Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-tj2md Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T15:49:14.549Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Psycholinguistic, cognitive, and neural implications of bimodal bilingualism*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 April 2015

KAREN EMMOREY*
Affiliation:
School of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences, San Diego State University
MARCEL R. GIEZEN
Affiliation:
San Diego State University
TAMAR H. GOLLAN
Affiliation:
University of California San Diego, Department of Psychiatry
*
Address for correspondence: Karen Emmorey Laboratory for Language and Cognitive Neuroscience 6495 Alvarado Road suite 200 San Diego CA 92120USAkemmorey@mail.sdsu.edu

Abstract

Bimodal bilinguals, fluent in a signed and a spoken language, exhibit a unique form of bilingualism because their two languages access distinct sensory-motor systems for comprehension and production. Differences between unimodal and bimodal bilinguals have implications for how the brain is organized to control, process, and represent two languages. Evidence from code-blending (simultaneous production of a word and a sign) indicates that the production system can access two lexical representations without cost, and the comprehension system must be able to simultaneously integrate lexical information from two languages. Further, evidence of cross-language activation in bimodal bilinguals indicates the necessity of links between languages at the lexical or semantic level. Finally, the bimodal bilingual brain differs from the unimodal bilingual brain with respect to the degree and extent of neural overlap for the two languages, with less overlap for bimodal bilinguals.

Type
Keynote Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research was supported by NIH grant HD047736 to Karen Emmorey and SDSU, Rubicon grant 446-10-022 from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research to Marcel Giezen, and NIH grant DC011492 to Tamar Gollan. We would like to thank Jennifer Petrich for her tremendous help in running many of the studies discussed in this paper, and all our research participants.

References

Abutalebi, J., Annoni, J.-M., Zimine, I., Pegna, A. J., Seghier, M. L., Lee-Jahnke, H., Lazeyras, F., Cappa, S. F., & Khateb, A. (2008). Language control and lexical competition in bilinguals: An event-related fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 14961505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abutalebi, J., Brambati, S. M., Annoni, J. M., Moro, A., Cappa, S. F., & Perani, D. (2007). The neural cost of the auditory perception of language switches: An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study in bilinguals. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 1376213769.Google Scholar
Allen, J. S., Emmorey, K., Bruss, J., & Damasio, H. (2008). Morphology of the insula in relation to hearing status and sign language experience. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 1190011905.Google Scholar
Allen, J. S., Emmorey, K., Bruss, J., & Damasio, H. (2013). Neuroanatomical differences in visual, motor, and language cortices between congenitally deaf signers, hearing signers, and hearing non-signers. Frontiers in Neuroanatomy, 7, 26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Awh, E., Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Schumacher, E. H., Koeppe, R. A., & Katz, S. (1996). Dissociation of storage and rehearsal in verbal working memory: Evidence from positron emission tomography. Psychological Science, 7, 2531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, A. E., & Van den Bogaerde, B. (2008). Code mixing in signs and words in input to and output from children. In Plaza Pust, C. & Morales, E. Lopez (eds.), Sign bilingualism: Language development, interaction and maintenance in language contact situations, pp. 127. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing.Google Scholar
Baum, S., & Titone, D. A. (2014). Moving toward a neuroplasticity view of bilingualism, executive control, and aging. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35, 857894.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernolet, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J. (2007). Shared syntactic representations in bilinguals: Evidence for the role of word-order repetition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 931949.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Green, D. W., & Gollan, T. H. (2009). Bilingual minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 10, 89129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, M. (2010). Happen can't hear: An analysis of code-blends in hearing, native signers of American Sign Language. Sign Language Studies, 11, 205240 Google Scholar
Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2011). Bilingualism influences inhibitory control in auditory comprehension. Cognition, 118, 245257.Google Scholar
Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2013). Parallel language activation and cognitive control during spoken word recognition in bilinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 547567.Google Scholar
Boyes Braem, P., & Sutton-Spence, R. (eds.) (2001). The hands are the head of the mouth. Hamburg, Germany, Signum Press.Google Scholar
Braun, A. R., Guillemin, A., Hosey, L., & Varga, M. (2001). The neural organization of discourse: An H215O-PET study of narrative production in English and American sign language. Brain, 124, 20282044.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M., & Duyck, W. (2010). Is it time to leave behind the Revised Hierarchical Model of bilingual language processing after fifteen years of service? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 359371.Google Scholar
Buchweitz, A., & Prat, C. (2013). The bilingual brain: Flexibility and control in the human cortex. Physics of Life Reviews, 10, 428443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burke, D. M., MacKay, D. G., Worthley, J. S., & Wade, E. (1991). On the tip of the tongue: What causes word finding failures in young and older adults? Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 542579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, S., & Emmorey, K. (2009). Co-speech gesture in bimodal bilinguals. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24, 290312.Google Scholar
Casey, S., Emmorey, K., & Larrabee, H. (2012). The effects of learning American Sign Language on co-speech gesture. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 677686.Google Scholar
Comesaña, M., Perea, M., Piñeiro, A., & Fraga, I. (2009). Vocabulary teaching strategies and conceptual representations of words in L2 in children: Evidence with novice learners. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 2233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corina, D. P., Lawyer, L. A., & Cates, D. (2013). Cross-linguistic differences in the neural representation of human language: Evidence from users of signed languages. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, A., & Caramazza, A. (1999). Is lexical selection in bilingual speech production language-specific? Further evidence from Spanish–English and English–Spanish bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2, 231244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Caramazza, A., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2000). The cognate facilitation effect: Implications for models of lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 12831296.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Miozzo, M., & Caramazza, A. (1999). Lexical selection in bilinguals: Do words in the bilingual's two lexicons compete for selection? Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 491511.Google Scholar
Damasio, H., Grabowski, T. J., Tranel, D., Ponto, L. L., Hichwa, R. D., & Damasio, A. R. (2001). Neural correlates of naming actions and of naming spatial relations. Neuroimage, 13, 10531064.Google Scholar
Dijkstra, T., & Van Heuven, W. J. B. (2002). The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5, 175197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T., Van Jaarsveld, H., & Ten Brinke, S. (1998). Interlingual homograph recognition: Effects of task demands and language intermixing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 5166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donati, C., & Branchini, C. (2013). Challenging linearization: Simultaneous mixing in the production of bimodal bilinguals. In Biberauer, T. & Roberts, I. (eds.), Challenges to linearization, pp. 93128. Berlin, Germany: Mouton De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dufour, R. (1997). Sign language and bilingualism: Modality implications for bilingual language representation. In Kroll, J. F. & de Groot, A. M. B. (eds.), Tutorials in bilingualism: Psycholinguistic perspectives, pp. 301330. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Google Scholar
Duñabeitia, J. A., Hernández, J. A., Antón, E., Macizo, P., Estévez, A., Fuentes, L. J., & Carreiras, M. (2014). The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: Myth or reality? Experimental Psychology, 61, 234251.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K. (2002). Language, cognition and the brain: Insights from sign language research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K., Borinstein, H. B., Thompson, R., & Gollan, T. H. (2008). Bimodal bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 4361.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K., Grabowski, T., McCullough, S., Ponto, L. L., Hichwa, R. D., & Damasio, H. (2005). The neural correlates of spatial language in English and American Sign Language: A PET study with hearing bilinguals. Neuroimage, 24, 832840.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K., Luk, G., Pyers, J. E., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The source of enhanced cognitive control in bilinguals. Psychological Science, 19, 12011206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emmorey, K., & McCullough, S. (2009). The bimodal bilingual brain: Effects of sign language experience. Brain and Language, 109, 124132.