Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-qxdb6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T16:02:05.968Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Psychological effects of snacks and altered meal frequency

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 March 2007

Robin Kanarek
Affiliation:
Tufts University, Department of Psychology, Medford, MA 01255, USA
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Over the past two decades, substantial research has been conducted to investigate the idea that alterations in short-term nutritional intake play a role in influencing cognitive behaviour and mood. A portion of this research has examined specifically the effect of meal intake on the performance of mental tasks and subjective feelings of mood. Results of this research indicate that a number of variables including the timing and nutritional composition of the meal, nutritional status, habitual patterns of feeding behaviour, beliefs about food, and the nature of the mental tasks, can influence the effects of meals on cognitive behaviour. For example, studies have demonstrated that breakfast intake generally is associated with an improvement in cognitive performance later in the morning, while lunch intake is associated with an impairment in mid-afternoon performance on mental tasks and more negative reports of mood. Intake of nutrients late in the afternoon appears to have a positive effect on subsequent performance on tasks involving sustained attention or memory. Although research has provided insights into the role of meal intake on cognitive behaviour and mood, there are a number of factors which remain to be studied. These include the interaction of age, gender, activity level, meal composition, personality factors, stress with the effects of meals on cognitive behaviour. Additionally, more work is needed on the time-course of short-term nutrient effects, and the effects of chronic changes in meal intake on behaviour.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Nutrition Society 1997

References

REFERENCES

Benton, D. & Owens, D. (1993). Is raised blood glucose associated with the relief of tension? Journal of Psychosomatic Research 37, 723735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, D. & Sargent, J. (1992). Breakfast, blood glucose and memory. Biological Psychology 33, 207210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blake, M. J. F. (1967). Time of day effects on performance in a range of tasks. Psychonomic Science 9, 349350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bull, N. L. (1992). Dietary habits, food consumption, and nutrient intake during adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Health 13, 384388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chandler, A. M., Walker, S. P., Connolly, K. & Grantham-McGregor, S. M. (1995). School breakfast improves verbal fluency in undernourished Jamaican children. Journal of Nutrition 125, 894900.Google ScholarPubMed
Christie, J. J. & McBrearty, E. M. T. (1979). Psychophysiological investigations of post lunch state in male and female subjects. Ergonomics 22, 307323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conners, C. K. (1984) Nutritional therapy in children. In Nutrition and Behaviour, pp. 159192 [Galler, J. R. editor]. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conners, C. K. & Blouin, A. G. (19821983). Nutritional effects on behaviour of children. Journal of Psychiatric Research 17, 193201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, A. (1986). Acute effects of meals on perceptual and cognitive efficiency. Nutrition Reviews 44, Suppl., 163171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, A., Baer, K. & Diekmann, A. (1981). The effects of lunch on sensory-perceptual functioning in man. International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health 49, 105114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craig, A. & Richardson, E. (1989). Effects of experimental and habitual lunch-size on performance, arousal, hunger and mood. International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health 61, 313319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cromer, B. A., Tarnowski, K. J., Stein, A. M., Harton, P. & Thornton, D. J. (1990). The school breakfast program and cogaition in adolescents. Developmental and Behavioural Paediatrics 11, 295300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cross, A. T., Babicz, D. & Cushman, L. F. (1994). Snacking patterns among 1,800 adults and children. Journal of the American Dietetic Association 94, 13981403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickie, N. H. & Bender, A. E. (1982). Breakfast and performance. Human Nutrition: Applied Nutrition 36A, 4656.Google Scholar
