Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c47g7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T23:01:34.859Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Successful aging at 100 years: the relevance of subjectivity and psychological resources

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 July 2015

Lia Araújo*
Affiliation:
UnIFai- ICBAS, University of Porto; ESEV, CI&DETS, Viseu, Portugal
Oscar Ribeiro
Affiliation:
UnIFai, CINTESIS-ICBAS, University of Porto; University of Aveiro; Higher Institute of Social Service of Porto (ISSSP), Portugal
Laetitia Teixeira
Affiliation:
UnIFai, CINTESIS-ICBAS, University of Porto, Portugal
Constança Paúl
Affiliation:
UnIFai, CINTESIS-ICBAS, University of Porto, Portugal
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Lia Araújo, UNIFAI/ICBAS (Research and Education Unit on Ageing), Department of Behavioral Sciences, Institute of Biomedical Sciences Abel Salazar, University of Porto, Rua de Jorge Viterbo Ferreira, 228, 4050-313 Porto, Portugal. Phone: +351220428161. Email: liajaraujo@esev.ipv.pt.

Abstract

Background:

Very old individuals seem to present an admirable capacity to overcome adversities and adapt to the challenges of advanced age. However, studies focusing successful pattern of centenarians found that they may easily fail to be categorized as successful agers when objective criteria are applied. The present study examines if centenarians can be considered successful agers. Following Rowe and Kahn's successful aging model, the primary goal was to clarify whether centenarians are able to be successful agers according to objective and subjective criteria of no major disease and disability, high cognitive and physical functioning and engagement with life. The second goal was to investigate whether socio-demographic factors, psychological, social, and economic resources are related to objective and subjective successful aging profiles.

Methods:

We examined different profiles of successful aging in a high selected sample of individuals aged 100 and more years old from the population-based Oporto Centenarian Study.

Results:

Main findings reveal that centenarians do not represent the prototype of successful aging, but self-ratings demonstrate that many of them feel successful, despite not being objectively considered as so. Those who were considered successful agers presented higher values of self-efficacy, hope, and purpose in life, as well as few difficulties in covering financial expenses.

Conclusions:

As a basis for strengthening the existing model, the value of subjectivity should be explored and psychological resources promoted in interventions to foster positive adaptation in very old age.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © International Psychogeriatric Association 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andersen-Ranberg, K., Schroll, M. and Jeune, B. (2001). Healthy centenarians do not exist, but autonomous centenarians do: a population-based study of morbidity among danish centenarians. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 49, 900908. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2001.49180.x.Google Scholar
Baltes, P. B. and Baltes, M. M. (Eds.) (1990). Successful Aging: Perspectives from the Behavioral Sciences. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bowling, A. and Iliffe, S. (2006). Which model of successful ageing should be used? Baseline findings from a British longitudinal survey of ageing. Age and Ageing, 35, 607614. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afl100.Google Scholar
Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M. and Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: a theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54, 165181. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cho, J., Martin, P. and Poon, L. (2012). The older they are, the less successful they become? findings from the Georgia centenarian study. Journal of Aging Research, 2012, 695854. doi:10.1155/2012/695854.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Depp, C. and Jeste, D. (2006). Definitions and predictors of successful aging: a comprehensive review of larger quantitative studies. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14, 620. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.JGP.0000192501.03069.bc.Google Scholar
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J. and Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 7175.Google Scholar
Fillenbaum, G. G. and Smyer, M. A. (1981). The development, validity, and reliability of the OARS multidimensional functional assessment questionnaire. Journal of Gerontology, 36, 428–34. doi: 10.1093/geronj/36.4.428.Google Scholar
Freund, A. M., Nikitin, J. and Riediger, M. (2012). Successful aging. In Weiner, I. B., Lerner, R. M., Easterbrooks, A. and Mistry, J. (eds.), Handbook of Psychology Vol. 6: Developmental psychology (pp. 615637). London, UK: Wiley.Google Scholar
Gobbens, R. J. and van Assen, M. A. (2012). Frailty and its predictors of disability and health care utilization: the added value of interviews and physical measures following a self-report questionnaire. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 55, 369379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hagberg, B., Alfredson, B., Poon, L. and Homma, A. (2001). Cognitive dunctioning in centenarians: a coordinated analysis of results from three countries. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 56B, 141151. doi: 10.1093/geronb/56.3.P141.Google Scholar
Hank, K. (2011). How “successful” do older Europeans age? Findings from share. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 66B, 230236. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbq089.Google Scholar
INE (2011). Censos 2011. Available at: www.ine.pt; last accessed 6 september 2014.Google Scholar
Jopp, D. and Rott, C. (2006). Adaptation in very old age: exploring the role of resources, beliefs, and attitudes for centenarians´ happiness. Psychology and Aging, 21, 266280.Google Scholar
Lawton, M. P., Moss, M., Hoffman, C., Kleban, M., Ruckdeschel, K. and Winter, L. (2001). Valuation of life: a concept and a scale. Journal of Aging and Mental Health, 13, 331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald, M., Martin, P., Margrett, J. and Poon, L. (2009). Correspondence of perceptions about centenarians’ mental health. Aging & Mental Health, 13, 827837. doi: 10.1080/13607860902918249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, P., MacDonald, M., Margrett, J. and Poon, L. (2010). Resilience and longevity: expert survivorship of centenarians. In Fry, P. and Keys, C. (eds.), New Frontiers in Resilient Aging: Life Strengths and Well-being in Later Life (pp. 213238). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McLaughlin, S. J., Connell, C. M., Heeringa, S. G., Li, L. W. and Roberts, J. S. (2010). Successful aging in the United States: prevalence estimates from a national sample of older adults. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 65B, 216226. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbp101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Motta, M. et al. (2005). Successful aging in centenarians: myths and reality. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 40, 241251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2004.09.002.Google Scholar
Pruchno, R., Wilson-Genderson, M. and Cartwright, F. (2010). A two-factor model of successful aging. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 65B, 671679. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbq051.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Randall, G., Martin, P., MacDonald, M. and Poon, L. (2010). Social resources and longevity: findings from the Georgia Centenarian Study. Gerontology, 56, 106111. doi: 10.1159/000272026.Google Scholar
Reisberg, B., Ferris, S. H., de Leon, M. J. and Crook, T. (1982). The global deterioration scale for assessment of primary degenerative dementia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 11361139.Google Scholar
Reker, G. and Woo, L. (2011). Personal meaning orientations and psychosocial adaptation in older adults. SAGE Open, 1, 110. doi: 10.1177/2158244011405217.Google Scholar
Ribeiro, O, Teixeira, L., Araújo, L., Afonso, R. and Pachana, N. (2015). Predictors of anxiety in centenarians: health, economic factors, and loneliness. International Psychogeriatrics, 27, 11671176.Google Scholar
Rott, C., Jopp, D., D’Heureuse, V. and Becker, G. (2006). Predictors of well-being in very old age. In Wahl, H.-W., Brenner, H., Mollenkopf, H., Rothenbacher, D. and Rott, C. (eds.), The Many Faces of Health, Competence and Well-Being in Old Age: Integrating Epidemiological, Psychological and Social Perspectives (pp. 119129). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowe, J. W. and Kahn, R. L. (1997). Successful aging. The Gerontologist, 37, 433440. doi 10.1093/geront/37.4.433.Google Scholar
Strawbridge, W., Wallhagen, M. and Cohen, R. (2002). Successful aging and well-being: self-rated compared with Rowe and Kahn. The Gerontologist, 42, 727733. doi: 10.1093/geront/42.6.727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tornstam, L. (2005). Gerotranscendence: A Developmental Theory of Positive Aging. New York, NY: Springer Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Von Faber, et al. (2001). Successful aging in the oldest old: who can be characterized as successfully aged? Archives of Internal Medicine, 161, 26942700. doi:10.1001/archinte.161.22.2694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, Y., Frick, K. and Phelan, E. (2009). Can successful aging and chronic illness coexist in the same individual? A multidimensional concept of successful aging. Journal of the American Medical Directors Association, 10, 8792. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2008.11.003.Google Scholar