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K., McCullough, S., Mehta, S., & Grabowski, T. J. (2014). How sensory-motor systems impact the neural organization for language: Direct contrasts between spoken and signed language. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 484.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K., Petrich, J. A. F., & Gollan, T. H. (2012). Bilingual processing of ASL–English code-blends: The consequences of accessing two lexical representations simultaneously. Journal of Memory and Language, 67, 199210.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K., Petrich, J. A. F., & Gollan, T. H. (2013). Bimodal bilingualism and the frequency-lag hypothesis. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 18, 111.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K., Petrich, J. A. F., & Gollan, T. H. (2014). Evidence from bimodal bilinguals indicates “turning a language ON” is not costly but “turning a language OFF” is. Presented at the 55th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Long Beach.Google Scholar
Geraci, G., Gozzi, M., Papagno, C., & Cecchetto, C. (2008). How grammar can cope with limited short-term memory: Simultaneity and seriality in sign languages. Cognition, 106, 780804.Google Scholar
Giezen, M. R., Blumenfeld, H. K., Shook, A., Marian, V., & Emmorey, K. (2015). Parallel language activation and inhibitory control in bimodal bilinguals. Cognition. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2015.04.009 Google Scholar
Giezen, M. R., & Emmorey, K. (2015). Language co-activation and lexical selection in bimodal bilinguals: Evidence from picture–word interference. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. doi:10.1017/S1366728915000097 Google Scholar
Gollan, T. H., & Acenas, L. A. R. (2004). What is a TOT? Cognate and translation effects on tip-of-the-tongue states in Spanish–English and Tagalog–English bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 246269.Google Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Ferreira, V. S., Cera, C., & Flett, S. (2014). Translation-priming effects on tip-of-the-tongue states. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29, 274288.Google Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C., & Sandoval, T. C. (2008). More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 787814.Google Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Sandoval, T. C., & Salmon, D. P. (2011). Cross-language intrusion errors in aging bilinguals reveal the link between executive control and language selection. Psychological Science, 22, 11551164.Google Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Slattery, T. J., Goldenberg, D., Van Assche, E., & Duyck, W. (2011). Frequency drives lexical access in reading but not in speaking: The frequency-lag hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 186209.Google Scholar
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 6781.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. (1988). Exploring the recognition of guest words in bilingual speech. Language and Cognitive Processes, 3, 233274.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. (2008). Studying bilinguals. Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hall, M. L. (2011). Bilingual picture–word studies constrain theories of lexical selection. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 381.Google Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., Pickering, M. J., & Veltkamp, E. (2004). Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in Spanish–English bilinguals. Psychological Science, 15, 409414.Google Scholar
Hermans, D. (2004). Between-language identity effects in picture–word interference tasks: A challenge for language non-specific or language-specific models of lexical access? International Journal of Bilingualism, 8, 115125.Google Scholar
Hermans, D., Knoors, H., Ormel, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2008). Modeling reading vocabulary learning in deaf children in bilingual education programs. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 13, 155174.Google Scholar
Hermans, D., Ormel, E., Knoors, H., & Verhoeven, L. (2008). The relationship between the reading and signing skills of deaf children in bilingual education programs. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 13, 518530.Google Scholar
Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M. (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 625658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holle, H., Gunter, T. C., Rüschemeyer, S. A., Hennenlotter, A., & Iacoboni, M. (2008). Neural correlates of the processing of co-speech gestures. Neuroimage, 39, 20102024.Google Scholar
