Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-42gr6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T07:01:16.431Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Who Leads? Who Follows? Measuring Issue Attention and Agenda Setting by Legislators and the Mass Public Using Social Media Data

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 July 2019

PABLO BARBERÁ*
Affiliation:
University of Southern California
ANDREU CASAS*
Affiliation:
New York University
JONATHAN NAGLER*
Affiliation:
New York University
PATRICK J. EGAN*
Affiliation:
New York University
RICHARD BONNEAU*
Affiliation:
New York University
JOHN T. JOST*
Affiliation:
New York University
JOSHUA A. TUCKER*
Affiliation:
New York University
*
*Pablo Barberá, Assistant Professor, School of International Relations, University of Southern California, pbarbera@usc.edu.
Andreu Casas, Moore-Sloan Research Fellow, Center for Data Science, New York University, andreucasas@nyu.edu.
Jonathan Nagler, Professor, Wilf Family Department of Politics, New York University, jonathan.nagler@nyu.edu.
**Patrick J. Egan, Associate Professor, Wilf Family Department of Politics, New York University, patrick.egan@nyu.edu.
††Richard Bonneau, Professor, Center For Genomics and Systems Biology, Courant Institute of Mathematical Sciences, Computer Science Department, and Center for Data Science, New York University; and Flatiron Institute, Center for Computational Biology, Simons Foundation, bonneau@nyu.edu.
‡‡John T. Jost, Professor, Department of Psychology, New York University, john.jost@nyu.edu.
***Joshua A. Tucker, Professor, Wilf Family Department of Politics, New York University, joshua.tucker@nyu.edu.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Are legislators responsive to the priorities of the public? Research demonstrates a strong correspondence between the issues about which the public cares and the issues addressed by politicians, but conclusive evidence about who leads whom in setting the political agenda has yet to be uncovered. We answer this question with fine-grained temporal analyses of Twitter messages by legislators and the public during the 113th US Congress. After employing an unsupervised method that classifies tweets sent by legislators and citizens into topics, we use vector autoregression models to explore whose priorities more strongly predict the relationship between citizens and politicians. We find that legislators are more likely to follow, than to lead, discussion of public issues, results that hold even after controlling for the agenda-setting effects of the media. We also find, however, that legislators are more likely to be responsive to their supporters than to the general public.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © American Political Science Association 2019

INTRODUCTION

An enduring topic in the study of democratic polities is how responsive governments are to the preferences of the public. Two main lines of inquiry lead this research: Do politicians respond to the issue priorities of the public (Edwards and Wood Reference Edwards and Wood1999; Jones and Baumgartner Reference Jones and Baumgartner2004; Neundorf and Adams Reference Neundorf and Adams2018; Sulkin Reference Sulkin2005)? And, if so, do they reflect the policy preferences that citizens have on these issues (Caughey and Warshaw Reference Caughey and Warshaw2018; Page and Shapiro Reference Page and Shapiro1983; Soroka and Wlezien Reference Soroka and Wlezien2009; Stimson, Mackuen, and Erikson Reference Stimson, Mackuen and Erikson1995)? Our manuscript focuses on the first of these two questions, because while a correspondence between public and political agendas has definitively been shown to exist, there is still high uncertainty about who leads and who follows in the agenda-setting process. Evidence is even more scant on the important question of which citizens have the strongest ability to set political agendas: the general public (Downs Reference Downs1957), attentive citizens (Aldrich Reference Aldrich1995; Arnold Reference Arnold1990), or politicians’ own supporters (Egan Reference Egan2013; Kastellec et al. Reference Kastellec, Lax, Malecki and Phillips2015).

We aim to shed new light on these classic and relevant political science questions by analyzing the issues to which members of the US Congress and the American public pay attention. Although determining whether politicians also follow constituents’ issue preferences and priorities on the policies they implement—and, if so, which constituents’ issue preferences—is of equal relevance to say the least, “policy actions cannot be taken unless attention is directed at the matter” (Jones and Baumgartner Reference Jones and Baumgartner2004, 2). Hence, disentangling whether politicians devote more time discussing an issue after attention to that issue by the public increases is a first and crucial step toward fully understanding political representation in the United States.Footnote 1

We believe the lack of clear findings on who leads public opinion is partly a function of data limitations, as time and issue units available for previous studies did not allow for sufficiently granular measurement of the relationship between politicians’ and the public’s agendas. Most existing research relies on monthly survey data (typically Gallup’s “Most Important Problem” [MIP] question) to measure the public agenda. However, in our 24-hour media environment, politicians and the public are constantly adjusting the issues to which they devote attention, which means that changes in attention allocation are likely to happen within monthly survey waves. Hence, while survey data allow us to observe whether the public and political agendas covary, they provide limited information on which one moves first. Moreover, existing analyses trace attention to issue categories that are very broad (such as “the economy” or “immigration”), which has the advantage of facilitating comparisons across long periods of time and units, such as states and countries, but can make it difficult to uncover who leads public opinion by grouping together issues that are in fact quite different.Footnote 2

In an effort to address previous data limitations, we pursue a novel empirical strategy by using the microblogging social media platform Twitter to measure the expressed agenda of legislators and the American public. To be clear, our goal is not to assess whether a social media platform such as Twitter is a useful agenda setting tool for politicians or the public but rather to use their “tweets” as a proxy to measure attention being paid to political issues. We are reassured in pursuing such a measurement strategy because virtually all members of the US Congress are active Twitter users and their tweets have been shown to constitute a standardized representation of their expressed issue agenda (Casas and Morar Reference Casas and Morar2015). Moreover, the issues Americans discuss on social media are highly correlated with other measures of issue salience such as the MIP survey question (O’Connor et al. Reference O’Connor, Balasubramanyan, Routledge and Smith2010).Footnote 3

Twitter data provide two main advantages to address the questions at hand. First, the data allow us to measure public and political agendas using the same source: both members of Congress and their constituents are present on the platform, sending tweets that have the same format and symbolic references such as hashtags. Second, the high granularity of the data allows us to observe swiftly changing temporal patterns in topic salience. We are therefore able to pinpoint with precision the extent to which politicians allocate attention to different issues before or after shifts in issue attention by the public (or whether they devote attention to the issue at all). Although previous work has used Twitter data to evaluate the issues to which politicians and constituents pay attention (particularly, the work by Lilly Hemphill and colleagues: e.g., Hemphill and Roback (Reference Hemphill and Roback2014) and Shapiro and Hemphill (Reference Shapiro and Hemphill2017)), this work has examined a limited number of issues and has primarily focused on issue congruence rather than issue responsiveness.

We first analyze all tweets sent by members of the 113th Congress from January 2013 to December 2014. Using a Latent Dirichlet Allocation (LDA) model, we extract 100 topics that represent the diversity of issues legislators discuss on the social networking site. We show that this method is able to classify legislators’ tweets into a set of validated topics that exhibit meaningful variation over time and across parties. We then employ a vector autoregression (VAR) approach to explore the extent to which legislators’ expressed political agendas adapt after a change in issue attention by three different subgroups of the public: partisans, people who are particularly attentive to politics, and a random sample of US Twitter users. Our tests examine the extent to which changes in issue attention by these groups of citizens predict policy makers’ agendas.

Our observational analysis is necessarily of a descriptive rather than a causal nature. Our VAR approach takes advantage of multiple lags of data to distinguish when groups lead conversations about particular topics and by contrast when they are joining debates that are already established, but it cannot rule out confounding by unobservable factors. Nevertheless, our analyses provide revealing information about the configuration of political agendas and public opinion in the United States. Further, it allows us to ascertain in ways not previously possible whether observable data conform to empirical implications of theories specifying how the agendas of different sets of actors impact those of others, providing corroborative evidence for some of these theories.

Our findings show definitively that members of Congress are more likely to follow the issue priorities of the public than to lead them. However, this responsiveness is limited in ways that reinforce polarization and inequality. Lawmakers are more likely to change their behavior after shifts in attention by party supporters, as previous work leads us to expect (Bawn et al. Reference Bawn, Cohen, Karol, Masket, Noel and Zaller2012; Clinton Reference Clinton2006; Egan Reference Egan2013; Kastellec et al. Reference Kastellec, Lax, Malecki and Phillips2015; Shapiro et al. Reference Shapiro, Brady, Brody and Ferejohn1990). To a lesser extent, politicians are also responsive to the issue priorities of attentive citizens over those less inclined to follow politics (Aldrich Reference Aldrich1995; Arnold Reference Arnold1990). But despite well-established models predicting that politicians should reflect the priorities of the general public (Downs Reference Downs1957), we find little evidence for this. Our findings also suggest that mainstream media is in part to blame for this inequality on issue responsiveness: mass media are more likely to cover those issues that are of interest to partisans, and they often lead the political agenda.

POLITICIANS’ RESPONSIVENESS TO THE PUBLIC’S PRIORITIES

Empirical studies on policy (not issue) responsiveness have substantially advanced due to great innovations in data collection and measurement (Burstein Reference Burstein2014; Caughey and Warshaw Reference Caughey and Warshaw2018; Gilens Reference Gilens2012; Lax and Phillips Reference Lax and Phillips2011; Soroka and Wlezien Reference Soroka and Wlezien2009; Tausanovitch and Warshaw Reference Tausanovitch and Warshaw2014), but without a more clear understanding of issue responsiveness, an evaluation of the extent to which governments are responsive to their citizens is incomplete. As Jones and Baumgartner (Reference Jones and Baumgartner2004) note, “How representative is a legislative action that matches the policy preferences of the public on a low priority issue but ignores high priority issues?” (p. 2). For politicians to be truly responsive to the public, they first need to pay attention to the issues constituents deem relevant, and then their actions must reflect people’s preferences on those issues.

Research on agenda setting and political responsiveness in the United States has found a strong relationship between the issue priorities of the public and the agenda of members of Congress (Baumgartner and Jones Reference Baumgartner and Jones1993). For issues such as the economy, health, environment, and foreign trade, changes in public issue salience (measured using Gallup’s long-standing MIP question) correlate at high levels with changes in political attention (measured as the proportion of Congressional hearings on the same issue) (Jones and Baumgartner Reference Jones and Baumgartner2004).

However, existing studies on issue responsiveness do not clearly address a very important question: who leads whom (Page Reference Page1994)? Are policy makers more likely to follow than to lead changes in issue attention by their constituents, or is it the other way around? Research indicates that both scenarios are possible, but it is unclear who (if any) has the largest capacity to lead the issue agenda of the other.

On the one hand, research on policy responsiveness argues that politicians have strong incentives to be responsive to the preferences of the public (Erikson, Mackuen, and Stimson Reference Erikson, Mackuen and Stimson2002; Geer Reference Geer1996; Stimson, Mackuen, and Erikson Reference Stimson, Mackuen and Erikson1995). Building on the “retrospective voting” idea, (Campbell, Dettrey, and Yin Reference Campbell, Dettrey and Yin2010), scholars, such as Stimson, Mackuen, and Erikson (Reference Stimson, Mackuen and Erikson1995), argue (and find) that electorally oriented politicians update their preferences to maximize reelection prospects once they perceive a shift in public opinion: “when politicians perceive public opinion to change, they adapt their behavior to please their constituents” (p. 545). Canes-Wrone and Shotts (Reference Canes-Wrone and Shotts2004) also show that public opinion can influence the preferences of political figures, such as the president, particularly on issues directly related to people’s daily life. Overall, this literature suggests that politicians are responsive to public priorities and leads to the expectation that (H1) the public’s priorities predict the issues to which members of Congress subsequently pay attention.

On the other hand, another body of research argues the opposite. Building on the image of “policy-oriented” politicians, scholars argue that most politicians are mainly motivated by policy goals rather than by the goal of seeking reelection (Jacobs and Shapiro Reference Jacobs and Shapiro2000). Research shows that most citizens are not interested in (Hibbing and Theiss-Morse Reference Hibbing and Theiss-Morse2002) and know very little (Delli Carpini and Keeter Reference Delli Carpini and Keeter1996) about politics, and that instead of evaluating politicians based on their past actions and performance, they make decisions based on group attachments (Campbell et al. Reference Campbell, Converse, Miller and Stokes1960) and elite cues (Lupia, McCubbins, and Arthur Reference Lupia, McCubbins and Arthur1998; Sniderman, Brody, and Tetlock Reference Sniderman, Brody and Tetlock1993). Authors such as Lawrence R. Jacobs, Robert Y. Shapiro, and Benjamin I. Page draw on this literature and their own empirical evidence to show that policy-oriented politicians take advantage of people’s political disconnect to set the agenda to their liking. In interviews with administration officials, the authors are repeatedly told that the government tracks public opinion “not to ‘pander’ but to educate, lead, or otherwise influence public attitudes” (Jacobs and Shapiro Reference Jacobs and Shapiro1997, 3). Overall, this other body of research leads to the expectation that (H2) members of Congress initiate debates about issues that are subsequently followed by the public.

Thus, there are good reasons to believe that politicians follow the issue preferences of the public, but also that the public responds to politicians’ issue priorities. But who has the strongest ability to lead the issue agenda of the other? A primary contribution of our analysis will be to evaluate the magnitude of these effects to determine who has the largest agenda-setting effect (if any). We explore this question without a theoretical preference for either hypothesis, but rather as an open debate that must be addressed to truly evaluate the nature of political responsiveness in the American democratic system (Burstein Reference Burstein2003; Page Reference Page1994).

MODELS OF RESPONSIVENESS

Beyond whether politicians or the public have the largest agenda-setting effect, a second question is also crucial for advancing a more complete picture of issue responsiveness in the United States: to whom should we expect members of Congress to be responsive?

