Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-42gr6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T10:45:47.004Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Simple morphological spelling rules are not always used: Individual differences in children and adults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 March 2017

NENAGH KEMP*
Affiliation:
University of Tasmania
PAUL MITCHELL
Affiliation:
Nottingham Trent University
PETER BRYANT
Affiliation:
University of Oxford
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Nenagh Kemp, Division of Psychology, School of Medicine, University of Tasmania, Locked Bag 30, Hobart 7001, Tasmania, Australia. E-mail: nenagh.kemp@utas.edu.au

Abstract

The English spelling system has a variety of rules and exceptions, but both theoretical and empirical accounts have generally concluded that by about age 9 or 10, children master the morphological rule that regular plural nouns (e.g., socks) and third-person singular present verbs (e.g., lacks) are spelled with the inflectional ending –s. In three experiments, however, we found that when forced to rely exclusively on morphological cues, only a minority of primary school children, secondary school children, and even adults performed significantly above chance at choosing the appropriate spelling for novel words presented as inflected or uninflected nouns and verbs. Further, significantly above-chance performance was more common in adults who had attended school until age 18, compared to age 16. We conclude that many spellers, especially those who do not go on to tertiary education, never learn some simple morphological spelling rules, and instead rely on a store of individual word-specific spellings.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Beers, C. S., & Beers, J. W. (1992). Children's spelling of English inflectional morphology. In Templeton, S. & Bear, D. R. (Eds.), Development of orthographic knowledge and the foundations of literacy: A memorial Festschrift for Edmund H. Henderson (pp. 231252). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bourassa, D. C., Beaupre, J., & MacGregor, K. (2011). Fourth graders’ sensitivity to morphological context in spelling. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 109114. doi:10.1037/a0021852 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, R. (1973). A first language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryant, P., & Nunes, T. (2008). Morphemes, spelling, and development: Comments on “The timing and mechanisms of children's use of morphological information in spelling” by Pacton, S. and Deacon, H.. Cognitive Development, 23, 360369. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.11.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burt, J. S., Salzgeber, A., & Carroll, M. F. (2013). Spelling recognition after exposure to misspellings: Implications for abstractionist vs. episodic theories of orthographic representations. Acta Psychologica, 142, 383393. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.01.015 Google Scholar
Coleman, C., Gregg, N., McLain, L., & Bellair, L. W. (2009). A comparison of spelling performance across young adults with and without dyslexia. Assessment for Effective Intervention, 34, 94105. doi:10.1177/1534508408318808 Google Scholar
Coltheart, M. (1981). The MRC psycholinguistic database. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33, 497505. doi:10.1080/14640748108400805 Google Scholar
de Villiers, J. G., & de Villiers, P. A. (1973). A cross-sectional study of the acquisition of grammatical morphemes in child speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2, 267278. doi:10.1007/BF01067106 Google Scholar
Ehri, L. C. (1986). Sources of difficulty in learning to read and spell. In Wolraich, M. L. & Routh, D. (Eds.), Advances in developmental and behavioural pediatrics (Vol. 7, pp. 121195). Greenwich, CT: JAI.Google Scholar
Ehri, L. C. (1992). Review and commentary: Stages of spelling development. In Templeton, S. & Bear, D. R. (Eds.), Development of orthographic knowledge and the foundations of literacy: A memorial Festschrift for Edmund H. Henderson (pp. 307332). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ehri, L. C. (1997). Learning to read and learning to spell are one and the same, almost. In Perfetti, C. A., Rieben, L., & Fayol, M. (Eds.), Learning to spell: Research, theory, and practice across languages (pp. 237269). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In Patterson, K., Coltheart, M., & Marshall, J. (Eds.), Surface dyslexia (pp. 301330). London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gentry, J. R. (1982). An analysis of developmental spelling in GYNS AT WRK. Reading Teacher, 36, 192200.Google Scholar
Gentry, J. R., & Gillet, J. W. (1993). Teaching kids to spell. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Green, L., McCutchen, D., Schwiebert, C., Quinlan, T., Eva-Wood, A., & Juelis, J. (2003). Morphological development in children's writing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 752764. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.752 Google Scholar
Greenberg, D., Ehri, L. C., & Perin, D. (2002). Do adult literacy students make the same word-reading and spelling errors as children matched for word-reading age? Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 221244. doi:10.1207/S1532799XSSR0603_2 Google Scholar
Hayes, H., Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2006). Children use vowels to help them spell consonants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94, 2742. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2005.11.001 Google Scholar