Gatenby, S. J. (1997). Eating frequency: methodological and dietary aspects. British Journal of Nutrition 77, Suppl. 1, S7S20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haines, P. S., Guilkey, D. K. & Popkin, B. M. (1996). Trends in breakfast consumption of US adults between 1965 and 1991. Journal of the American Dietetic Association 96, 464470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammer, F. J. (1951). The relation of odour, task and flicker-fusion thresholds to food intake. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 44, 403411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helakorpi, S., Uutela, A., Prattala, R. & Puska, P. (1996). Health Behaviour Among Finnish Adult Population. Helskinki, Finland: National Public Health Institute.Google Scholar
Hildebrandt, G., Rohmert, W. & Rutenfranz, J. (1974). 12 and 24 h rhythms in error frequency on locomotive drivers and the influence of tiredness. International Journal of Chronobiology 2, 175180.Google ScholarPubMed
Hutchinson, R. C. (1952). Meal habits and their effects on performance. Nutrition Abstracts Review 22, 283297.Google ScholarPubMed
Kanarek, R. B. & Marks-Kaufman, R. (1991) Nutrition and Behaviour: New Perspectives. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanarek, R. B. & Orthen-Gambill, N. (1986). Complex interactions affecting nutrition-behaviour research. Nutrition Reviews 44s, 172175.Google Scholar
Kanarek, R. B. & Swinney, D. (1990). Effects of food snacks on cognitive performance in male college students. Appetite 14, 1527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keister, M. (1950). Relation of mid-morning feeding to behaviour of nursery school children. Journal of the American Dietetic Association 26, 2530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, T. H., Foltin, R. W., Rolls, B. J. & Fischman, M. W. (1994). Effect of meal macronutrient and energy content on human performance. Appetite 23, 97111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, H. R., Spring, B. J. & Garfield, G. S. (1986). The behavioural effects of food constituents: strategies used in studies of amino acids, protein, carbohydrate and caffeine. Nutrition Reviews 44s, 6170.Google Scholar
Lininger, F. (1933). Relation of the use of milk to the physical and scholastic progress of undernourished school children. American Journal of Public Health 23, 555558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lloyd, H. M., Green, M. W. & Rogers, P. J. (1994). Mood and cognitive performance effects of isocaloric lunches differing in fat and carbohydrate content. Physiology and Behavior 56, 5157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopez, I., de Andraca, I., Perales, C. G., Heresi, E., Castillo, M. & Colombo, M. (1993). Breakfast omission and cognitive performance of nod, wasted and stunted schoolchildren. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition 47, 533542.Google Scholar
Meyers, A. F., Sampson, A. E., Weitzman, M., Rogers, B. L. & Kayner, H. (1989). School breakfast program and school performance. American Journal of Diseases of Children 143, 12341239.Google ScholarPubMed
Pollitt, E. (1995). Does breakfast make a difference in school? Journal ofthe American Dietetic Association 95, 11341139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollitt, E., Jacoby, E. & Cueto, S. (1996). School breakfast and cognition amongnutritionally at-risk children in the Peruvian Andes. Nutrition Reviews 54, S22–S26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollitt, E., Lewis, N. L., Garza, C. & Shulman, R. J. (19821983). Fasting and cognitive function. Journal of Psychiatric Research 17, 169174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollitt, E., Liebel, R. L. & Greenfield, D. (1981). Brief fasting, stress and cognition in children. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 34, 15251533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Powell, C., Grantham-McGregor, S. & Elston, M. (1983). An evaluation of giving the Jamaican government school meal to a class of children. Human Nutrition: Clinical Nutrition 37C, 381388.Google Scholar
