Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-x5gtn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-07T10:51:08.307Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Structural explanation of developmental change

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 February 2010

Kurt W. Fischer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Denver, Denver, Col. 80208.

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1978

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bertenthal, B. I., and Fischer, K. W.The development of self-recognition in the infant. Developmental Psychology. 1978, in press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brainerd, C.Piaget's Theory of Intelligence. Englewood Cliffs, N. J., Prentice-Hall, 1978.Google Scholar
Feldman, C. F., and Toulmin, S.Logic and the theory of mind. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. 23:409–76. 1975.Google ScholarPubMed
Fischer, K. W.Structuralism for psychologists. Contemporary Psychology. 17:329–31. 1972.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, K. W.A Theory of Cognitive Development: The Control and Construction of a Hierarchy of Skills. 1977.Google Scholar
Fischer, K. W. (ed.) Sequence and Synchrony in Cognitive Development.Symposium presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association,San Francisco,August, 1977.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H.Stage-related properties of cognitive development. Cognitive Psychology. 2:421–53. 1971.Google Scholar
Jackson, E., Campos, J. J., and Fischer, K. W.The question of décalage between object permanence and person permanence. Developmental Psychology. 1978, in press.Google Scholar
Kofsky, E.A scalogram study of classificatory development. Child Development. 37:191204. 1966.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liben, L. S.Long-term memory for pictures related to seriation, horizontality, and verticality concepts. Developmental Psychology. 11:795806. 1975.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCall, R. Qualitative transitions in behavioral development in the first years of life. In: Bornstein, M. H. and Kessen, W. (eds.), Psychological Development From Infancy. New York, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, in press.Google Scholar
McCall, R., Eichorn, D. H., and Hogarty, P. S.Transitions in early mental development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 42(3, Serial No. 171). 1977.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J.The Origins of Intelligence in Children (Cook, M., trans.). New York, International Universities Press, 1952. (Originally published, 1936.)Google Scholar
Piaget, J.Structuralism (Maschler, C., trans.). New York, Basic Books, 1970. (Originally published, 1968.)Google Scholar
Roberts, R. and Corbitt, P.. Using the theory to predict the development of classification skills. In: (Fischer, K. W., ed.) Sequence and Synchrony in Cognitive Development.Symposium presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association,San Francisco,August, 1977.Google Scholar
Uzgiris, I. C. Organization of sensorimotor intelligence. In: Lewis, M. (ed.), Origins of Intelligence: Infancy and Early Childhood. New York, Plenum Press, 1976.Google Scholar
Watson, M. W. A. Developmental Sequence of Social Role Concepts in Preschool Children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Denver, 1977. Dissertation Abstracts, in press.Google Scholar
Watson, M. W. A. and Fischer, K. W.A developmental sequence of agent use in late infancy. Child Development. 48:828–35. 1977.CrossRefGoogle Scholar