Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-27gpq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T10:48:19.842Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Variation in nasal–obstruent clusters and its influence on price and mouth in Scouse1

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 August 2015

AMANDA CARDOSO*
Affiliation:
Linguistics and English Language, University of Edinburgh, Dugald Stewart Building, 3 Charles Street, Edinburgh EH8 9AD, UKA.B.Cardoso@sms.ed.ac.uk

Abstract

This article has two main goals: (i) to show how nasal–obstruent clusters interact with a Canadian-Raising-type pattern in Liverpool English and (ii) to provide evidence that fine phonetic variation in the realisation of nasal–obstruent clusters influences the production of the preceding vowels. I present quantitative evidence from an acoustic study on price and mouth vowel realisations before nasal–obstruent clusters in Liverpool English. The investigation looks at price and mouth separately before obstruents, nasals and nasal–obstruent clusters, in order to demonstrate that nasal–obstruent clusters influence vowels differently depending on the quality of the vowel. Price realisations before nasal–obstruent clusters are similar to productions before singleton obstruents with the same voicing. Specifically, price has a raised realisation before nasal–voiceless obstruent clusters, but a non-raised realisation before nasal–voiced obstruent clusters, which is the same pattern as before singleton obstruents. Mouth realisations preceding nasal–obstruent clusters show evidence of a greater influence from the nasal. The nucleus formant measurements are similar to those before singleton obstruents, but there is frequent monophthongisation preceding nasal–obstruent clusters in mouth, which is mainly found before singleton nasals. Furthermore, I show that the variation in nasal–obstruent clusters in Liverpool English helps to explain the differences in realisation of the target vowels. Nasal deletion is more frequent in nasal–voiceless obstruent clusters following price, leading to vowel productions similar to those before singleton voiceless obstruents. However, nasal durations are longer in nasal–obstruent clusters following mouth, leading to a greater influence of the nasal in the form of more monophthongal vowel productions.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

1

I am grateful to Patrick Honeybone, Warren Maguire, Lauren Hall-Lew and Márton Sóskuthy for providing insightful advice and support. I would also like to thank Ricardo Bermúdez-Otero and an anonymous reviewer for the useful suggestions and comments.