Hosemann, J., Altvater-Mackensen, N., Herrman, A., & Mani, N. (2013). Cross-modal language activation. Does processing a sign (L1) also activate its corresponding written translations (L2)? Presented at the 11th Theoretical Issues in Sign Language Research Conference, London.Google Scholar
Hoshino, N., & Kroll, J. F. (2008). Cognate effects in picture naming: Does cross-language activation survive a change of script? Cognition, 106, 501511.Google Scholar
Howard, D., Nickels, L., Coltheart, M., & Cole-Virtue, J. (2006). Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: Experimental and computational studies. Cognition, 100, 464482.Google Scholar
Ju, M., & Luce, P. A. (2004). Falling on sensitive ears: constraints on bilingual lexical activation. Psychological Science, 15, 314318.Google Scholar
Kaufmann, E. (2014). Costs and advantages in bimodal bilingual language production: Language-switching and dual-task paradigms. PhD thesis, Universität zu Köln.Google Scholar
Kovács, Á. M., & Mehler, J. (2009). Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 65566560.Google Scholar
Kovelman, I., Shalinsky, M. H., White, K. S., Schmitt, S. N., Berens, M. S., Paymer, N., & Petitto, L.-A. (2009). Dual language use in sign-speech bimodal bilinguals: fNIRS brain-imaging evidence. Brain and Language, 109, 112123.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 497514.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., Misra, M., & Guo, T. (2008). Language selection in bilingual speech: Evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta Psychologica, 128, 416430.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Bogulski, C. A., & McClain, R. (2012). Psycholinguistic perspectives on second language learning and bilingualism: The course and consequence of cross-language competition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 2, 124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Dussias, P. E. (2013). The comprehension of words and sentences in two languages. In Bhatia, T. & Ritchie, W. (eds.), The handbook of bilingualism and multilingualism (2nd ed.), pp. 216243. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Stewart, E. (1994). Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 149174.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Van Hell, J. G., Tokowicz, N., & Green, D. W. (2010). The Revised Hierarchical Model: A critical review and assessment. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 373381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kubus, O., Villwock, A., Morford, J. P., & Rathmann, C. (2014). Word recognition in deaf readers: Cross-language activation of German Sign Language and German. Applied Psycholinguistics. doi:10.1017/S0142716413000520. Published online by Cambridge University Press on January 27, 2014.Google Scholar
Kushalnagar, P., Hannay, H. J., & Hernandez, A. E. (2010). Bilingualism and attention: A study of balanced and unbalanced bilingual deaf users of American Sign Language. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 15, 263273.Google Scholar
Li, P., Legault, J., & Litcofsky, K. A. (2014). Neuroplasticity as a function of second language learning: Anatomical changes in the human brain. Cortex, 58, 301324.Google Scholar
Lillo-Martin, D., De Quadros, R. M., Chen Pichler, D., & Fieldsteel, Z. (2014). Language choice in bimodal bilingual development. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1163.Google Scholar
Lillo-Martin, D., Koulidobrova, H., De Quadros, R.M., & Chen Pichler, D. (2012). Bilingual language synthesis: Evidence from WH-questions in bimodal bilinguals. In Biller, A.K., Chung, E.Y. & Kimball, A.E. (eds.), Proceedings of the 36th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, pp. 302314. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Linck, J. A., Schwieter, J. W., & Sunderman, G. (2012). Inhibitory control predicts language switching performance in trilingual speech production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 651662.Google Scholar
MacNamara, B. N., & Conway, A. R. A. (2014). Novel evidence in support of the bilingual advantage: Influences of task demands and experience on cognitive control and working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 520525.Google Scholar
MacSweeney, M., Capek, C. M., Campbell, R., & Woll, B. (2008). The signing brain: The neurobiology of sign language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 432440.Google Scholar
MacSweeney, M., Woll, B., Campbell, R., McGuire, P. K., David, A. S., Williams, S. C., Suckling, J., Calvert, G.A., & Brammer, M. J. (2002). Neural systems underlying British Sign Language and audio-visual English processing in native users. Brain, 125, 15831593.Google Scholar
Marian, V., & Spivey, M. J. (2003a). Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within-and between-language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6, 97116.Google Scholar
