminars in Speech and Language 26(1), 6475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haznedar, B. (2001). Acquisition of the IP system in child L2 English. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 23, 139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review 26, 5588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ionin, T. & Wexler, K. (2002). Why is ‘is’ easier than ‘-s’? Acquisition of tense/agreement morphology by child second language learners of English. Second Language Research 18(2), 95136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jia, G. (2003). The acquisition of the English plural morpheme by native Mandarin Chinese-speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 46, 1297–311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, D. & Aronoff, M. (1991). Morphological disintegration and reconstruction in first language attrition. In Seliger, H. W. & Vago, R. M. (eds), First language attrition, 175–88. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krakow, R. A., Tao, S. & Roberts, J. (2005). Adoption age effects on English language acquisition: Infants and toddlers from China. Seminars in Speech and Language 26(1), 3343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P., Williams, K., Lacerda, F., Stevens, K. & Linblom, B. (1992). Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. Science 255, 606608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk, 3rd edn.Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mayberry, R. I. (1993). First-language acquisition after childhood differs from second-language acquisition: The case of American Sign Language. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36, 5168.Google ScholarPubMed
Miyata, S., Hirakawa, M., Ito, K., MacWhinney, B., Oshima-Takane, Y., Otomo, K., Shirai, Y., Sirai, H. & Sugiura, M. (2009). Constructing a new language measure for Japanese: Developmental sentence scoring for Japanese. In Miyata, S. (ed.), Development of a developmental index of Japanese and its application to speech developmental disorders, 1566. Nagoya: Aichi Shukutoku University.Google Scholar
Nicoladis, E. & Grabois, H. (2002). Learning English and losing Chinese: A case study of a child adopted from China. International Journal of Bilingualism 6, 441–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pallier, C., Dehaene, S., Poline, J. B., LeBihan, D., Argenti, A. M., Dupoux, E. & Mehler, J. (2003). Brain imaging of language plasticity in adopted adults: Can a second language replace the first? Cerebral Cortex 13, 155–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradis, J. (2005). Grammatical morphology in children learning English as a second language: Implications of similarities with specific language impairment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 36, 172–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. (2008). Tense as a clinical marker in English L2 acquisition with language delay/impairment. In Gavruseva, E. & Haznedar, B. (eds), Current trends in child second language acquisition: A generative perspective, 337–56. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. (2011). Individual differences in child English second language acquisition: Comparing child-internal and child-external factors. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 1(3), 213–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Rice, M. L., Crago, M. & Marquis, J. (2008). The acquisition of tense in English: Distinguishing child second language from first language and specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics 29, 689722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollock, K. E. (2005). Early language growth in children adopted from China: Preliminary normative data. Seminars in Speech and Language 26(1), 2232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollock, K. E., Price, J. R. & Fulmer, K. C. (2003). Speech–language acquisition in children adopted from China: A longitudinal investigation of two children. Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders 1(3), 184–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice, M. & Wexler, K. (2001). Test of early grammatical impairment. New York: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Tan, T. X. & Yang, Y. (2005). Language development of Chinese adoptees 18–35 months old. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 20, 5768.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werker, J. K. & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behaviour and Development 7, 4963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar