No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Aspiration fuels willpower: Evidence from the addiction literature
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 26 April 2021
Abstract
Ainslie identifies two possible motivational sources for resolve: “thinking categorically” and “intertemporal bargaining.” Ainslie opts for intertemporal bargaining, adding that thinking categorically has no motivational power. The most researched instance of willpower is remission from addiction. This literature shows that aspirations for a more desirable identity and comfortable lifestyle motivate remission. In other words, “thinking categorically” drives willpower.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Creative Commons
- The target article and response article are works of the U.S. Government and are not subject to copyright protection in the United States.
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Biernacki, P. (1986). Pathways from heroin addiction: Recovery without treatment. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.Google Scholar
Hanson, B. (1985). Life with heroin: Voices from the inner city. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.Google Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J., & Prelec, D. (1992). A theory of addiction. In Loewenstein, G. & Elster, J. (Eds.), Choice over time (pp. 331–360). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Heyman, G. M. (2009). Addiction: A disorder of choice. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyman, G. M., & Gibb, S. P. (2006). Delay discounting in college cigarette chippers. Behavioural Pharmacology, 17(8), 669–679. http://dx.doi.org.proxy.bc.edu/10.1097/FBP.0b013e3280116cfe.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorquez, J. S. (1983). The retirement phase of heroin using careers. Journal of Drug Issues, 13, 343–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirby, K. N., Petry, N. M., & Bickel, W. K. (1999). Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(1), 78–87. http://dx.doi.org.proxy.bc.edu/10.1037/0096-3445.128.1.78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Premack, D. (1970). Mechanisms of self-control. In Hunt, W. A. (Ed.), Learning mechanisms in smoking (pp. 107–123). Chicago: Aldine.Google Scholar
Target article
Willpower with and without effort
Related commentaries (26)
Aspiration fuels willpower: Evidence from the addiction literature
Beyond willpower
Evolving resolve
Increasing resolution in the mechanisms of resolve
Is resolve mainly about resisting hyperbolic discounting?
Is “willpower” a scientific concept? Suppressing temptation contra resolution in the face of adversity
It's not a bug, it's boredom: Effortful willpower balances exploitation and exploration
More dynamical and more symbiotic: Cortico-striatal models of resolve, suppression, and routine habit
Pleas for patience from the cumulative future self
Present-state dependency in valuation of the future
Putting the pieces together: Self-control as a complex interaction of psychological processes
Resolve is always effortful
Self-control (or willpower) seeks to bias the resolution of motivational conflicts toward an individual's long-term interests
Self-control from a multiple goal perspective of mixed reward options
Self-organization of power at will
Socializing willpower: Resolve from the outside in
Stress and imagining future selves: resolve in the hot/cool framework
Suppression, resolve, and habit in everyday financial behaviour
The complex nature of willpower and conceptual mapping of its normative significance in research on stress, addiction, and dementia
Weighting on waiting: Willpower and attribute weighting models of decision making
When will's wont wants wanting
Willpower is a form of, but not synonymous with, self-control
Willpower is overrated
Willpower needs tactical skill
Willpower through cultural tools: An example from alcoholics anonymous
Willpower without risk?
Author response
Reply to commentaries to willpower with and without effort