Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-gtxcr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-20T02:14:25.737Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Emotion recognition following early psychosocial deprivation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 April 2013

Charles A. Nelson III*
Affiliation:
Boston Children's Hospital Harvard Medical School Harvard Center on the Developing Child
Alissa Westerlund
Affiliation:
Boston Children's Hospital
Jennifer Martin McDermott
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts–Amherst
Charles H. Zeanah
Affiliation:
Tulane University
Nathan A. Fox
Affiliation:
University of Maryland
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Charles A. Nelson III, Laboratories of Cognitive Neuroscience, Boston Children's Hospital/Harvard Medical School, 6th Floor, 1 Autumn Street, Boston, MA 02215; E-mail: charles.nelson@childrens.harvard.edu.

Abstract

An important function of the brain is to scan incoming sensory information for the presence of relevant signals and act on this information. For humans, the most salient signals are often social in nature, such as the identity and the emotional expression of the faces we encounter in our everyday lives. It can be argued that our survival as a species depends in large measure on these skills.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bos, K., Fox, N., Zeanah, C. H., & Nelson, C. A. (2009). The effects of early psychosocial deprivation on the development of memory and executive function. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bos, K. J., Zeanah, C. H., Smyke, A. T., Fox, N. A., & Nelson, C. A. (2010). Stereotypies in children with a history of early institutional care. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 164, 406411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camras, L., Grow, J. G., & Ribordy, S. C. (1983). Recognition of emotional expressions by abused children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 12, 325328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camras, L. A., Ribordy, S. C., Hill, J., Martino, S., Spaccarelli, S., & Stefani, R. (1988). Emotion recognition and production by abused children and mothers. Developmental Psychology, 24, 776781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, N. A., Almas, A. N., Degnan, K. A., Nelson, C. A., & Zeanah, C. H. (2011). The effects of severe psychosocial deprivation and foster care intervention on cognitive development at 8 years of age: Findings from the Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1111/j.14697610.2010.02355.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, S. E., Levitt, P., & Nelson, C. A. (2010). How the timing and quality of early experiences influence the development of brain architecture. Child Development, 81, 2840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gleason, M. M., Fox, N. A., Drury, S., Smyke, A. T., Egger, H. L., Nelson, C. A., et al. (2011). The validity of evidence-derived criteria for reactive attachment disorder: Indiscriminately social/disinhibited and emotionally withdrawn/inhibited types. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 50, 216231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 55, 468484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jasper, H. H. (1958). The ten twenty electrode system of the international federation. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 10, 371375.Google Scholar
Jeon, H., Nelson, C. A., & Moulson, M. (2010). The effects of early experience on emotion recognition: A study of institutionalized children in Romania. Infancy, 15, 209221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, D. E., Guthrie, D., Smyke, A. T., Koga, S. F., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., et al. (2010). Growth and associations between auxology, caregiving environment, and cognition in socially deprived Romanian children randomized to foster vs. ongoing institutional care. Archives of Pediatric Adolescent Medicine, 164, 507516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Le Grand, R., Mondloch, C. J., Maurer, D., & Brent, H. P. (2001). Neuroperception: Early visual experience and face processing. Nature, 410, 890.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leppanen, J. M., & Nelson, C. A. (2009). Tuning the developing brain to social signals of emotions. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 3747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leppanen, J. M., & Nelson, C. A. (in press). Early development of fear processing. Current Directions in Psychological Science.Google Scholar
Marshall, P. J., Fox, N.A., & the BEIP Core Group (2004). A comparison of the electroencephalogram between institutionalized and community children in Romania. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 13271338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, P., Reeb, B. C., Fox, N. A., Nelson, C. A., & Zeanah, C. H. (2008). Effects of early intervention on EEG power and coherence in previously institutionalized children in Romania. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 861880.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDermott, J. M., Westerlund, A., Zeanah, C. H., Nelson, C. A., & Fox., N. A. (in press). Early adversity and neural correlates of inhibitory control: Implications for academic adjustment. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.Google Scholar