Despite a substantial number of studies on the issue, the answer is not as straightforward as one might think. As Burstein (Reference Burstein2003, 30) points out, “one might hope that 20 years of research would enhance the credibility of some [political responsiveness] theories and reduce that of others. But this does not seem to have happened.” In particular, we observe three main theoretical models to pose three different answers to our question of interest. We call them here the Downsian, the Attentive, and the Supporter models.Footnote 4

In An Economic Theory of Democracy, Downs (Reference Downs1957) argued that, in a bipartisan democratic system, policy makers interested in reelection should be responsive to the median voter or “centrist opinion” (Jacobs and Shapiro Reference Jacobs and Shapiro2000). The implications for political responsiveness are easier to envision from a policy—as opposed to from an issue responsiveness—perspective: members of Congress should adopt the policy preference of their median constituent. Following the same logic, we should also expect legislators to increase their chances of reelection by focusing on issues that a majority of the general public deems relevant. Some strongly disagree and argue that politicians have very little incentives to devote attention to the preferences of the median voters: only a small proportion of the mass public pays attention to, know about, and participate in politics; and those who do are more likely to be partisans than the typical median voter (Hibbing and Theiss-Morse Reference Hibbing and Theiss-Morse2002; Delli Carpini and Keeter Reference Delli Carpini and Keeter1996; de Vreese and Boomgaarden Reference de Vreese and Boomgaarden2006).Footnote 5 Nevertheless, existing empirical evidence still gives some credit to the Downsian logic. An extensive literature on cue-taking argues that even the least informed and attentive public often draws on multiple sources of information to make decisions about politics, also keeping politicians in check (Lau and Redlawsk Reference Lau and Redlawsk2006; Lupia, McCubbins, and Arthur Reference Lupia, McCubbins and Arthur1998). In fact, in their exhaustive work on policy responsiveness in the United States and Canada, Soroka and Wlezien (Reference Soroka and Wlezien2009) find “roughly the same degree [of policy responsiveness] across groups. In most cases, representation is neither markedly better nor markedly worse when we look solely at certain groups” (p. 165).Footnote 6 Hence, given the conflicting arguments and evidence, it is pertinent to test whether a Downsian logic is still in place. A main testable hypothesis that derives from the argument is that (H3) changes in attention allocation by the general public predict changes in issue attention by members of Congress.

Other scholars, however, disagree with this premise as being too optimistic about the public’s agenda-setting role. Instead of responding to the median voter or the general public, some believe members of Congress have incentives to be mostly responsive to attentive voters. Studies of opinion formation show that most voters do not follow day-to-day politics (Hibbing and Theiss-Morse Reference Hibbing and Theiss-Morse2002) and that many do not have clear issue priorities nor policy preferences (Converse Reference Converse2006). Nevertheless, this is not the case for all citizens. Some attentive voters care a great deal about the political world, and according to theoretical models such as Katz and Lazarsfeld (Reference Katz and Lazarsfeld1955)’s “two-step communication flow” and Page and Shapiro (Reference Page and Shapiro1992)’s “rational public,” these attentive voters have the potential to influence the issue priorities and preferences of less attentive citizens. This type of logic leads congressional scholars such as Arnold (Reference Arnold1990) and Aldrich (Reference Aldrich1995) to argue that members of Congress should be particularly concerned about the issues to which attentive voters pay attention. A testable hypothesis that derives from this logic is that (H4) changes in attention allocation by attentive publics predict allocation changes by members of Congress.

Another group of researchers proposes a third alternative: legislators should be mostly interested in responding to core party supporters. They have issue priorities that are easier to distinguish and represent (Wright Reference Wright1989), they play a very active role in nomination processes (Bawn et al. Reference Bawn, Cohen, Karol, Masket, Noel and Zaller2012), their support is crucial to win not only primaries (Fenno Reference Fenno1978; Gerber and Morton Reference Gerber and Morton1998) but also general elections (Holbrook and McClurg Reference Holbrook and McClurg2005), and the priorities of policy-oriented members are more likely to align with theirs (Egan Reference Egan2013; Kastellec et al. Reference Kastellec, Lax, Malecki and Phillips2015). Some empirical research finds that in fact legislators are more likely to represent the policy preferences of their supporters (Clinton Reference Clinton2006; Kastellec et al. Reference Kastellec, Lax, Malecki and Phillips2015; Neundorf and Adams Reference Neundorf and Adams2018; Shapiro et al. Reference Shapiro, Brady, Brody and Ferejohn1990), but no research yet exists showing whether that is the case for issue attention allocation. From this model, however, we can derive that (H5) changes in attention allocation by party supporters predict allocation changes by members of Congress.

As a final theoretical consideration, scholars argue (and find) that the more salient an issue becomes in the eyes of the public, the larger the degree of political responsiveness we should expect (Burstein Reference Burstein2003; Canes-Wrone and Shotts Reference Canes-Wrone and Shotts2004; Jones Reference Jones2004; Soroka and Wlezien Reference Soroka and Wlezien2009; Sulkin Reference Sulkin2005): “the public importance of policy domains may tell us a lot about policy makers’ responsiveness. There is good reason, after all, to expect policy makers to reflect the importance of the different domains because of possible electoral consequences” (Wlezien Reference Wlezien2004, 7). If politicians aim to be responsive to a certain group of the public, they should be interested in reacting to shifts in attention involving issues that are particularly salient to that group. In other words, if members of Congress are mainly responsive to their party supporters (or to the attentive or general public), they should be more likely to react to shifts in attention by party supporters on issues that take on 10% rather than 1% of their supporters’ discussion. Hence, building on this literature, if the previous responsiveness models apply, we should expect that (H6) to the extent that particular issues are more salient among the general public (according to the Downsian model), attentive citizens (according to the Attentive model), and party supporters (according to the Supporter model), these publics’ priorities will more strongly predict the politicians’ agenda.Footnote 7

THE MECHANISMS OF ISSUE RESPONSIVENESS

Here, we use messages sent on Twitter by politicians and the public as measures of these individuals’ issue priorities, allowing us to evaluate the extent to which reciprocal relationships exist among these priorities. Previous research has suggested several mechanisms by which politicians might become informed about the public’s priorities, and vice versa. In particular, this literature puts forward a set of mechanisms through which the issue priorities of politicians and the public can directly lead the priorities of the other, as well as ways in which the mass media can channel these reciprocal dynamics.

One mechanism by which politicians and citizens learn about one another’s priorities is direct interaction and communication between lawmakers and their constituents, which include town halls, “lobby days” organized by interest groups and offline and online correspondence. For example, about 24% of Americans report having written a letter to a public representative (Schlozman, Verba, and Brady Reference Schlozman, Verba and Brady2012). Surveys indicate that politicians pay some attention to social media messages from constituents as well, although these messages are not weighed as heavily as other constituent communications (Chen, Lee, and Marble Reference Chen, Lee and Marble2018). Another way that politicians learn about citizens’ priorities is by tracking public opinion. Polling organizations, such as Gallup and the Pew Research Center, regularly release polls revealing the public’s issue priorities. Political parties, campaign staff, and government agencies run their own polls to assess the issues that are of interest to the public (Jacobs and Shapiro Reference Jacobs and Shapiro2000). Modern practices also include using dashboards to track the issues that are mostly discussed by citizens in social media (Webster and Ksiazek Reference Webster and Ksiazek2012).

In this study, we focus on what is arguably the primary mechanism by which politicians and the public learn about each other’s issue priorities: the news media. First, the media’s powerful role in this regard derives from the fact that lawmakers are incentivized to expend effort to generate media coverage of their priorities, while at the same time media outlets are incentivized to cover issues that resonate with their audiences’ priorities. A wide range of factors determine media content, including assigned “beats,” journalistic practices, and the occurrence of newsworthy events (Graber Reference Graber1997; Shoemaker and Reese Reference Shoemaker and Reese1996). Among these factors, audience preferences and political institutions play a very important role, ensuring that media covers issues that are of interest to both the public and politicians. And second, the media’s substantive role derives from its ability to drive both public and political attention (McCombs and Shaw Reference McCombs and Shaw1972; Zaller Reference Zaller1992).

The media is responsive to political elites in part because political institutions represent an important source of constant newsworthy information (Shoemaker and Reese Reference Shoemaker and Reese1996). Media outlets regularly appoint correspondents to institutions such as the White House and Congress, ensuring that major issues discussed in these political venues achieve media attention. Media outlets are responsive to public demands in part because of market pressures: particularly in a context in which most outlets face economic hardships, discussing the issues that are of interest to the public increases their chances of getting the readers and viewers needed to generate profits. More than ever, media outlets today have a wide range of instruments in their hands to measure, and respond to, the issues in which the public is interested (including tracking social media attention) (Anand and Peterson Reference Anand and Peterson2000; Webster and Ksiazek Reference Webster and Ksiazek2012). In addition, public sentiment is often reflected by newsworthy political events in themselves, including election results, strikes, and demonstrations (Gitlin Reference Gitlin1980). To be sure, neither the public nor politicians have exclusive control over the agenda, which is frequently set by external unexpected shocks (such as natural disasters) (Birkland Reference Birkland1998) and recurrent events (and expiring statutory provisions) (Adler and Wilkerson Reference Adler and Wilkerson2012) that simultaneously affect the attention distribution of politicians but also of the media and the public.

There is clear evidence showing that not only politicians and the public lead media attention but also that the media can drive political agendas and public opinion (Soroka Reference Soroka2002; Walgrave, Stuart, and Nuytemans Reference Walgrave, Soroka and Nuytemans2008; Walgrave and Van Aelst Reference Walgrave and Van Aelst2006). The media “construct” and highlight problems for politicians to solve, and they increase the salience of issues that voters might consider relevant, which reelection-seeking politicians should address in other to please them (Wouters and Walgrave Reference Wouters and Walgrave2017). Moreover, a long-standing literature also shows that the issues covered in the media are very likely to lead public opinion and preferences (Boydstun Reference Boydstun2013; Iyengar and Kinder Reference Iyengar and Kinder2010; McCombs and Shaw Reference McCombs and Shaw1972; Zaller Reference Zaller1992).

Exploring all the mechanisms through which groups of the public and politicians influence the agenda of the other is outside the scope of this study. Nevertheless, given that research points to mass media as playing a crucial agenda setting role, in our analysis we will control for potential media effects as well as explore the extent to which mass media coverage favors particular responsiveness models.

DATA

Members of Congress on Twitter

To test our hypotheses, we use tweets sent by members of the 113th House and Senate of the US Congress (2013–14). Twitter use in Congress has increased steadily over the past years (Chi and Yang Reference Chi and Yang2011; Evans, Cordova, and Sipole Reference Evans, Cordova and Sipole2014; Golbeck, Grimes, and Rogers Reference Golbeck, Grimes and Rogers2010; Shapiro, Hemphill, and Otterbacher Reference Shapiro, Hemphill, Otterbacher, Rogowski and Sinclair2012). Ninety-five percent of legislators that served in the 113th Congress had active Twitter accounts, sending a total of 651,116 messages (excluding retweets), about 900 tweets per day.Footnote 8

Golbeck, Grimes, and Rogers (Reference Golbeck, Grimes and Rogers2010) argue that members of Congress use Twitter primarily to advertise their policy positions and to provide information about their activities. However, more recent studies have shown that the platform can also be a tool for members of Congress to be responsive to their constituents, exercise control of the political agenda, express party loyalty, engage in partisan taunting, and report on their constituency service (Hemphill, Otterbacher, and Shapiro Reference Hemphill, Otterbacher and Shapiro2013; Evans, Cordova, and Sipole Reference Evans, Cordova and Sipole2014; Russell Reference Russell2018b, Reference Russell2018a). Moreover, research indicates that the topics discussed in tweets are a fair representation of the legislators’ overall expressed agenda: there is a very high correlation between the issues they discuss on social media and their press releases, for example (Casas and Morar Reference Casas and Morar2015).

Citizens on Twitter

In addition to tweets sent by members of Congress, we also collected tweets sent by different samples of Twitter users. These allow us to test our hypotheses (H3,4,5,6) regarding the part of the public whose shifts in attention politicians are more likely to follow. We consider four samples of Twitter users:

  1. 1 General Public: includes about 25,000 Twitter users, sampled by generating random numeric user IDs, then checking whether the users existed, and then checking whether the users resided in the United States.Footnote 9

  2. 2 Attentive Public: a randomly generated sample of Twitter users that follow at least one of five major media outlets in the United States (CNN, Wall Street Journal, New York Times, Fox News, and MSNBC). We apply a geographic restriction based on the time zone on users’ profiles, which is available for most users. In particular, we exclude users whose time zone indicates they are likely to be located outside the United States. We also filter based on activity: only users who have ever sent 100 tweets or more are included. After applying these filters, the final sample size is 10,000 users.

  3. 3 Republican Supporters: a random sample of 10,000 Twitter users who follow three or more Republican members of Congress and no Democrat in Congress. The same geographic and activity filters as in the attentive public sample are applied here. In Online Appendix F, we demonstrate why this sampling method is able to select party supporters.

  4. 4 Democratic Supporters: a random sample of 10,000 Twitter users who follow three or more Democratic members of Congress and no Republican in Congress. The same geographic and activity filters as in the sample of Republican supporters apply.

After identifying these four samples, we then collected all the tweets they sent during our period of analysis (January 2013 to December 2014) using Twitter’s REST API.Footnote 10 The final number of users and tweets in each group is available in Table 1. Retweets are excluded from our sample to avoid inflating the correlations we observe between politicians and the public regarding the issues they discuss.

TABLE 1. Description of the Tweets in the Dataset

Note: Period of analysis: January 1, 2013, to December 31, 2014. N corresponds to the number of Twitter accounts in each sample. Avg, Min, and Max correspond to the average, minimum, and maximum number of tweets, respectively, sent by individual users in each group during the whole period of analysis. Tweets corresponds to the total number of tweets sent by all users in each group during the period of analysis.