References

Ali, Latif, Daniloff, Ray & Hammarberg, Robert. 1979. Intrusive stops in nasal–fricative clusters: An aerodynamic and acoustic investigation. Phonetics 36, 8597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo. 2014. Philadelphia /ai/-raising without rule insertion. Presented at the Symposium on Historical Phonology, Edinburgh, 13 January.Google Scholar
Boersma, Paul & Weenink, David. 2010. Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (version 5.2.11). www.praat.org (1 October).Google Scholar
Buizza, Emanuela & Plug, Leendert. 2012. Lenition, fortition and the status of plosive affrication: The case of spontaneous RP English /t/. Phonology 29 (1), 138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cardoso, Amanda. 2011. Patterns of the price vowel in Liverpool English: History, phonetics, and corpus phonology. Master's thesis, University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Cardoso, Amanda. 2015. Dialectology, phonology, diachrony: Liverpool English realisations of Price and Mouth. PhD thesis, University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Chambers, Jack K. 1973. Canadian Raising. Canadian Journal of Linguistics 18, 113–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chambers, Jack K. 2006. Canadian Raising: Retrospect and prospect. Canadian Journal of Linguistics 51, 105–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chambers, John, Cleveland, William, Kleiner, Beat & Tukey, Paul. 1983. Graphical methods for data analysis. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Cohn, Abigail C. 1993. Nasalisation in English: Phonology or phonetics. Phonology 10, 4381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Docherty, Gerard J. 1992. The timing of voicing in British English obstruents. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fujimura, Osamu. 1981. Elementary gestures and temporal organization – what does an articulatory constraint mean? In Myers, Terry, Laver, John & Anderson, John (eds.), The cognitive representation of speech, 101–10. Amsterdam: North-Holland.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gregg, Robert. 1957. Notes on the pronunciation of Canadian English as spoken in Vancouver, BC. Journal of the Canadian Linguistic Association 3, 20–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gussenhoven, Carlos. 2007. A vowel height split explained: compensatory listening and speaker control. In Cole, Jennifer & Hualde, José Ignacio (eds.), Laboratory phonology 9, 145–72. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Guy, Gregory R. & Boberg, Charles. 1997. Inherent variability and the obligatory contour principle. Language Variation and Change 9, 149–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, Zellig S. 1960. Structural linguistics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Honeybone, Patrick. 2001. Lenition inhibition in Liverpool English. English Language and Linguistics 5, 213–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, Arthur & Trudgill, Peter. 1996. English accents and dialects, 3rd edn. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Idsardi, William J. 2006. Canadian Raising, opacity and rephonemicization. Canadian Journal of Linguistics 51, 119–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joos, Martin. 1942. A phonological dilemma in Canadian English. Language 18, 141–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendall, Tyler & Thomas, Erik R.. 2009–2014. Vowels: Vowel manipulation, normalization, and plotting in R. R package, version 1.2-1. [ Software Resource: http://ncslaap.lib.ncsu.edu/tools/norm/]Google Scholar
Knowles, Gerald. 1973. Scouse: The urban dialect of Liverpool. PhD thesis, University of Leeds.Google Scholar
Lobanov, Boris M. 1971. Classification of Russian vowels spoken by different speakers. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 49 (2B), 606–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lovins, Julie. 1978. ‘Nasal reduction’ in English syllable codas. Proceedings of the Chicago Linguistic Society 14, 241–53.Google Scholar
Malécot, André. 1960. Vowel nasality as a distinctive feature in American English. Language 36, 222–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moreton, Elliot. 2004. Realization of the English postvocalic [-voice] contrast in F1 and F2. Journal of Phonetics 32 (1), 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moreton, Elliot & Thomas, Erik R.. 2007. Origins of Canadian Raising in voiceless-coda effects: A case study in phonologization. In Cole, Jennifer & Hualde, José Ignacio (eds.), Laboratory phonology 9, 3764. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
O'Cain, Jennifer. 1997. Canadian Raising in a midwestern US city. Language Variation and Change 9, 107–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ohala, John J. 1980. The application of phonological universals in speech pathology. In Lass, Norman J. (ed.), Speech and language: Advances in basic research and practice 3, 7597. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Raphael, Lawrence, Michael, J., Dormann, F., Freeman, Frances & Tobin, Charles. 1975. Vowel and nasal duration as cues to voicing in word-final stop consonants: Spectrographic and perceptual studies. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 18, 389400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenfelder, Ingrid. 2007. Canadian Raising in Victoria, BC: An acoustic analysis. Arbeiten aus Anglistik und Amerikanistik 32 (2), 257–84.Google Scholar
RStudio. 2012. RStudio: Integrated development environment for R (version 0.97.248). (21 December).Google Scholar
Selkirk, Elizabeth O. 1972. The phrase phonology of English and French. PhD thesis, MIT.Google Scholar
Wang, Hongyan &van Heuven, Vincent J.. 2006. Acoustical analysis of English vowels produced by Chinese, Dutch and American speakers. Linguistics in the Netherlands 23 (1), 237–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warner, Natasha. 2002. The phonology of epenthetic stops: Implications for the phonetics–phonology interface in optimality theory. Linguistics 40 (1), 127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, Kevin. 2006. Lenition and segmental interaction: Evidence from Liverpool English (and Spanish). Glossa – An Ambilingual Interdisciplinary Journal 1 (1), 130.Google Scholar
Wells, John C. 1982. Accents of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
West, Helen. 2009. ‘I'm not Geordie! I'm not actually anything!’ Convergent and divergent trends: Dialect levelling and the struggle for identity in a south Durham new-town. Master's thesis, University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Zue, Victor W. & Laferriere, Martha. 1979. Acoustical study of medial /t, d/ in American English. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 66, 1039–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar