No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
A ritual by any other name
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 06 April 2018
Abstract
We question the privileged role of trance within the framework presented. The features that Singh suggests make it unique are not well demarcated from those of rituals more generally, and we challenge the depth of explanation presented for the mechanisms of trance. We outline the form of a solution, which may facilitate increased operational utility for the presented framework.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018
References
Banerjee, K., Haque, O. S. & Spelke, E. S. (2013) Melting lizards and crying mailboxes: Children's preferential recall of minimally counterintuitive concepts. Cognitive Science 37(7):1251–89.Google Scholar
Bering, J. M. (2006) The folk psychology of souls. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29(5):453–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bering, J. M. & Bjorklund, D. F. (2004) The natural emergence of reasoning about the afterlife as a developmental regularity. Developmental Psychology 40(2):217–33.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2004) Descartes' baby: How the science of child development explains what makes us human. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2001) Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Cohen, E. (2007) The mind possessed: The cognition of spirit possession in an Afro-Brazilian religious tradition. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Csibra, G. (2008) Goal attribution to inanimate agents by 6.5-month-old infants. Cognition 107(2):705–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, R., Callander, R., Reddish, P. & Bulbulia, J. (2013) How do rituals affect cooperation? An experimental field study comparing nine ritual types. Human Nature 24(2):115–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamlin, J. K., Kiley Hamlin, J. & Baron, A. S. (2014) Agency attribution in infancy: Evidence for a negativity bias. PLoS ONE 9(5):e96112.Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2009) The evolution of costly displays, cooperation, and religion. Evolution and Human Behavior (4):244–60.Google Scholar
Huang, J., Cheng, L. & Zhu, J. (2013) Intuitive conceptions of dead persons' mentality: A cross-cultural replication and more. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 23(1):29–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kapitány, R. & Nielsen, M. (2015) Adopting the ritual stance: The role of opacity and context in ritual and everyday actions. Cognition 145:13–29.Google Scholar
Kapitány, R. & Nielsen, M. (2016) The ritual stance and the precaution system: The role of goal-demotion and opacity in ritual and everyday actions. Religion, Brain, and Behavior 7(1):27–42.Google Scholar
Kelemen, D. (1999a) Function, goals, and intention: Children's teleological reasoning about objects. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3(12):461–68.Google Scholar
Kelemen, D. (1999b) Why are rocks pointy? Children's preference for teleological explanations of the natural world. Developmental Psychology 35(6):1440–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, D., Wellman, H. M., Tardif, T. & Sabbagh, M. A. (2008) Theory of mind development in Chinese children: A meta-analysis of false-belief understanding across cultures and languages. Developmental Psychology 44(2):523–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norton, M. I. & Gino, F. (2014) Rituals alleviate grieving for loved ones, lovers, and lotteries. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 143(1):266–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purzycki, B. G. & Willard, A. K. (2015) MCI theory: A critical discussion. Religion, Brain & Behavior 6(3):207–48.Google Scholar
Rudski, J. (2001) Competition, superstition, and the illusion of control. Current Psychology 20(1):68–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sosis, R., Kress, H. & Boster, J. (2007) Scars for war: Evaluating alternative signaling explanations for cross-cultural variance in ritual costs. Evolution and Human Behavior 28(4):234–47.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S., Bernier, E. P. & Skerry, A. E. (2013) Core social cognition. In: Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us, ed. Banaji, M. R. & Gelman, S. A., pp. 11–16. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D. & Watson, J. (2001) Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development 72(3):655–84.Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H. (2004) Modes of religiosity: A cognitive theory of religious transmission. AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
Xygalatas, D. (2014) The burning saints: Cognition and culture in the fire-walking rituals of the Anastenaria. Routledge.Google Scholar
Target article
The cultural evolution of shamanism
Related commentaries (25)
A ritual by any other name
An existential perspective on the psychological function of shamans
Biological foundations and beneficial effects of trance
Commitment enforcement also explains shamanism's culturally shared features
Complexity and possession: Gender and social structure in the variability of shamanic traits
Do shamans violate notions of humanness?
Enjoying your cultural cheesecake: Why believers are sincere and shamans are not charlatans
Financial alchemists and financial shamans
Genetic predilections and predispositions for the development of shamanism
Identifying the nature of shamanism
Increased affluence, life history theory, and the decline of shamanism
Missing links: The psychology and epidemiology of shamanistic beliefs
Psychosis is episodically required for the enduring integrity of shamanism
Shamanism and efficacious exceptionalism
Shamanism and psychosis: Shared mechanisms?
Shamanism and the psychosis continuum
Shamanism and the social nature of cumulative culture
Shamanism within a general theory of religious action (no cheesecake needed)
Shamans as healers: When magical structure becomes practical function
Some needed psychological clarifications on the experience(s) of shamanism
The cultural evolution of war rituals
The evolution of the shaman's cultural toolkit
The social functions of shamanism
Therapeutic encounters and the elicitation of community care
Toward a neurophysiological foundation for altered states of consciousness
Author response
Why is there shamanism? Developing the cultural evolutionary theory and addressing alternative accounts