Media

As discussed previously, it may well be that both public and political issue agendas are led by the mass media (Gerber, Dean, and Bergan Reference Gerber, Karlan and Bergan2009; Habel Reference Habel2012; King, Schneer, and White Reference King, Schneer and White2017; Ladd and Lenz Reference Ladd and Lenz2009), particularly so on social media (Feezell Reference Feezell2018). To account for this possibility, we also collected tweets from a sample of media outlets and use them to control for media effects. In particular, we collected all tweets sent over the same time period from the Twitter accounts of the 36 largest media outlets in the United States (print, broadcast, online), as identified by the Pew Research Center.

MEASURING ATTENTION TO POLITICAL ISSUES WITH TOPIC MODELS

Our purpose in this paper is to characterize the different issues that members of Congress, ordinary citizens, and media outlets discussed on Twitter, and how their importance varies over time and across groups defined by their partisanship and political interest. To extract these categories, we estimate a probabilistic model of word occurrences in documents called an LDA (Blei, Ng, and Jordan Reference Blei, Ng and Jordan2003), which belongs to a general category of latent variable models that infer topics from documents using a “bag-of-words” approach.

As we explain in greater detail in Online Appendix G.1, this method treats each document as a random mixture over latent topics, and each topic as a probability distribution over tokens. In our analysis, tokens are n-grams (combinations of one and two words). Our definition of “document” is the aggregated total of tweets sent by members of Congress each day, by party and chamber.Footnote 11

The alternative to using an unsupervised topic model would be for the analyst to choose the topics and then build a supervised classifier predicting them. Despite the existence of well-known categories of political issues,Footnote 12 training an accurate classifier would be an incredibly arduous task, given the large number of categories, making unsupervised models a preferable option. However, as we show in Online Appendix E.2, it is possible to map the topics derived from the data to an existing classification of political topics—the topics used in Jones and Baumgartner (Reference Jones and Baumgartner2004)—with similar results.Footnote 13 And as we demonstrate in Online Appendix G.3, the topics generated by the model pass standard tests of both predictive and semantic validity (Quinn et al. Reference Quinn, Monroe, Colaresi, Crespin and Radev2010).

Note two additional features of our analysis. First, we fit the model at first only for members of Congress (instead of fitting it to the messages sent by all groups) to increase the likelihood of discovering topics that were politically salient during the 113th Congress, and then use the estimated parameters to compute the posterior topic distributions for citizens and media outlets, also aggregated by day, based on their observed words. However, a replication of the results based on an LDA model fit to the tweets of politicians, the media, and the public leads to similar conclusions (see Online Appendix E.4). Second, in our estimation we assume that topic distributions are independent over time, and that the number of topics and the content of each topic is constant over time.

We fix the number of topics to K = 100 after exploring a wide range of values by running 10-fold cross-validations and computing common goodness-of-fit measures (Chang et al. Reference Chang, Gerrish, Wang, Boyd-graber, Blei, Bengio, Schuurmans, Lafferty, Williams and Culotta2009) (see Online Appendix G.2 for a detailed description of how we chose K).

In general, we find that most of the 100 resulting topics can be easily labeled. However, not all of them are political in nature: for example, we find topics about anniversaries and celebrations (Valentine’s Day, Flag Day, Constitution Day, Thanksgiving, etc.). Because we are not interested in these topics, in our analysis we will only include political issues, of which 53 were identified.Footnote 14 After reviewing their content, we noted that some topics that referred to a single issue were classified as different topics because distinct words were being used by different groups when talking about the same issue. For example, we found separate topics for Republican and Democratic members of Congress discussing the 2013 Government Shutdown. This may influence our results by overestimating how often parties in Congress respond to their supporters. To avoid this potential source of bias, we decided to merge some topics and focus our analysis on 46 political issues. Table 2 displays the list of all these topics we have classified as political issues.

TABLE 2. List of Political Issues

Note: The topic number in parentheses indicate issues that have been created ad hoc by merging very similar topics from the topic model.

RESULTS

Issue Attention Congruence

The key substantive question we want to answer is whether the distribution of topics discussed by members of Congress leads or follows that of their constituents, and vice versa. Are members following their constituents? And if so, are they following particular types of constituents?

Similar to previous studies on the issue, we start by examining simple congruence in the way members of Congress and citizens allocate attention to the 46 political issues we identified. In this issue congruence framework, a correlation between the public and the political agenda is a necessary condition for political responsiveness to be present. Table 3 displays Pearson correlation coefficients, indicating how similar the issue distribution of Democratic and Republican supporters, attentive publics, and the general public are to the expressed agenda of Republicans and Democrats in Congress over the two-year period studied. Higher coefficients indicate that groups tend to discuss the same issues.

TABLE 3. Correlation in Issue Attention Between Members of Congress and Groups of the Public and the Media Over 46 Political Issues

These initial results show potential for corroborating the presence of political responsiveness at the issue attention level, and they seem to indicate that some responsiveness models have a stronger explanatory power than others. In particular, these results provide stronger support for the Supporter and, to a lesser extent, the Attentive models, than for the Downsian argument. There is a positive, and in some cases large, correlation between the agenda of members of Congress and the issues discussed by their constituents. Nevertheless, when paying attention to the coefficients for specific groups, we observe the highest correlations to be between members and their party supporters (0.69 for Democrats and 0.77 for Republicans) and between members and the attentive public (0.49 for Democrats and 0.52 for Republicans). The correlation between the expressed agenda of legislators and the attention allocation of supporters of the other party is much lower (a 0.41 correlation between Democratic members and Republican supporters, and 0.51 between Republican members and Democratic supporters). We observe the lowest correlation coefficients when comparing the agenda of lawmakers and the issues the general public discuss. As expected, the issue attention distribution of the media is also highly correlated to the agenda of members of Congress, emphasizing mass media’s agenda-setting and mediating role.

In Figure 1, we provide information about the average daily attention that each party in Congress, each public group, and the media paid to the political issues under study. This figure provides a more detailed understanding of the agenda level correlations we observe in Table 3 and some potential reasons as to why we observe a particularly strong relationship between the issue agenda of members of Congress and the attention distribution of their party supporters. We see, for example, how Democrats in Congress and Democratic supporters paid much more general attention to the Affordable Care Act (row 3 of figure 1) and Marriage Equality (row 15) than did Republicans, whereas Republicans in Congress and Republican supporters paid more attention than did Democrats to the (troubled) release and implementation of the ACA website (11th to the last row), its employer mandate clause (last row), and to the Border Crisis in Texas (10th to the last row). The attentive public, and especially the general public, paid less attention not only to these issues but also to all political issues in general. (See Online Appendix G.4 for further discussion of these results.)

FIGURE 1. Average Issue Attention by Groups of Politicians, the Public, and the Media

Note: Attention is represented as daily posterior LDA topic probabilities expressed in percentages. These are percentages based on all 100 topics of the LDA model.

Who Leads? Who Follows?