Henderson, E. H., & Templeton, S. (1986). A developmental perspective of formal spelling instruction through alphabet, pattern, and meaning. Elementary School Journal, 86, 305316. doi:10.1086/461451 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmes, V. M., & Malone, N. (2004). Adult spelling strategies. Reading and Writing, 17, 537566. doi:10.1023/B:READ.0000044368.17444.7d Google Scholar
Hurry, J., Curno, T., Parker, M., & Pretzlik, U. (2006). An intervention program for classroom teaching about morphemes: Effects on the children's vocabulary. In Nunes, T. & Bryant, P. (Eds.), Improving literacy by teaching morphemes (pp. 134154). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hurry, J., Nunes, T., Bryant, P., Pretzlik, U., Parker, M., Curno, T., & Midgely, L. (2005). Transforming research on morphology into teacher practice. Research Papers in Education, 20, 187206.Google Scholar
Kemp, N., & Bryant, P. (2003). Do beez buzz? Rule-based and frequency-based knowledge in learning to spell the plural -s. Child Development, 74, 6374. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00521 Google Scholar
Kemp, N., Parrila, R., & Kirby, J. (2009). Phonological and orthographic spelling in high-functioning adult dyslexics. Dyslexia, 15, 105128. doi:10.1002/dys.364 Google Scholar
Kessler, B., & Treiman, R. (2003). Is English spelling chaotic? Misconceptions concerning its irregularity. Reading Psychology, 24, 267289. doi:10.1080/02702710390227228 Google Scholar
Leikin, M., & Hagit, E. Z. (2006). Morphological processing in adult dyslexia. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 35, 471490. doi:10.1007/s10936-006-9025-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leong, C. K. (2009). The role of inflectional morphology in Canadian children's word reading and spelling. Elementary School Journal, 4, 343358. doi:10.1086/593937 Google Scholar
Mitchell, P., Kemp, N., & Bryant, P. (2011). Variations among adults in their use of morphemic spelling rules and of word-specific knowledge when spelling. Reading Research Quarterly, 46, 119133. doi:10.1598/RRQ.46.2.2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Bindman, M. (1997a). Morphological spelling strategies: Developmental stages and processes. Developmental Psychology, 33, 637649. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.33.4.637 Google Scholar
Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Bindman, M. (1997b). Learning to spell regular and irregular verbs. Reading and Writing, 9, 427449. doi:10.1023/A:1007951213624 Google Scholar
Pollo, T. C., Kessler, T., & Treiman, R. (2009). Statistical patterns in children's early writing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 410426. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.07.003 Google Scholar
Rastle, K., Harrington, J., & Coltheart, M. (2002). 358,534 nonwords: The ARC nonword database. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 13391362. doi:10.1080/02724980244000099 Google Scholar
Raveh, M., & Schiff, R. (2008). Visual and auditory morphological priming in adults with developmental dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12, 221252. doi:10.1080/10888430801917068 Google Scholar
Read, C. (1986). Children's creative spelling. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Rueckl, J. G., & Aicher, K. A. (2008). Are CORNER and BROTHER morphologically complex? Not in the long term. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 9721001. doi:10.1080/01690960802211027 Google Scholar
Sánchez-Gutiérrez, C., & Rastle, K. (2013). Letter transpositions within and across morphemic boundaries: Is there a cross-language difference? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. Advance online publication. doi:10.3758/s13423-013-0425-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterling, C. M. (1983). Spelling errors in context. British Journal of Psychology, 74, 353364.Google Scholar
Thompson, G. B., Fletcher-Flinn, C. M., & Cottrell, D. S. (1999). Learning correspondences between letters and phonemes without explicit instruction. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 2150.Google Scholar
Treiman, R. (1993). Beginning to spell: A study of first-grade children. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2006). Spelling as statistical learning: Using consonantal context to spell vowels. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 642652. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.98.3.642 Google Scholar
Turnbull, K., Deacon, S. H., & Kay-Raining Bird, E. (2011). Mastering inflectional suffixes: A longitudinal study of beginning writers’ spellings. Journal of Child Language, 38, 533553. doi:10.1017/S030500091000022X Google Scholar
UK Government, Department for Business, Innovation, and Skills. (2014). Participation rates in higher education. Retrieved from https://www.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/458034/HEIPR_PUBLICATION_2013-14.pdf Google Scholar
Varnhagen, C. K., McCallum, M., & Burstow, M. (1997). Is children's spelling naturally stage-like? Reading and Writing, 9, 451481. doi:10.1023/A:1007903330463 Google Scholar
Walker, J., & Hauerwas, L. (2006). Development of phonological, morphological, and orthographic knowledge in young spellers: The case of inflected verbs. Reading and Writing, 19, 819843. doi:10.1007/s11145-006-9006-1 Google Scholar
Wilkinson, G. S. (1993). Wide Range Achievement Test (version 3). Wilmington, DE: Wide Range.Google Scholar