Marian, V., & Spivey, M. J. (2003b). Bilingual and monolingual processing of competing lexical items. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 173193.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mechelli, A., Crinion, J. T., Noppeney, U., O’Doherty, J., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S., & Price, C. J. (2004). Neurolinguistics: structural plasticity in the bilingual brain. Nature, 431, 757–757.Google Scholar
Meuter, R. F., & Allport, A. (1999). Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 2540.Google Scholar
Meyer, A. S., & Bock, K. (1992). The tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon: Blocking or partial activation? Memory & Cognition, 20, 715726.Google Scholar
Morford, J. P., Kroll, J. F., Piñar, P., & Wilkinson, E. (2014). Bilingual word recognition in deaf and hearing signers: Effects of proficiency and language dominance on cross-language activation. Second Language Research, 30, 251271.Google Scholar
Morford, J. P., Wilkinson, E., Villwock, A., Piñar, P., & Kroll, J. F. (2011). When deaf signers read English: Do written words activate their sign translations? Cognition, 118, 286292.Google Scholar
Navarrete, E., Caccaro, A., Pavani, F., Mahon, B. Z., & Peressotti, F. (2015). With or without semantic mediation: Retrieval of lexical representations in sign production. doi:10.1093/deafed/enu045. Published online by Oxford University Press, 1 January 2015Google Scholar
Neville, H. J., Bavelier, D., Corina, D., Rauschecker, J., Karni, A., Lalwani, A., Braun, A., Clark, V., Jezzard, P., & Turner, R. (1998). Cerebral organization for language in deaf and hearing subjects: Biological constraints and effects of experience. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95, 922929.Google Scholar
Olulade, O. A., Koo, D. S., LaSasso, C. J., & Eden, G. F. (2014). Neuroanatomical profiles of deafness in the context of native language experience. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 56135620.Google Scholar
Ormel, E., Hermans, D., Knoors, H., & Verhoeven, L. (2012). Cross-language effects in written word recognition: The case of bilingual deaf children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 288303.Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232258.Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Sawi, O. (2014). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 962.Google Scholar
Petitto, L. A., Katerelos, M., Levy, B. G., Gauna, K., Tétreault, K., & Ferraro, V. (2001). Bilingual signed and spoken language acquisition from birth: Implications for the mechanisms underlying early bilingual language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 28, 453496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petroj, V., Guerrera, K., & Davidson, K. (2014). ASL dominant code-blending in the whispering of bimodal bilingual children. In Orman, W. & Valleau, M. J. (eds.), Proceedings of the 38th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, pp. 319330. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Piñar, P., Dussias, P. E., & Morford, J. P. (2011). Deaf readers as bilinguals: An examination of deaf readers’ print comprehension in light of current advances in bilingualism and second language processing. Language and Linguistics Compass, 5, 691704.Google Scholar
Pizer, G., Walters, K., & Meier, R. P. (2013). “We communicated that way for a reason”: Language practices and language ideologies among hearing adults whose parents are deaf. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 18, 7592.Google Scholar
Poulisse, N. (1999). Slips of the tongue: Speech errors in first and second language production. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prior, A., & Gollan, T. H. (2011). Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: evidence from Spanish–English and Mandarin–English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 682691.Google Scholar
Pyers, J. E., & Emmorey, K. (2008). The face of bimodal bilingualism: Grammatical markers in American Sign Language are produced when bilinguals speak to English monolinguals. Psychological Science, 19, 531535.Google Scholar
Pyers, J. E., Emmorey, K., & Gollan, T. H. (2014). Unlike gestures, production of ASL signs impairs word retrieval for ASL–English bilinguals. Presented at the 6th Conference of the International Society for Gesture Studies, San Diego.Google Scholar
Pyers, J. E., Gollan, T. H., & Emmorey, K. (2009). Bimodal bilinguals reveal the source of tip-of-the-tongue states. Cognition, 112, 323329.Google Scholar
Rauscher, F. H., Krauss, R. M., & Chen, Y. (1996). Gesture, speech, and lexical access: The role of lexical movements in speech production. Psychological Science, 7, 226231.Google Scholar
Ressel, V., Pallier, C., Ventura-Campos, N., Díaz, B., Roessler, A., Ávila, C., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2012). An effect of bilingualism on the auditory cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 1659716601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Runnqvist, E., Strijkers, K., Alario, F.-X., & Costa, A. (2012). Cumulative semantic interference is blind to language: Implications for models of bilingual speech production. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 850869.Google Scholar