The previous correlations and percentages, however, are not sufficient evidence to conclude that members of Congress lead the issue attention distribution of their constituents nor to adjudicate between the competing Downsian, Attentive, and Supporter models. Here, we take advantage of the time series nature of our dataset to establish who puts issues on the agenda first by estimating a VAR models with topic-fixed effects. These models are well-suited to capture the relationship between endogenous variables (Freeman, Williams, and Lin Reference Freeman, Williams and Lin1989; Sims Reference Sims1980) and have been used in previous political science studies with similar objectives (Edwards and Wood Reference Edwards and Wood1999; Enders and Sandler Reference Enders and Sandler1993; Wood and Peake Reference Wood and Peake1998).Footnote 15

In our VAR model, we have a set of stationary time series Y i representing the proportion of daily attention each of our groups i Footnote 16 paid to each topic j in day t of the 113th Congress.Footnote 17 The values of these random variables range from 0 to 1 but neither of the extreme values are present (0 < Y ijt < 1). Their distributions are right skewed, with few days of very high issue attention and much lower attention during the rest of the two-year period. We follow a common practice in time series analysis of skewed proportions (Wallis Reference Wallis1987) and model the log odds Z i of the described series Y i instead of the raw proportions.

We then express the autoregressive and endogenous relationship of these variables as a system of equations in which each variable Z i is a function of its previous lags plus the lags of the other variables. Given that there are no time restrictions when it comes to posting messages on Twitter, we would theoretically expect members of Congress to follow changes in public issue attention quite rapidly. However, to account for the potential of longer-term decay we use a seven-lag structure.Footnote 18 The final model can be & formally expressed as follows:

(1)$$\eqalign{ &#x0026; Z = \log \left( {{Y \over {1 - Y}}} \right) \cr &#x0026; {Z_{i,j,t}} = {\alpha _j} + \sum\limits_i {\sum\limits_{p = 1}^7 {{\beta _{i,p}}{Z_{i,j,t - p}} + {\varepsilon _{i,j,t}}} } . \cr}$$

Note that given the issue-fixed effects structure of the model (α j), we are assuming that the estimates of interest are constant across issues. Although this is an inaccurate assumption, it is a useful one for what we intend to accomplish here. It allows us to estimate how much on average we should expect changes in issue attention by a given group to predict subsequent attention allocation of the other groups.Footnote 19

The results of the estimated VAR model can be best expressed using cumulative impulse response functions (IRFs). These cumulative IRFs indicate how an x-unit increase in attention to a given topic by a group predicts the cumulative attention that other actors dedicate to the same topic over time. Cumulative IRFs can be calculated for a varying number of subsequent days. We calculate and report in Figure 2 two different types of IRFs for a 15-day period.Footnote 20 In both cases, we assume that at day 0 none of the groups is paying attention to a given issue j. First, we want to explore the effect of brief changes in attention and we calculate how a 10 percentage point increase in attention to an issue by each group (going from 0% to 10% of attention in day 0) affects future issue attention by the other groups. We are also interested in the effect of attention changes that last longer and calculate how a permanent attention change to a given issue from 0% to 10% by one group affects the attention of the others.Footnote 21 Each single panel in Figure 2 shows how much more cumulative attention to the issue the group in the panel title is predicted to pay after a one-time (in gray) and permanent (in black) 10 percentage point increase by the groups along the y-axis (the row groups) 15 days ago. The predicted responses (95% confidence interval lines) are expressed in percentage points (0–100 scale). Most one-time effects (gray coefficients) range from 0 to 4. We believe these are meaningful and substantive responses. As we know from the long-standing literature on agenda setting, politics is often a fight for attention and simply getting an issue into the agenda is extremely difficult (Jones and Baumgartner Reference Jones and Baumgartner2005; Schattschneider Reference Schattschneider1975). Moreover, dynamics of attention often follow nonlinear functions with tipping points, where small amounts of additional attention around the tipping point can have large political implications (Baumgartner and Jones Reference Baumgartner and Jones1993; Kingdon Reference Kingdon2013).

FIGURE 2. 15 Day Cumulative IRFs: Predicted Issue Responsiveness Across Groups

Note: The coefficients (with 95% confidence intervals) indicate (in percentage points) how much more cumulative attention the groups in the panel titles paid to a given issue as a result of the groups in the y-axis increasing the attention to the same issue by 10 percentage points once (in gray) and permanently (in black) 15 days ago.

The results in Figure 2 corroborate the first two expectations in regard to the ability of members of Congress and the public to predict each other’s issue attention. Politicians from both parties (first two rows in all the panels in Figure 2) are able to predict the attention distribution of the public (H2). Specifically, they are able to lead the issue attention of party supporters and attentive publics, although both parties lead the issue attention of their own supporters than supporters of the opposing party. And we see in the far right panel that the issues prioritized by both parties appear to be very poor predictors of the issue attention of the general public.

We also find strong evidence supporting a political responsiveness dynamic (H1): we see changes in issue attention by citizens to be positive predictors of the issues members of Congress discuss, and we also see these effects to always be of a larger magnitude than the ability of members of Congress to lead the agenda of the public. The ability of Republican supporters to set political issue agendas represents the most extreme case. This group of the public is predicted to increase the cumulative amount of attention to an issue only by 0.75 and 1.25 percentage points 15 days after a 10-point increase in attention by Democrats and Republicans in Congress, respectively (first and second gray estimates from the top in the fourth panel from the right). The changes in the opposite direction, however, are two to three times larger: both Democrats and Republicans in Congress are predicted to increase their cumulative attention by about 3 percentage points (Republican supporters’ gray estimates in the two most left panels).

These differential effects can be better appreciated in Figure 3, where we rearrange the one-time attention changes estimates (gray responses) from Figure 2 to more easily compare who has the largest ability to lead the issue agenda of the other, members of Congress or the public. However, there is an additional factor we need to take into consideration when interpreting these results. As we observed in Figure 5 and Table A.6, politicians devote more attention to political issues than their party supporters (and much more than the attentive and general public). This means that although an increase in attention of the same size has a larger effect when it goes from the public to politicians, we are more likely to observe members of Congress, rather than the public, to make large shifts in political attention.

FIGURE 3. Politicians’ Ability to Set Public Agendas versus the Ability of the Public to Influence Political Agendas

Note: The coefficients (with 95% confidence intervals) indicate (in percentage points) how much more cumulative attention the groups in the panel titles paid to a given issue as a result of the groups in the y-axis increasing the attention to the same issue by 10 percentage points (in black) 15 days ago. The gray coefficients indicate the vice versa effect.

Our results also provide strong evidence in favor of the Supporter model of responsiveness (H5). If we focus only on the variables predicting the agenda of members of Congress the most (two left panels in Figure 2), we observe that the strongest predictors of a positive attention change by lawmakers is a change of attention by their own party supporters. The VAR model predicts Democrats in Congress to go from paying no attention to an issue to dedicating a cumulative attention of approximately 3% as a result of a one-time 10 point attention shift by Democratic supporters, and 7% as a result of permanent 10 point change by their party supporters (Democratic supporter estimates in the left panel). We see Republicans in Congress respond similarly to changes in attention by their own supporters (Republican supporter estimates in the second panel from the left). All the other IRFs for the one-time and permanent attention shocks are of smaller magnitude.