Ruxton, C. H. S., Kirk, T. R. & Belton, N. R. (1996). The contribution of specific dietary patterns to energy intake and nutrient intakes in 7–8 year old Scottish school children. III. Snacking habits. Journal of Human Nutrition and Dietetics 9, 2331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sampson, A. E., Dixit, S., Meyers, A. F. & Hauser, R. (1995). The nutritional impact of breakfast consumption on the diets of inner-city African-American elementary school children. Journal of the National Medical Association 87, 195202.Google ScholarPubMed
Simeon, D. T. & Grantham-McGregor, S. (1989). Effects of missing breakfast on the cognitive functions of school children of differing nutritional status. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 49, 646653.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, A. P., Kendrick, A. M. & Maben, A. L. (1992). Effects of breakfast and caffeine on performance and mood in the morning and after lunch. Neuropsychobiology 26, 198204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, A., Kendrick, A., Maben, A. & Salmon, J. (1994). Effects of breakfast and caffeine on cognitive performance, mood and cardiovascular sunctioning. Appetite 22, 3955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, A., Leekam, S., Ralph, A. & McNeill, G. (1988). The influence of meal composition on post-lunch changes in performance efficiency and mood. Appetite 22, 3955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, A., Maben, A. & Brochan, P. (1994). Effects of evening meals and caffeine on cognitive performance, mood and cardiovascular function. Appetite 10, 195203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, A. & Miles, C. (1986 a). Acute effects of meals, noise and nightwork. British Journal of Psychology 77, 377387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, A. & Miles, C. (1986 b). Effects of lunch on cognitive vigilance tasks. Ergonomics 29, 12511261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, A. P. & Miles, C. (1987). Effects of lunch on selective and sustained attention. Neuropsychobiology 16, 117120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, A., Ralph, A. & McNeill, G. (1991). Influences of meal size on post-lunch changes in performance efficiency, mood and cardiovascular function. appetite 16, 8591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, A. P., Rusted, J. M., Eaton-Williams, P., Savory, M. & Leathwood, P. (1990). Effects of caffeine given before and after lunch on sustained attention. Neuropsychobiology 23, 160163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spring, B. (1986). Effects of foods and nutrients on the behaviour of normal individuals. In Nutrition and the Brain, vol 7, pp. 147 [Wurtman, R. J. and Wurtman, J. I. editors] New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
Spring, B., Lieberman, H. R., Swope, G. & Garfield, G. S. (1986). Effects of carbohydrates on mood and behaviour. Nutrition Reviews 44s, 5160.Google Scholar
Summerbell, C. D., Moody, R. C., Shanks, J., Stock, M. J. & Geissler, C. (1995). Sources of energy from meals versus snacks in 220 people in four age groups. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition 49, 3341.Google ScholarPubMed
Tuttle, W. W., Dawn, K., Larsen, R., Salzano, J. & Roloff, L. (1954). Effect on schoolboys of omitting breakfast. Physiologic responses, attitudes, and scholastic attainments. Journal of the American Dietetic Association 30, 674677.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaisman, N., Vock, H., Akivis, A. & Vakil, E. (1996). Effect of breakfast time on cognitive functions of elementary school students. Archives of Paediatrics and Adolescent Medicine 150, 10891092.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, D. (1990). The impact of increasing blood glucose on psychological functioning. Biological Psychology 30, 1319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, D., Brett, V. & Brain, P. F. (1987). Glucose improves attention and reaction to frustration in children. Biological Psychology 24, 95100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, D. & Owens, D. S. (1993). Blood glucose and human memory. Psychopharmacology 113, 8388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, D., Owens, D. S. & Parker, P. Y. (1994). Blood glucose influences memory and attention in young adults. Neuropsychologia 32, 595607.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, D. & Parker, P. Y. (1997). Breakfast, blood glucoseand cognition. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition (In the Press).Google Scholar