Mehta, M. A., Golembo, N. I., Nosarti, C., Colvert, E., Mota, A., Williams, S. C. R., et al. (2009). Amygdala, hippocampal and corpus callosum size following severe early institutional deprivation: The English and Romanian Adoptees Study Pilot. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 943951.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mondloch, C. J., Geldart, S., Maurer, D., & Le Grand, R. (2002). Developmental changes in face processing skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 6784.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moulson, M. C., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., & Nelson, C. A. (2009). Early adverse experiences and the neurobiology of facial emotion processing. Developmental Psychology, 45, 1730.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moulson, M. C., Westerlund, A., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., & Nelson, C. A. (2009). The effects of early experience on face recognition: An event related potential study of institutionalized children in Romania. Child Development, 80, 10391056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, C. A., Bos, K., Gunnar, M. R., & Sonuga-Barke, E. (in press). The neurobiological toll of early human deprivation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.Google Scholar
Nelson, C. A., & De Haan, M. (1996). Neural correlates of infants' visual responsiveness to facial expressions of emotion. Developmental Psychobiology, 29, 577595.3.0.CO;2-R>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, C. A., Parker, S. W., Guthrie, D., & the BEIP Core Group (2006). The discrimination of facial expressions by typically developing infants and toddlers and those experiencing early institutional care. Infant Behavior and Development, 29, 210219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, C. A., Zeanah, C. H., Fox, N. A., Marshall, P. J., Smyke, A., & Guthrie, D. (2007). Cognitive recovery in socially deprived young children: The Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Science, 318, 19371940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, S. W., Nelson, C. A., & the BEIP Core Group (2005). The impact of deprivation on the ability to discriminate facial expressions of emotion: An event-related potential study. Child Development, 76, 5472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollak, S. D., Cicchetti, D., Hornung, K., & Reed, A. (2000). Recognizing emotion in faces: Developmental effects of child abuse and neglect. Developmental Psychology, 36, 679688.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollak, S. D., & Kistler, D. J. (2002). Early experience is associated with the development of categorical representations for facial expression of emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99, 90729076.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollak, S. D., & Sinha, P. (2002). Effects of early experience on children's recognition of facial displays of emotion. Developmental Psychology, 38, 784791.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smyke, A. T., Zeanah, C. H., Fox, N. A., Nelson, C. A., & Guthrie, D. (2010). Placement in foster care enhances quality of attachment among young institutionalized children. Child Development, 81, 212223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, L. A., De Bellis, M. D., Graham, R., & LaBar, K. S. (2007). Development of emotional facial recognition in late childhood and adolescence. Developmental Science, 10, 547558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tottenham, N., Hare, T. A., Quinn, B. T., McCarry, T. W., Nurse, M., Gihooly, T., et al. (2010). Prolonged institutional rearing is associated with atypically large amygdala volume and differences in emotion recognition. Developmental Science, 13, 4661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J. W., Leon, A., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Hare, T. A., et al. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 168, 242249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vanderwert, R. E., Marshall, P. J., Nelson, C. A., Zeanah, C. H., & Fox, N. A. (2010). Timing of intervention affects brain electrical activity in children exposed to severe psychosocial neglect. PloS ONE, 5, 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Windsor, J., Benigno, J. P., Wing, C. A., Carroll, P. J., Koga, S. F., Nelson, C. A., et al. (in press). Language recovery in young children. Child Development.Google Scholar
Windsor, J., Glaze, L. E., Koga, S. F., & the BEIP Core Group (2007). Language acquisition with limited input: Romanian institution and foster care. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 13651381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wismer-Fries, A. B., & Pollak, S. D. (2004). Emotion understanding in postinstitutionalized Eastern European children. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 355369.Google Scholar
Zeanah, C. H., Egger, H. L., Smyke, A. T., Nelson, C. A., Fox, N. A., Marshall, P. J., et al. (2009). Altering early experiences reduces psychiatric disorders among institutionalized Romanian preschool children. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 777785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeanah, C. H., Koga, S., Simion, B., Stanescu, A., Tabacaru, C., Fox, N., et al. (2006). Ethical considerations in international research collaboration: The Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Infant Mental Health Journal, 27, 559576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeanah, C. H., Nelson, C. A., Fox, N. A., Smyke, A. T., Marshall, P., Parker, S. W., et al. (2003). Designing research to study the effects of institutionalization on brain and behavioral development: The Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 885907.CrossRefGoogle Scholar