Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

Pseudoword reading errors of poor, dyslexic, and normally achieving readers on multisyllable pseudowords

  • Naomi Frankel Tal (a1) and Linda S. Siegel (a2)

Abstract

This study examined the pseudoword reading strategies of dyslexic readers (i.e., children whose reading was significantly lower than predicted by their IQ score) and poor readers (i.e., children whose reading scores were consistent with their lower IQ scores). The disabled readers were grouped according to their reading grade level and were compared with reading level matched, normally achieving readers. The reading performance on a test of pseudoword reading (Woodcock Word Attack Subtest) for the three groups (dyslexic, poor, and normal readers) was analyzed according to the type of error committed. The performance of dyslexic and poor readers was virtually indistinguishable at both reading grade levels 2–3 and 4–5. There was very little difference among dyslexic, poor, and normally achieving readers in the types of errors made. Nearly 50010 of all the oral reading errors of all three groups were vowel substitutions, followed by consonant substitution and deletion and insertion errors. Sequential, reversal, and word substitution errors were committed infrequently for all three reader groups. The findings failed to support the existence of a critical phonological processing difference between IQ reading- discrepant and IQ reading-nondiscrepant disabled readers and suggest that disabled readers lag behind normally achieving readers in phonological decoding skills.

Copyright

Corresponding author

Linda S. Siegel, Department of Applied Psychology, OISE, 252 Bloor Street, Ontario, Canada M5S 1V6

