Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-qsmjn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T04:16:28.268Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Public Health Communication in Time of Crisis: Readability of On-Line COVID-19 Information

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 May 2020

Corey H. Basch*
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, William Paterson University, Wayne, New Jersey
Jan Mohlman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, William Paterson University, Wayne, New Jersey
Grace C. Hillyer
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology, Mailman School of Public Health, Columbia University, New York, New York
Philip Garcia
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, William Paterson University, Wayne, New Jersey
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to Corey Hannah Basch, Department of Public Health, William Paterson University, University Hall 366, Wayne, NJ07470 (e-mail: baschc@wpunj.edu).
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

The purpose of this study was to assess the readability of information on the Internet posted about coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) to determine how closely these materials are written to the recommended reading levels.

Methods:

Using the search term “coronavirus,” information posted on the first 100 English language websites was identified. Using an online readability calculator, multiple readability tests were conducted to ensure a comprehensive representation would result.

Results:

The mean readability scores ranged between grade levels 6.2 and 17.8 (graduate school level). Four of the 5 measures (GFI, CLI, SMOG, FRE) found that readability exceeded the 10th grade reading level indicating that the text of these websites would be difficult for the average American to read. The mean reading level for nearly all noncommercial and commercial websites was at or above the 10th grade reading level.

Conclusions:

Messages about COVID-19 must be readable at an “easy” level, and must contain clear guidelines for behavior. The degree to which individuals seek information in response to risk messages is positively related to the expectation that the information will resolve uncertainty. However, if the information is too complex to interpret and it fails to lead to disambiguation, this can contribute to feelings of panic.

Type
Brief Report
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © 2020 Society for Disaster Medicine and Public Health, Inc.

Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) is caused by the virus severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus-2 (SARS-CoV-2), and, as of the end of March 2020, is responsible for nearly half a million cases and over 20,000 deaths in 200 countries, areas, or territories. 1 Thus, the World Health Organization declared this a pandemic. 1 As information about this pandemic is slowly unfolding, and while guidelines and restrictions are being formulated and distributed, the general public is left to seek information to inform their health decisions.

Written communication is an essential tool in times of crisis; thus, it is imperative to understand optimal elements of emergency messages. Research to date suggests that written messages are more easily and accurately remembered than auditory messages. Reference Edworthy, Hellier and Newbold2 Regardless of mode of transmission, messages pertaining to risk must contain accurate information, be rapidly disseminated, and be easily understood by the majority of the population. Reference Edworthy, Hellier and Newbold2,Reference Basch, Basch and Redlener3 Health professionals recommend that materials for the general public, particularly in emergencies, be readable at the 6th grade reading level to have maximum impact, Reference McKenzie, Neiger and Thackeray4 yet there are no published studies that analyze the readability of information on COVID-19. To this end, the purpose of this study was to assess the readability of information on the Internet posted about COVID-19 to determine how closely these materials are written to the recommended reading levels.

METHODS

The methods for this study were based on a prior cross-sectional study assessing readability. Reference Basch, Fera and Ethan5 Using the keyword “coronavirus,” a search of the Internet using Google Chrome as a browser was conducted. Websites were vetted to ascertain that they contained relevant content written in English. The sample was comprised of information from the first 100 websites that met the inclusion criteria resulting from the search. Articles were included if they were written in the English language, contained material relevant to COVID-19, and had a distinct URL leading to an article as opposed to a “splash” or menu page. Articles were excluded if they were derived from a sponsored website.

All URL extensions and websites were recorded, and were processed using on-line readability software, Readable.io. 6 We conducted 5 readability tests to ensure a comprehensive representation would result. Those readability tests were as follows: Coleman-Liau Index (CLI), Gunning Fog Index (GFI), the Simple Measure of Gobbledygook (SMOG) Grade Level, Flesch-Kincaid Grade Level (FKGL), and Flesch-Kincaid Reading Ease (FRE). Each test measures the same construct, readability, but does so in slightly different ways. 6 CLI is based on the average sentence length and an average of the number of letters for every 100 words. 6 The GFI assesses the frequency of words that are polysyllabic in conjunction with an average of the length of sentences. 6 The SMOG test also assesses the frequency of words that are polysyllabic but does so in a sample of sentences. 6 The FKGL and FRE tests both involve determining the mean number of syllables per word and the mean sentence length. 6

Readability measures reporting US grade levels were then categorized as “easy,” “average,” and “difficult,” corresponding to less than grade 6, grades 6-10, and greater than grade 10 reading levels, respectively. The mean readability score and standard deviation were calculated for each measure along with the range of minimum and maximum scores. Based on the URL extension, websites were then categorized at commercial (.com and.net) or noncommercial (.org,.gov,.edu). Comparisons between the mean score for each measure were computed using analysis of variance and between grade level categories (easy, average, and difficult) for commercial versus noncommercial websites using chi square test or Fisher’s exact test. P-Values <0.05 were considered statistically significant. All analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS version 26. The Institutional Review Board at William Paterson University does not review studies that do not include human subjects.

RESULTS

For the 100 websites examined, the mean readability scores ranged between grade levels 6.2 and 17.8 (graduate school level). Four of the 5 measures (GFI, CLI, SMOG, FRE) found that readability exceeded the 10th grade reading level, indicating that the text of these websites would be difficult for the average American to read (Table 1). Only 4 websites were scored as “easy, <6th grade” (GFI). The mean reading level for nearly all noncommercial and commercial websites was at or above the 10th grade reading level with the exception of commercial URLs scored with the FKGL (9.9; SD 1.7) (Table 2). Only when using the CLI measure was a difference between website type detected with noncommercial scored at the 12.2 grade level versus commercial at 11.5 (P = 0.04). When evaluating noncommercial and commercial websites by easy, average, and difficult readability categories, the SMOG detected a borderline significant difference with 60.4% of noncommercial sites coded as difficult versus 78.7% of commercial (P = 0.048); no differences were detected with the other 4 measures.

TABLE 1 Readability Characteristics of Coronavirus Websites

a FRE scored on a scale of 0-100, with higher scores indicating greater ease of readability.

TABLE 2 Comparison of Coronavirus Websites by URL Extension (Non-commercial vs. Commercial), n = 100

a FRE scored on a scale of 0-100 with higher scores indicating greater ease of readability.

DISCUSSION

The material on websites analyzed in this study was written at reading levels much higher than recommended. Given that negative emotional states may function as an obstacle to the readability of messages, materials written at higher than recommended levels can further compound the issue. Following the announcement of a crisis, such as the outbreak of a pandemic-type illness, anxiety would be expected to rapidly rise in the general public. Reference Tausczik, Faasse and Pennebaker7 Anxiety is known as the “fight or flight” syndrome and, as such, is meant to gear the mind and body up for action. Reference Barlow8 The body becomes energized to cope with threat, and attention becomes narrowly focused on threat-related information, sometimes to the detriment of accurate interpretation or assimilation of information. Thus, anxiety acts both as a catalyst for information seeking and an obstacle to the ability to read and interpret written messages, Reference Rosen and Knauper9 particularly when prior knowledge of the subject is limited. Reference Minnaert10 Thus, anxiety fuels the quest for disambiguating information in poorly understood emergency situations, and when none is found or when the message itself is difficult to comprehend, anxiety is likely to rise. Reference Ladouceur, Gosselin and Dugas11

This cycle of anxiety-fueled information seeking, difficulty comprehending health related information, or discovery of a lack of disambiguating information, can then fuel panic and lead to maladaptive behaviors, such as unnecessary trips to emergency rooms or overuse of other emergency health resources. Reference Lynch and Galbraith12,Reference Sung, Rush and Earnest13 Thus, the cascade of events in response to anxiety provoking messages is associated with strain on resources. Reference Lynch and Galbraith12,Reference Sung, Rush and Earnest13

Thus, we argue that messages about COVID-19 must be readable at an “easy” level, and must contain clear guidelines for behavior. The degree to which individuals seek information in response to risk messages is positively related to the expectation that the information will resolve uncertainty. Reference Rosen and Knauper9 However, if the information is too complex or too challenging to interpret and it fails to lead to disambiguation, this can contribute to feelings of anxiety and panic.