Benton, D. & Sargent, J. (1992). Breakfast food glucose and memory. Biological Psychology 33, 207210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craft, S., Dagogo-Jack, S. E., Wiethop, B. V., Murphy, C., Nevins, R. T., Fleischman, S., Rice, V., Newomer, J. W. & Cryer, P. E. (1993). Effects of hyperglycernia on memory and hormone levels in dementia of the Alzheimer type: a longitudinal study. Behavioral Neuroscience 107, 926940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craft, S., Zallen, G. & Baker, L. D. (1992). Glucose and memory in mild senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 14, 253267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delezal, V. & Tucek, S. (1982). Effects of choline and glucose on atropine-induced alteration of acetylcholine synthesis and content in the brain of rats. Brain Research 240, 280293.Google Scholar
Durkin, T. P., Messier, C., de Boer, P. & Westerink, B. H.C. (1992). Raised glucose level enhance scoplamine- induced acetylcholine overflow from the hippocampus: an in vivo microdialysis study in the rat. Behavioural Bruin Research 49, 181188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, J. L., Gonder-Frederick, L. A., Chewing, W. W., Silveira, J. & Gold, P. E. (1989). Glucose enhancement of performance on memory test in young and aged humans. Neuropsychologia 27, 11291138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keul, J., Huber, G., Lehmann, M., Berg, A. & Jakob, E. F. (1982). Einfluß von Destrose auf Fahrleistung, Konzentrationsfahigkeit, Kreislauf und Stoffwechsel im Kraftfahrzeug-simulator (Doppelblindstudie im cross-over Design (The effect of glucose on driving performance, ability to concentrate, circulation and metabolism in the drinking simulator (double-blind study with cross-over design)). Aktuelle Ernaehrungsmedizin 7, 714.Google Scholar
Kopelman, M. D. (1986). The cholinergic neurotransmitter system in human memory and dementia: a review. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 38A, 535573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuntscherova, J. (1972). Effect of short-term starvation and choline on the acetylcholine content of organs of albino rats. Physiologia Bohemoslovasca 21, 655660.Google ScholarPubMed
Lapp, J. E. (1981). Effects of glycaemic alterations and noun imagery on the learning of paired associates. Journal of Learning Disabilities 14, 3538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manning, C. A., Hall, J. L. & Gold, P. E. (1990). Glucose effects on memory and other neuropsychological tests in elderly humans. Psychological Science 1, 307311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Messier, C., Durkin, T., Mrabet, O. & Destrade, C. (1990). Memory-improving action of glucose: indirect evidence for a facilitation of hippocampal acetylcholine synthesis. Behaviourul Bruin Research 39, 135143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michaud, C., Musse, N., Nicolas, J. P. & Mejan, L. (1991). Effects of breakfast size on short-term memory concentration and blood glucose. Journal of Adolescent Health 12, 5357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moser, L., Plum, H. & Buckmann, M. (1983). Der Einfluß von Destrose auf Diet psychophysische Leistungsfahigkeir des Autofahrers (The effect of glucose in the diet on the pyschological performance ability of car drivers). Aktuelle Ernaehrungsmedizin 8, 247249.Google Scholar
Parker, P. Y. & Benton, D. (1995). Blood glucose levels selectively influence memory for word list dichotically presented to the right ear. Neuropsychologia 33, 843854.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollitt, E., Lewis, N. L., Garza, C. & Shulman, R. J. (19821983). Fasting and cognitive function. Journal of Psychiatric Research 17, 169174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ricny, J., Tucek, S. & Novakova, J. (1992). Acetylcarnitine, camitine and glucose diminish the effect of muscarinic antagonist quinuclidinyl benzilate on striatal acetylcholine content. Bruin Research 576, 215219.Google Scholar
Stone, W. S., Croul, C. E. & Gold, P. E. (1988). Attenuation of scopolamine-induced amnesia in mice. Psychophannacology 96, 417420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, A. P., Kendrick, A. M. & Maben, A. L. (1992). Effects of breakfast and caffeine on performance and mood in the late morning and after lunch. Neuropsychobiology 26, 198204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, A. P., Kendrick, A., Maben, A. & Salmon, J. (1994). Effects of breakfast and caffeine on cognitive performance, mood and cardiovascular functioning. Appetite 22, 3955.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tucek, S. (1983). Acetylcoenzyme A and the synthesis of acetylcholine in neurones: a review of recent progress. General Physiology and Biochemistry 2, 313324.Google ScholarPubMed