References

Hide All
Backman, J. E., Mamen, M., & Ferguson, H. (1984). Reading-level design: Conceptual and methodological issues in reading research. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 560568.
Beech, J. R., & Harding, L. M. (1984). Phonemic processing and the poor reader from a developmental lag viewpoint. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 357366.
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1978). Difficulties in auditory organization as a possible cause of reading backwardness. Nature, 271, 9991011.
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1985). Rhyme and reason in reading and spelling. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.
Bruck, M. (1988). The word recognition and spelling of dyslexic children. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 5169.
Bruck, M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexics' phonological awareness deficits. Developmental Psychology, 28, 874886.
Bruck, M., & Treiman, R. (1990). Phonological awareness and spelling in normal children and dyslexics: The case of initial consonant clusters. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 50, 156178.
Bryant, P. E., & Goswami, U. (1986). Strengths and weaknesses of the reading level design: A comment on Backman, Mamen, and Ferguson. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 100103.
Bryson, S., & Werker, J. (1989). Toward understanding the problem in severely disabled readers. Part 1: Vowel errors. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 112.
Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1990). Children's literacy environments and early word recognition skills. Reading and Writing, 5, 193204.
Das, J. P., Mensink, D., & Mishra, R. K. (1990). Cognitive processes separating good and poor readers when IQ is covaried. Learning and Individual Differences, 2, 423436.
Ehri, L. C. (1992). Development of the ability to read words. In Barr, R., Kamil, M. L., Mosenthal, P. B., & Pearson, P. D. (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 383417). New York: Longman.
Ellis, N. C., & Large, B. (1987). The development of reading: As you seek so shall you find. British Journal of Psychology, 78, 128.
Felton, R. H., & Wood, F. B. (1992). A reading level match study of nonword reading skills in poor readers with varying IQ. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 318326.
Fletcher, J. M., Espy, K. A., Francis, D. J., Davidson, K. C., Roarke, B. P., & Shaywitz, S. E. (1989). Comparison of cutoff and regression-based definitions of reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 334338.
Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Rourke, B. P., Shaywitz, B. A., & Shaywitz, S. E. (1992). The validity of discrepancy-based definitions of reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 555561.
Fletcher, J. M., & Morris, R. (1986). In Ceci, S. J. (Ed.), Handbook of cognitive, social, and neuropsychological aspects of learning disabilities (pp. 5580). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Fletcher, J. M., Shaywitz, S. E., Shankweiler, D. P., Katz, L., Liberman, I. Y., Stuebing, K. K., Francis, D. J., Fowler, A. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1994). Cognitive profiles of reading disability: Comparisons of discrepancy and low achievement definitions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 623.
Fowler, C., Shankweiler, D., & Liberman, I. (1979). Apprehending the spelling patterns of vowels: A developmental study. Language and Speech, 22, 243251.
Francis, D. J., Espy, K. A., Rourke, B. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (1991). Validity of intelligence scores in the definition of learning disability: A critical analysis. In Rourke, B. P. (Ed.), Neuropsychological validitation of learning disability subtypes (pp. 1540). New York: Guilford.
Frankel, Tal N., & Siegel, L. S. (1996). Phonological reading errors of dyslexic, poor, and normally achieving readers on monosyllable pseudowords.
Fredman, G., & Stevenson, J. (1988). Reading processes in specific reading retarded and reading backward 13 year-olds. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 97108.
Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In Patterson, K. E., Marshall, J. C., & Coltheart, M. (Eds.), Surface dyslexia (pp. 301330). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Frith, U., & Snowling, M. J. (1983). Reading for meaning and reading for sound in autistic and dyslexic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 97108.
Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. E. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gough, P. B., & Hillinger, M. L. (1980). Learning to read: An unnatural act. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 20, 179196.
Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 610.
Holligan, C., & Johnston, R. S. (1988). The use of phonological information by good and poor readers in memory and reading tasks. Memory and Cognition, 16, 522542.
Jastak, S., & Wilkinson, G. S. (1984). The Wide Range Achievement Test–Revised. Wilmington, DE: Jastak Associates.
Jorm, A., & Share, D. (1983). Phonological reading and reading acquisition: An invited article. Applied Psycholinguistics, 4, 103147.
Jorm, A., Share, D., Maclean, R., & Matthews, R. (1986). Cognitive factors at school entry predictive of specific reading retardation and general reading backwardness: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 4554.
Lovegrove, W. (1992). The visual deficit hypothesis. In Singh, N. & Beale, I. (Eds.), Learning disabilities: Nature, theory, and treatment (pp. 246269). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Lyon, G. R. (1983). Subgroups of learning disabled readers: Clinical and empirical identification. In Myklebust, H. R. (Ed.), Progress in learning disabilities (Vol. 5, pp. 103134). New York: Grune &Stratton.
Manis, F. R., Szeszulski, P. A., Holt, L. K., & Graves, K. (1990). Variation in component word recognition and spelling skills among dyslexic children and normal readers. In Carr, T. H. & Levy, B. A. (Eds.), Reading and its development: Component skills approaches (pp. 207259). San Diego, CA: Academic.
Morrison, F. J. (1984). Reading disability: A problem in rule learning and word decoding. Developmental Review, 4, 3647.
Morrison, F. J. (1987). The nature of reading disability: Toward an integrative framework. In Ceci, S. J. (Ed.), Handbook of cognitive, social, and neuropsychological aspects of learning disabilities (Vol. 2, pp. 3362). Hillsdaie, NJ: Erlbaum.
Olson, R. K., Kliegl, R., Davidson, B., & Foltz, G. (1985). Individual and developmental differences in reading disability. In MacKinnon, G. E., & Waller, T. G. (Eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice (Vol. 4, pp. 164). San Diego, CA: Academic.
Olson, R. K., Wise, B., Conners, F., Rack, J. P., & Fulker, D. (1989). Specific deficits in component reading and language skills: Genetic and environmental influences. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 339348.