We also find some support for the Attentive model (H4). For example, after a one-time and a permanent 10 percentage point change in attention by the attentive public, Democratic members of Congress are predicted to increase their cumulative attention by 2.25 and 5.75 percentage points, respectively, and Republican policy makers by about 1.75 and 4.25. If we treat the supporters of the other party also as an attentive public (they follow not one but at least three members of Congress in Twitter), we observe a similar pattern. Changes in attention by Democratic Supporters are also predicted to have a positive effect of 2 and 4.25 points on Republican members, and changes by Republican Supporters are predicted to increase the cumulative attention of Democratic lawmakers by 2.5 and 5.5 points. However, the estimated effects are of smaller size than the effects we observed in favor of the Supporter model.

Finally, the results show weak support for the Downsian model (H3). Democratic members of Congress are only predicted to increase their cumulative attention to an issue by 2 and 4.5 percentage points after a one-time and a permanent 10 point increase of attention by the general public. The Republican members’ response is expected to be even lower—their cumulative attention increases only by 1.5 and 3 percentage points. This means that among the different groups of the public, the General Public has the lowest ability to lead the agenda of members of Congress. The effect of a permanent increase of attention by the general public (black general public estimates in the two left panels) is of a similar magnitude to a one-time attention increase by party supporters (Democratic supporter gray estimate in the left panel and Republican supporter gray coefficient in the second panel from the left). Moreover, given the low attention the general public pays to politics, attention shifts of this magnitude (10 percentage points) are unlikely to take place. Nevertheless, when comparing the results of fitting the same model only to data from 2013 with data only from 2014, we interestingly find that politicians are slightly more responsive to the general public during election year (2014) than during a nonelection year (although even during an election year politicians are also more likely to follow shifts in attention by party supporters and the attentive public).Footnote 22

Overall, the results show that politicians are more likely to follow changes in issue attention distribution by their own party supporters than to attentive voters, and that they rarely follow the issue priorities of the general public.

Responsiveness and Issue Relevance

If members of Congress have an interest in being responsive to specific groups of constituents, then we expect that (H6) they should be particularly interested in responding to changes in attention involving issues that are salient to these groups. To test this hypothesis, we first need to estimate by how much each group led the attention that all other groups paid to each separate political topic. To do so, we relax our assumption that the ability of one group to lead the agenda of the others is constant across issues, and we model the data in a different way. In the previous model, we included topic-fixed effects (α j). In this section, we instead estimate 46 separate VAR models, one for each political issue. We include the same endogenous variables into the model, again apply a logit transformation to all time series and use the same seven-lag structure. Then, for each of the VAR issue models, we calculate 15-day cumulative IRFs capturing how a one-time 10 percentage point increase in attention by a specific group predicts the attention of the other groups.

Figure 4 shows the results (15-day IRFs) for each of these 46 VAR models. Each panel reports how the groups in the panel titles are predicted to respond to changes in attention by the other groups: the circles represent the predicted effects (with lines representing a 95% confidence intervals), the colors of the circles show the group to which they are reacting, and the labels on the y-axis (the row labels) indicate the specific issue. To avoid overcrowding the plot, in the two left-most panels we only show the ability of the public groups to lead the expressed agenda of Democrats and Republicans in Congress, and in the four panels on the right we show the reverse effects, the ability of members of Congress to lead public issue attention. We only include the predicted effects for issues where the confidence intervals do not cross zero.Footnote 23

FIGURE 4. Predicted Issue Responsiveness Across Issues and Groups (15 Day IRFs)

Note: The coefficients (with 95% confidence intervals) indicate (in percentage points) how much more/less cumulative attention the groups in the panel titles paid to the issue in the y-axis as a result of a group (identified by the color) increasing the cumulative attention to the same issue by 10 percentage points 15 days ago. Only coefficients not crossing zero have been included. The two left-most panels show the influence of the public on Members of Congress. The four right-most panels show the influence of Democratic and Republican members of Congress on the public. Versions of this figure that also show the coefficients crossing zero are available in Online Appendix C.

The issues are sorted based on the predicted impact of Democratic Supporters on Democrats in Congress: from the issues with the largest estimated impact to the issues with the smallest. Among the top rows, we find issues such as healthcare reform (“Affordable Care Act,” row 1), gun violence (row 2), and minimum wage (row 7). A one-time 10-percentage-point increase on these issues by Democratic supporters is predicted to increase the cumulative attention that Democratic members of Congress pay to them by 2, 1.5, and 1 percentage points.

In the second panel from the left, we see the issues in which Republican supporters more strongly led the agenda of Republicans in Congress by looking at the red circles. The discussions around the IRS scandal (row 35) that took place around mid-2013 and the implementation of the Affordable Care Act and its website problems (row 23) were the issues on which Republican members of congress seemed to follow their supporters the most. In both cases, a one-time 10 percentage point shift is predicted to translate into an increase of cumulative attention of about 2 percentage points 15 days later. The effects represented by gray circles in the left-most panel indicate that social security (row 4) is the issue on which the attentive public was able to lead the discussion of Democratic members the most, and in the second panel, we see that they particularly led the attention that Republican members paid to the protests in Ukraine and Venezuela (row 30). The effects represented by orange circles in the two left panels indicate that discussions by Democratic and Republican members of congress on student debt (row 16) and the use of military force in Syria (row 17), respectively, were among the ones that the general public was able to positively lead the most.

To test the last of our hypothesis, that members of Congress will be more likely to follow shifts in attention on issues to which constituents pay more attention (H6), we build a measure of group issue relevance by calculating the average daily attention each group paid to each topic during the 113th Congress (these averages are displayed in Figure 1). By taking the average, we intend to focus less on how much attention a group paid to a given issue at a particular point in time and to capture instead how important the issue was for that specific group in general.

With this measure of average attention and the estimates from Figure 4 in hand, we can now move to a direct test of H6 by examining correlations between the two. Accordingly, in Figure 5 we plot on the x-axis the average daily attention paid to each issue by each group of the public (see panel titles). In the y-axis, we plot the cumulative attention members of Congress are predicted to pay to each issue as result of the groups in the panel title increasing their attention by 10 percentage points 15 days ago (15 day cumulative IRFs). Each dot is a single predicted response and the lines around them represent 95% confidence intervals. The four top panels show the predicted response of Democratic members of Congress, whereas the bottom ones illustrate the predicted reaction of Republican lawmakers.

FIGURE 5. Correlation Between Public Issue Relevance and the Ability of the Public to Set Political Agendas

Note: The x-axis indicates the average attention the groups in the top panel titles paid to each political topic during the 113th Congress. The y-axis indicates how much more/less cumulative attention Democrats (top four panels) and Republicans in Congress (bottom four panels) paid to these topics as a result of the groups in the top panel titles increasing the attention to the topic by 10 percentage points 15 days ago. Each dot represents a different political issue and the lines around the dots represent 95% confidence intervals. Rows are sorted by the largest effect of Democrats in Congress (left panel).