This study is limited as it represents websites at a single cross-section of time. As the Internet is constantly being updated with new information, especially in an evolving situation, the search would likely yield different results at a later point in time. It is possible that messages are tailored by authors to specific types of audiences, and we did not assess audience type. Reference Park and Avery14 The readability tools are capable of text analysis only, and do not provide any assessment of graphics that may be housed on the pages. Furthermore, the limitation to information in English only does not offer insight beyond 1 language. Nevertheless, this research fills a gap in literature and demonstrates the need for those communicating vitally important information in a time of crisis to be more diligent in writing content that can be easily understood by the largest number of individuals.

Funding

No financial support to report.

Conflicts of interest

None of the authors report a conflict of interest.

References

REFERENCES

World Health Organization (WHO). Coronavirus disease (COVID-19) pandemic. https://www.who.int/emergencies/diseases/novel-coronavirus-2019. Published 2020. Accessed March 27, 2020.Google Scholar
Edworthy, J, Hellier, E, Newbold, L, et al. Passing crisis and emergency risk communications: the effects of communication channel, information type, and repetition. Appl Ergon. 2015;48:252262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basch, CH, Basch, CE, Redlener, I. Coverage of the ebola virus disease epidemic in three widely circulated United States newspapers: implications for preparedness and prevention. Health Promot Perspect. 2014;4(2):247251.Google ScholarPubMed
McKenzie, JF, Neiger, BL, Thackeray, R. Planning, Implementing, and Evaluating Health Promotion Programs: A Primer. 7th ed. New York: Pearson; 2017.Google Scholar
Basch, CH, Fera, J, Ethan, D, et al. Readability of online material related to skin cancer. Public Health. 2018;163:137140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Readable.io. (n.d.). Measure text readability – readability and keyword density analysis and tools – unique readability tools to improve your writing! Readable.io. https://readable.io/. Accessed April 24, 2020Google Scholar
Tausczik, Y, Faasse, K, Pennebaker, JW, et al. Public anxiety and information seeking following the H1N1 outbreak: blogs, newspaper articles, and Wikipedia visits. Health Commun. 2012;27(2):179185.10.1080/10410236.2011.571759CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barlow, DH. Anxiety and Its Disorders. New York: Guilford Press; 2002.Google Scholar
Rosen, NO, Knauper, B. A little uncertainty goes a long way: state and trait differences in uncertainty interact to increase information seeking but also increase worry. Health Commun. 2009;24(3):228238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minnaert, AE. Individual differences in text comprehension as a function of test anxiety and prior knowledge. Psychol Rep. 1999;84(1):167177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ladouceur, R, Gosselin, P, Dugas, MJ. Experimental manipulation of intolerance of uncertainty: a study of a theoretical model of worry. Behav Res Ther. 2000;38(9):933941.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynch, P, Galbraith, KM. Panic in the emergency room. Can J Psychiatry. 2003;48(6):361366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sung, SC, Rush, AJ, Earnest, A, et al. A brief interview to detect panic attacks and panic disorder in emergency department patients with cardiopulmonary complaints. J Psychiatr Pract. 2018;24(1):3244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, S, Avery, EJ. Effects of media channel, crisis type and demographics on audience intent to follow instructing information during crisis. J Contin Crisis Manage. 2018;26(1):6978.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

TABLE 1 Readability Characteristics of Coronavirus Websites

Figure 1

TABLE 2 Comparison of Coronavirus Websites by URL Extension (Non-commercial vs. Commercial), n = 100