Pennington, B. F. (1986). Issues in the diagnosis and phenotype analysis of dyslexia: Implications for family studies. In Smith, S. D. (Ed.), Genetics and learning disabilities (pp. 6996). Bristol, PA: College-Hill Press.
Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.
Perfetti, C. A., & Hogaboam, T. (1975). The relationship between single word decoding and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 461469.
Perfetti, C. A., & Lesgold, A. M. (1979). Coding and comprehension in skilled reading and implications for reading instruction. In Resnick, L. B. & Weaver, P. (Eds.), Theory and practice of early reading (pp. 5784). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rack, J. P. (1985). Orthographic and phonetic coding in developmental dyslexia. British Journal of Psychology, 76, 325340.
Rack, J. P., Snowling, M. J., & Olson, R. K. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 2853.
Rodgers, B. (1983). The identification and prevalence of specific reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Child Psychiatry, 16, 181197.
Rutter, M., & Yule, W. (1975). The concept of specific reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Child Psychiatry, 16, 181197.
Sattler, J. M. (1982). Assessment of children's intelligence and special abilities (2nd ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Seidenberg, M. S., Bruck, M., Fornarolo, G., & Backman, J. (1985). Word recognition processes of poor and disabled readers: Do they necessarily differ? Applied Psycholinguistics, 6, 161180.
Shankweiler, D. P., Crain, S., Brady, S. A., & Macaruso, P. (1992). Identifying the causes of reading disability. In Gough, P. B., Ehri, L. C., & Treiman, R. (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 275305). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Share, D., McGee, R., McKenzie, D., Williams, S., & Silva, P. S. (1987). Further evidence related to the distinction between specific reading retardation and general reading backwardness. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 3544.
Shaywitz, S. E., Escobar, M. D., Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., & Makuch, R. (1992). Distribution and temporal stability of dyslexia in an epidemiological sample of 414 children followed longitudinally. New England Journal of Medicine. 326, 145150.
Siegel, L. S. (1988). Definitional and theoretical issues and research on learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21, 264266.
Siegel, L. S. (1989). IQ is irrelevent to the definition of learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 469478.
Siegel, L. S. (1992). An evaluation of the discrepancy definition of dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 25, 618629.
Siegel, L. S. (1993). Alice in IQ land, or why lQ is still irrelevant to learning disabilities. In Joshi, M. & Leong, C. K. (Eds.), Reading disabilities: Diagnosis and component processes (pp. 7184). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.
Siegel, L. S., & Heaven, R. K. (1986). Categorization of learning disabilities. In Ceci, S. J. (Ed.), Handbook of cognitive, social, and neuropsychological aspects of learning disabilities (Vol. 1, pp. 95121). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Siegel, L. S., & Ryan, E. B. (1988). Development of grammatical-sensitivity, phonological, and short-term memory skills in normally achieving and learning disabled children. Developmental Psychology, 24, 2837.
Silva, P. A., McGee, R., & Williams, S. (1985). Some characteristics of 9 year old boys with general reading backwardness or specific reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 26, 407421.
Snowling, M. J. (1980). The development of grapheme-phoneme correspondences in normal and dyslexic readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 29. 294305.
Snowling, M. J. (1981). Phonemic deficits in developmental dyslexia. Psychological Research, 43, 219234.
Snowling, M. J. (1991). Developmental reading disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 4977.
Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Cognitive processes and the reading problems of learning disabled children: Evaluating the assumption of specificity. In Torgesen, J. K. & Wong, B. (Eds.), Psychological and educational perspectives on learning disabilities (pp. 87131). San Diego, CA: Academic.
Stanovich, K. E. (1988). Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the garden-variety poor reader: The phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21, 590612.
Stanovich, K. E. (1992). Speculations on the causes and consequences of individual differences in early reading acquisition. In Gough, P. B., Ehri, L. C., & Treiman, R. (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 307342). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Stanovich, K., & Cunningham, A. (1993). Where does knowledge come from: Specific associations between printing exposure and information acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 211229.
Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core-variable difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 2453.
Szeszulski, P. A., & Manis, F. R. (1987). A comparison of word recognition processes in dyslexic and normal readers at two reading-age levels. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 44, 364376.
Tallal, P., Sainburg, R. L., & Jerrigan, T. (1991). The neuropathology of developmental dysphasia: Behavioral, morphological, and physiological evidence for a pervasive temporal processing disorder. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 3, 363377.
Treiman, R, & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (1985). Are there qualitative differences between dyslexic and normal readers? Memory and Cognition, 13, 357364.
Tunmer, W. E., & Hoover, W. (1992). Cognitive and linguistic factors in learning to read. In Gough, P. B., Ehri, L. C., & Treiman, R. (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 175214). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Vellutino, F. R. (1979). Dyslexia: Theory and research. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Vellutino, F. R., & Scanlon, D. M. (1989). Some prerequisites for interpreting results from reading level matched designs. Journal of Reading Behavior, 21, 361385.
Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101. 192212.
Werker, J. F., Bryson, S. E., & Wassenberg, K. (1989). Toward understanding the problem in severely disabled readers, Part II: Consonant errors. Applied Psycholinguistics. 10, 1330.
Willows, D. M. (1991). Visual processes in learning disabilities. In Wong, B. (Ed.), Learning about learning disabilities (pp. 163193). San Diego, CA: Academic.
Wolf, M. (1991). The word-retrieval deficit hypothesis and developmental dyslexia. Learning and Individual Differences, 3, 205223.
Woodcock, R. (1973). Woodcock Mastery Reading Tests. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

Related content

Powered by UNSILO

Pseudoword reading errors of poor, dyslexic, and normally achieving readers on multisyllable pseudowords

  • Naomi Frankel Tal (a1) and Linda S. Siegel (a2)

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed.