Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T17:21:03.316Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Increased affluence, life history theory, and the decline of shamanism

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 April 2018

Nicolas Baumard*
Affiliation:
Institut d'Etude de la Cognition, Ecole Normale Supérieure, 75005 Paris, France. nbaumard@gmail.comhttps://sites.google.com/site/nicolasbaumard/

Abstract

I applaud Singh's proposition to use evolutionary psychology to explain the recurrence of shamanistic beliefs. Here, I suggest that evolutionary mechanisms (i.e., life history theory) also can explain the variability of the distribution of shamanism. When resources are abundant, individuals become more patient and more open minded to the point that science becomes cognitively attractive and may replace magic.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, R. C. (2001) The great divergence in European wages and prices from the Middle Ages to the First World War. Explorations in Economic History 38(4):411–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumard, N. & Chevallier, C. (2015) The nature and dynamics of world religions: A life-history approach. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 282::20151593.Google ScholarPubMed
Baumard, N., Hyafil, A., Morris, I. & Boyer, P. (2015) Increased affluence explains the emergence of ascetic wisdoms and moralizing religions. Current Biology 25(1):1015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broadberry, S., Campbell, B. M., Klein, A., Overton, M. & Van Leeuwen, B. (2015) British economic growth, 1270–1870. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cashdan, E. & Steele, M. (2013) Pathogen prevalence, group bias, and collectivism in the standard cross-cultural sample. Human Nature 24(1):5975.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohn, A., Engelmann, J., Fehr, E. & Maréchal, M. A. (2015) Evidence for countercyclical risk aversion: An experiment with financial professionals. The American Economic Review 105(2):860–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornelisse, S., Van Ast, V., Haushofer, J., Seinstra, M. & Joels, M. (2013) Time-dependent effect of hydrocortisone administration on intertemporal choice (July 16, 2013). Available at SSRN: https://ssrn.com/abstract=2294189 or http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2294189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D., Sunde, U., Schupp, J. & Wagner, G. G. (2011) Individual risk attitudes: Measurement, determinants, and behavioral consequences. Journal of the European Economic Association 9(3):522–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelfand, M. J., Raver, J. L., Nishii, L., Leslie, L. M., Lun, J., Lim, B. C., Duan, L., Almaliach, A., Ang, S., Arnadottir, J., Aycan, Z., Boehnke, K., Boski, P., Cabecinhas, R., Chan, D., Chhokar, J., D'Amato, A., Ferrer, M., Fischlmayr, I. C., Fischer, R., Fülöp, M., Georgas, J., Kashima, E. S., Kashima, Y., Kim, K., Lempereur, A., Marquez, P., Othman, R., Overlaet, B., Panagiotopoulou, P., Peltzer, K., Perez-Florizno, L. R., Ponomarenko, L., Realo, A., Schei, V., Schmitt, M., Smith, P. B., Soomro, N., Szabo, E., Taveesin, N., Toyama, M., Van de Vliert, E., Vohra, N., Ward, C., Yamaguchi, S. (2011) Differences between tight and loose cultures: A 33-nation study. Science 332(6033):1100–104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guiso, L. & Paiella, M. (2008) Risk aversion, wealth, and background risk. Journal of the European Economic Association 6(6):1109–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guiso, L., Sapienza, P. & Zingales, L. (2013) Time varying risk aversion. Working paper. Available on the website of the National Bureau of Economic Research: Available at: http://www.nber.org/papers/w19284 National Bureau of Economic Research.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haushofer, J. (2013) The psychology of poverty: Evidence from 43 countries. Working paper. Available at: http://www.princeton.edu/joha/publications/Haushofer_2013.pdf.Google Scholar
Houston, A. I. & McNamara, J. M. (1999) Models of adaptive behaviour: An approach based on state. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ifcher, J. & Zarghamee, H. (2011) Happiness and time preference: The effect of positive affect in a random-assignment experiment. The American Economic Review 101(7):3109–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kandasamy, N., Hardy, B., Page, L., Schaffner, M., Graggaber, J., Powlson, A. S., Coates, J. (2014) Cortisol shifts financial risk preferences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 111(9):3608–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kenrick, D. T., Griskevicius, V., Neuberg, S. L. & Schaller, M. (2010) Renovating the pyramid of needs: Contemporary extensions built upon ancient foundations. Perspectives on Psychological Science 5(3):292314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lerner, J. S., Li, Y. & Weber, E. U. (2013) The financial costs of sadness. Psychological Science 24(1):7279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lundberg, J., Bobak, M., Malyutina, S., Kristenson, M. & Pikhart, H. (2007) Adverse health effects of low levels of perceived control in Swedish and Russian community samples. BMC Public Health 7 (1):314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malinowski, B. (1954) Magic, science and religion and other essays, pp. 8587. Doubleday.Google Scholar
McCauley, R. N. (2011) Why religion is natural and science is not. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Morris, I. (2013) The measure of civilization: How social development decides the fate of nations. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Murray, D. R., Trudeau, R. & Schaller, M. (2011) On the origins of cultural differences in conformity: Four tests of the pathogen prevalence hypothesis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37(3):318–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nettle, D. (2009) An evolutionary model of low mood states. Journal of Theoretical Biology 257(1):100103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pepper, G. V. & Nettle, D. (2017) The behavioural constellation of deprivation: Causes and consequences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 40:172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poortinga, W., Dunstan, F. D. & Fone, D. L. (2008) Health locus of control beliefs and socio-economic differences in self-rated health. Preventive Medicine 46(4):374–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, K. (1971b) Religion and the decline of magic: Studies in popular beliefs in sixteenth and seventeenth-century England. Penguin UK.Google Scholar
Yesuf, M., Bluffstone, R. (2008) Wealth and time preference in rural Ethiopia. Available at: https://ideas.repec.org/p/rff/dpaper/dp-08-16-efd.html.Google Scholar