Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008


Barbara Z. Pearson
Affiliation:
University of Miami
Sylvia C. Fernandez
Affiliation:
University of Miami
Vanessa Lewedeg
Affiliation:
University of Miami
D.Kimbrough Oller
Affiliation:
University of Miami

Abstract

The bilingual child is seen as a unique source of information about the relation between input and intake. The strength of the association between language exposure estimates and vocabulary learning was examined for 25 simultaneous bilingual infants (ages 8 to 30 months) with differing patterns of exposure to the languages being learned. Using the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories, standardized parent report forms in English and Spanish, the percentage of all words that were known in each language was calculated and then plotted against the estimates of language input (also in percentages). A significant correlation was found, r(25)= .82, p < .001. The correlation was also strong when examined point-by-point, even for children whose language environments changed by more than 20%; between observations, although it was not reliable at lower levels of exposure to Spanish. Especially for children with less input in the minority language, the factors which appeared to affect the strength of the association between input and amount learned in a language are discussed.


Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1997

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below.

References

Bayley, N. (1969). Bayley Scales of Infant Development. New York: New York Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., & Capatides, J. B. (1987). Expression of affect and the emergence of language. Child Development, 58, 15131522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1975). Reflections on language. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (1995). Bilingual language acquisition. In Fletcher, P. & MacWhinney, B. (Eds.), Handbook of child language. London: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Döpke, S. (1992). One parent, one language: An interactional approach. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dromi, E. (1987). Early lexical development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Eilers, R., Oller, D. K., Levine, S., Basinger, D., Lynch, M., & Urbano, R. (1993). Canonical babbling in infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 16, 297315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. 1971. An overview of theories of grammatical development. In Slobin, D. I. (Ed.), The ontogenesis of grammar (pp. 189212). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Fenson, L. (1991, June). CDI Basic Statistics: Final. Unpublished manuscript, San Diego State University, San Diego, California.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thai, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., Pethick, S., & Reilly, J. S. (1991). Technical manual for the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories. San Diego, CA: San Diego State University.Google Scholar
Fernández, M. C., & Umbel, V. M. (1991). Inventario del Desarrollo de las Habilidades Comunicativas, Adaptación Cubana [Communicative Development Skills Inventory, Cuban Adaptation]. Miami, FL: University of Miami, Mailman Center for Child Development.Google Scholar
Coodz, N. (1989). Parental language mixing in bilingual families. Infant Mental Health Journal, 10, 2544.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. (1987). The development of categorization in the second year and its relation to other cognitive and linguistic developments. Child Development, 58, 15231531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffith, P. L. (1985). Mode-switching and mode-finding in a hearing child of deaf parents. Sign Language Studies, 48, 195222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Before Headstart: Differences in the family experiences of 1 and 2-year-old children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Held, R. (1965). Plasticity in sensory-motor systems. Scientific American, 213, 8494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1991, October). Social class differences in maternal speech and their lack of consequences for children's early syntactic development.Paper presented to the 16th Annual Boston University Conference on Child Language Development,Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1992). How should frequency in input be measured? First Language, 12, 233244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1993, March). Object labels in early lexicons. Paper presented to the Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans, LA.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27, 236248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyams, N. (1986). Language acquisition and the theory of parameters. Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson-Maldonado, D., & Bates, E. (1988). Inventario del Desarrollo de las Habilidades Comunicativas [Communicative Development Skills Inventory]. San Diego: University of California, Center for Research in Language.Google Scholar
Lanza, E. (1988). Language strategies in the home: Linguistic input and infant bilingualism. In Holmen, A., Hansen, E., Gimbel, J., & Jorgensen, J. N. (Eds.), Bilingualism and the individual (pp. 6984). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Lenneberg, E. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory. (1989). San Diego: University of California, Center for Research in Language.Google Scholar
Newport, E., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. (1977). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (Eds.), Talking to children. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., & Fernandez, S. C. (1994). Patterns of interaction in the lexical growth in two languages of bilingual infants and toddlers. Language Learning, 44, 617653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernändez, S. C., & Oller, D. K. (1993). Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: Comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning, 43, 93120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernändez, S. C., & Oller, D. K. (1995). Cross-language synonyms in the lexicons of bilingual infants: One language or two? Journal of Child Language, 22, 345368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearson, B. Z., Navarro, A. M., & Gathercole, V. M. (1995). Assessment of phonetic differentiation in bilingual learning infants, 18 to 30 months. In MacLaughlin, D. & McEwen, S. (Eds.), Boston University Conference Proceedings (pp. 427–438).Sommerville, MA:Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Penfield, W. 1967. The learning of languages. In Michel, J. (Ed.), Foreign language teaching: An anthology (pp. 192214). London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1991). Rules of language. Science, 253, 530535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. New York: William Morrow.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reznick, J. S., & Goldfield, B. (1992). Rapid change in lexical development in comprehension and production. Developmental Psychology, 28, 406413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sachs, J., & Johnson, M. (1976). Language development in a hearing child of deaf parents. In von Raffler-Engel, W. & Lebrun, Y. (Eds.), Baby talk and infant speech. Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Schiff, N. B. (1979). The influence of deviant maternal input on development of language during the preschool years. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 22, 581603.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schiff, N. B., & Ventry, I. M. (1976). Communication problems in hearing children of deaf parents. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 41, 348358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scovel, T. (1988). A time to speak: A psycholinguistic inquiry into the critical period for human speech. New York: Newbury/Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Stromswold, K. (1994, January). Language comprehension without production: Implications for theories of language acquisition.Paper presented at the Boston University Conference on Language Development,Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Tartter, V. C. (1986). Language processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Western Pennsylvania School for the Deaf. (1992). Literacy initiatives for families of deaf parents with hearing children. (ERIC Document #ED 353 391)Google Scholar

Altmetric attention score


Full text views

Full text views reflects PDF downloads, PDFs sent to Google Drive, Dropbox and Kindle and HTML full text views.

Total number of HTML views: 1
Total number of PDF views: 911 *
View data table for this chart

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between September 2016 - 3rd December 2020. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Hostname: page-component-b4dcdd7-jwbp8 Total loading time: 0.212 Render date: 2020-12-03T17:48:47.636Z Query parameters: { "hasAccess": "0", "openAccess": "0", "isLogged": "0", "lang": "en" } Feature Flags last update: Thu Dec 03 2020 16:59:15 GMT+0000 (Coordinated Universal Time) Feature Flags: { "metrics": true, "metricsAbstractViews": false, "peerReview": true, "crossMark": true, "comments": true, "relatedCommentaries": true, "subject": true, "clr": false, "languageSwitch": true }

Send article to Kindle

To send this article to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about sending to your Kindle. Find out more about sending to your Kindle.

Note you can select to send to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be sent to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants
Available formats
×

Send article to Dropbox

To send this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Dropbox.

The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants
Available formats
×

Send article to Google Drive

To send this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Google Drive.

The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants
Available formats
×
×

Reply to: Submit a response


Your details


Conflicting interests

Do you have any conflicting interests? *