Schlegel, A. A., Rudelson, J. J., & Peter, U. T. (2012). White matter structure changes as adults learn a second language. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 16641670.Google Scholar
Schoonbaert, S., Duyck, W., Brysbaert, M., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2009). Semantic and translation priming from a first language to a second and back: Making sense of the findings. Memory & Cognition, 37, 569586.Google Scholar
Shook, A., & Marian, V. (2009). Language processing in bimodal bilinguals. In Caldwell, E. (ed.), Bilinguals: Cognition, education and language processing, pp. 3564. Hauppage, NY: Nova Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Shook, A., & Marian, V. (2012). Bimodal bilinguals co-activate both languages during spoken comprehension. Cognition, 124, 314324.Google Scholar
Shook, A., & Marian, V. (2013). The Bilingual Language Interaction Network for Comprehension of Speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 304324.Google Scholar
Söderfeldt, B., Ingvar, M., Rönnberg, J., Eriksson, L., Serrander, M., & Stone-Elander, S. (1997). Signed and spoken language perception studied by positron emission tomography. Neurology, 49, 8287.Google Scholar
Strijkers, K., Costa, A., & Thierry, G. (2009). Tracking lexical access in speech production: electrophysiological correlates of word frequency and cognate effects. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 912928.Google Scholar
Valian, V. (2015). Bilingualism and Cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 324.Google Scholar
Van den Bogaerde, B., & Baker, A. E. (2005). Code mixing in mother-child interaction in deaf families. Sign Language and Linguistics, 8, 155178.Google Scholar
Van Hell, J. G., & Dijkstra, T. (2002). Foreign language knowledge can influence native language performance in exclusively native contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 780789.Google Scholar
Van Hell, J. G., & Kroll, J. F. (2013). Using electrophysiological measures to track the mapping of words to concepts in the bilingual brain: A focus on translation. In Altarriba, J. & Isurin, L. (eds.), Memory, language, and bilingualism: Theoretical and applied approaches, pp. 126160. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Van Hell, J. G., Ormel, E., Van der Loop, J., & Hermans, D. (2009). Cross-language interaction in unimodal and bimodal bilinguals. Presented at the 16th Conference of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology, Cracow.Google Scholar
Van Hell, J. G., & Tanner, D. (2012). Second language proficiency and cross-language lexical activation. Language Learning, 62, 148171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verhoef, K., Roelofs, A., & Chwilla, D. J. (2009). Role of inhibition in language switching: Evidence from event-related brain potentials in overt picture naming. Cognition, 110, 8499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vinson, D. P., Thompson, R. L., Skinner, R., Fox, N., & Vigliocco, G. (2010). The hands and mouth do not always slip together in British sign language: Dissociating articulatory channels in the lexicon. Psychological Science, 21, 11581167.Google Scholar
Weber, A., & Cutler, A. (2004). Lexical competition in non-native spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 125.Google Scholar
Weisberg, J., McCullough, S, & Emmorey, K. (under review). Simultaneous perception of a spoken and a signed language: The brain basis of ASL–English codeblends.Google Scholar
Wilson, M., Bettger, J., Niculae, I., & Klima, E. (1997). Modality of language shapes working memory. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2, 150160.Google Scholar
Wu, Y. J., & Thierry, G. (2010). Investigating bilingual processing: The neglected role of language processing contexts. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 178.Google Scholar
Wu, Y. J., Cristino, F., Leek, C., & Thierry, G. (2013). Non-selective lexical access in bilinguals is spontaneous and independent of input monitoring: Evidence from eye tracking. Cognition, 129, 418425.Google Scholar
Yudes, C., Macizo, P., & Bajo, T. (2011). The influence of expertise in simultaneous interpreting on non-verbal executive processes. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 309.Google Scholar
Zeshan, U. (2014). The simultaneous co-production of Indian Sign Language and spoken Hindi: Syntax and semantics. Presented at the 6th Conference of the International Society for Gesture Studies, San Diego.Google Scholar
Zou, L., Abutalebi, J., Zinszer, B., Yan, X., Shu, H., Peng, D., & Ding, G. (2012a). Second language experience modulates functional brain network for the native language production in bimodal bilinguals. NeuroImage, 62, 13671375.Google Scholar
Zou, L., Ding, G., Abutalebi, J., Shu, H., & Peng, D. (2012b). Structural plasticity of the left caudate in bimodal bilinguals. Cortex, 48, 11971206.Google Scholar