No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
The alluring but misleading analogy between mirror neurons and the motor theory of speech
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 29 April 2014
Abstract
Speech is commonly claimed to relate to mirror neurons because of the alluring surface analogy of mirror neurons to the Motor Theory of speech perception, which posits that perception and production draw upon common motor-articulatory representations. We argue that the analogy fails and highlight examples of systems-level developmental approaches that have been more fruitful in revealing perception–production associations.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014
References
Fogassi, L. & Ferrari, P. F. (2007) Mirror neurons and the evolution of embodied language. Current Directions in Psychological Science
16:136–41.Google Scholar
Galantucci, B., Fowler, C. A. & Turvey, M. T. (2006) The motor theory of speech perception reviewed. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review
13:361–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guenther, F. H. & Vladusich, T. (2012) A neural theory of speech acquisition and production. Journal of Neurolinguistics
25:408–22.Google Scholar
Hickok, G., Houde, J. & Rong, F. (2011) Sensorimotor integration in speech processing: Computational basis and neural organization. Neuron
69:407–22.Google Scholar
Imada, T., Zhang, Y., Cheour, M., Taulu, S., Ahonen, A. & Kuhl, P. K. (2006) Infant speech perception activates Broca's area: A developmental magnetoencephalography study. NeuroReport
17(10):957–62.Google Scholar
Kluender, K. R., Lotto, A. J. & Holt, L. L. (2005) Contributions of nonhuman animal models to understanding human speech perception. In: Listening to speech: An auditory perspective, ed. Greenberg, S. & Ainsworth, W., pp. 203–20. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Liberman, A. M., Cooper, F. S., Shankweiler, D. P. & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967) Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review
74:431–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, A. M. & Mattingly, I. G. (1985) The motor theory of speech perception revised. Cognition
21:1–36.Google Scholar
Lotto, A. J. & Holt, L. L. (2006) Putting phonetic context effects into context: A commentary on fowler (2006)
Perception and Psychophysics
68:178–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lotto, A. J., Hickok, G. S. & Holt, L. L. (2009) Reflections on mirror neurons and speech perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences
13(3):110–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Massaro, D. W. & Chen, T. H. (2008) The motor theory of speech perception revisited. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review
15:453–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, C. J., Crinion, J. T. & MacSweeney, M. (2011) A generative model of speech production in Broca's and Werknicke's areas. Frontiers in Psychology
2:237.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G. & Arbib, M. A. (1998) Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences
21(5):188–94.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G. & Craighero, L. (2004) The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience
27:169–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, S. M., Saygin, A. P., Sereno, M. I. & Iacoboni, M. (2004) Listening to speech activates motor areas involved in speech production. Nature Neuroscience
7:701–702.Google Scholar
Target article
The alluring but misleading analogy between mirror neurons and the motor theory of speech
Related commentaries (1)
Mirror neurons: From origin to function