We find support for the issue relevance hypothesis (H6) only as it relates to the Supporter model. In the top right panel, we observe that changes in attention by Democratic supporters have a larger effect on the agenda of Democrats in Congress when they involve issues Democratic supporters deem relevant (such as gun violence). In the second from the right bottom panel, we also observe a similar pattern for Republicans, with Republican supporters being more likely to lead the expressed agenda of Republicans in Congress on issues that are important to them (such as the discussion around the Internal Revenue Service—IRS). Thus, members of congress appear to behave as if they are more likely to pay attention to the views of their supporters on issues their supporters care more about than on issues they care less.

In Figure 5, we see no support for the issue relevance hypothesis as it relates to the Attentive and Downsian models. On average, Democrats in Congress are more likely to follow changes in attention by the attentive public (including supporters of the other party) on issues that are relevant to these groups (two middle panels on the top). However, these positive correlations are of a very small magnitude. For Republicans in Congress, we do not even see a positive correlation between how much they follow shifts in attention on particular topics and the average attention the attentive public and supporters of the other party paid to the issues (flat yellow and blue lines at the bottom). Finally, we observe no correlation between how much the general public leads politicians’ agenda and the amount of average attention the general public devotes to any given issue. Overall, these results validate the strong findings from Figure 2 in favor of the Supporter model, as well as the lack of evidence in support of the Downsian model of responsiveness.

The Role of the Media

We now turn to an evaluation of the role played by news media in mediating the dynamics identified in this paper. Our data and methods allow us to examine in detail whether the mass media is equally likely to lead, and be led by, politicians and the public—or whether, in contrast, the media strengthens the voice of some groups and increases their ability to lead political agendas.

As a first cut, Table 4 confirms previous work indicating that media coverage reflects both politicians’ and the public’s issue priorities. The table displays correlations between the distribution of attention to issues by the media and that of the other groups under study. We find a particularly strong relationship between media issue attention and the issue attention of politically engaged Americans—that is, party supporters and the attentive public. Less substantial, but still very strong, relationships exist between the issue attention of mass media and that of members of Congress and the general public.

TABLE 4. Correlation in Issue Attention Between Media Outlets and the Other Groups of Analysis

These correlations, of course, do not provide information about the directions of these relationships. For a clearer picture of the role of the media, we take a closer look at the IRF coefficient estimates regarding the media originally displayed in the final column and bottom row of Figure 2, plotting them in more detail in Figure 6. In this figure’s left panel, media coverage is the dependent variable. Plotted here are the estimated impacts of changes in the attention to issues given by politicians and the public on the attention given to these issues by media. Here, we observe that demand-side forces (namely, the priorities of the most politically engaged Americans) are stronger predictors of what the media covers than supply-side forces (i.e., the priorities of members of Congress). These results corroborate the argument that media outlets are particularly likely to follow shifts in attention by the public due to market pressures (Anand and Peterson Reference Anand and Peterson2000; Webster and Ksiazek Reference Webster and Ksiazek2012) and suggest that the effort expended by lawmakers to raise the salience of their favored issues is relatively less influential.

FIGURE 6. Predicted Media Effects

Note: The effects (with 95%) in the left panel indicate how much the media outlets increased their attention to a given issue (in percentage points) 15 days after the groups in the y-axis increased their attention to the same issue by 10 percentage points. The coefficients in the right panel indicates the vice versa effects, how much the groups in the y-axis increase their attention to an issue 15 days after the media increased the attention to the same issue by 10 percentage points.

In the right panel of Figure 6, media coverage is the independent variable. This panel displays the estimated impacts of changes in media issue attention on the attention given to these issues by politicians and the public. Shifts in issue attention by media outlets have the strongest impact on the issue agendas of members of Congress and of party supporters. Notably, in each case, the power of shifts in media attention to predict subsequent shifts in attention among all audiences is greater than the reverse, confirming that media outlets play a crucial role in leading political attention (Soroka Reference Soroka2002; Walgrave, Stuart, and Nuytemans Reference Walgrave, Soroka and Nuytemans2008; Walgrave and Van Aelst Reference Walgrave and Van Aelst2006). Finally, the media effects in Figure 6 suggest that news media contribute to the promotion of a Supporter responsiveness model. Not all groups of the public are equally likely to lead the issues covered by media outlets. The “voice” of Democratic and Republican supporters is stronger than the voice of the attentive citizens and the general public. This is particularly relevant given that, as we observe in the right panel in Figure 6, the issues covered by the media are strong predictors of the subsequently expressed issue agenda of members of Congress.

DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS

It is well known in American politics that politicians and the public tend to pay attention to the same political issues (Jones and Baumgartner Reference Jones and Baumgartner2004), but due to data limitations, the question of who leads whom has previously been unanswered (Burstein Reference Burstein2003). In this paper, we have contributed to answering this open question by characterizing the agenda of members of Congress and their constituents using latent topic modeling applied to the text of the tweets they sent between January 2013 and December 2014 (113th Congress). In doing so, we have been able to create fine-grained political and public agenda measures and to study not only the extent to which members of Congress follow shifts in issue attention by their constituents when deciding what issues to discuss, but also to adjudicate between three competing models of political responsiveness: whether public representatives follow changes in attention by their party supporters, the attentive public, or the general public.

We modeled how public and political agendas predict each other using a VAR model accounting for endogenous and media effects. First, we found a political responsiveness dynamic to be in place during the period of analysis. The public was not only able to lead the expressed agenda of members of Congress, but the magnitude of this phenomenon was greater than that associated with politicians’ ability to lead public agendas: an attention shift from party supporters preceded a larger shift by members of Congress than the attention shift from party supporters following an attention shift by members of Congress. Moreover, we found stronger support for some responsiveness models than others. Our findings suggest that members of Congress are mainly responsive to changes in attention allocation by party supporters and, to a lesser extent, attentive publics. The findings also suggest that mainstream media promote similar dynamics: they are particularly likely to follow the issue preferences of party supporters, and they are likely to lead the issue agenda of members of Congress. In addition, we observed Democrats and Republicans in Congress to particularly follow party supporters on issues that are relevant to them. Finally, we found very little empirical support for the claim that politicians are responsive to the general public.

These issue responsiveness findings align with the literature on policy and policy preference responsiveness that shows that political and policy agendas in the United States are mainly driven by the priorities of strong partisans (Clinton Reference Clinton2006; Kastellec et al. Reference Kastellec, Lax, Malecki and Phillips2015; Shapiro et al. Reference Shapiro, Brady, Brody and Ferejohn1990). The study also supports the claim that, due to existing representation and responsiveness dynamics, political agendas are more polarized than is the American public (Grimmer Reference Grimmer2013). While others show that this is in part a function of geographic sorting and an increasing number of clearly partisan districts (Bishop and Cushing Reference Bishop and Cushing2008; Grimmer Reference Grimmer2013), we show that low political attention by the general public and a higher media coverage of partisan issue preferences is also in part to blame. This has important normative implications for democratic politics, as it could be an indirect factor contributing to political polarization.

Our analysis is limited to the 113th Congress, but we argue that our findings are likely to be generalizable to the current context. Social media usage by members of Congress was already almost universal in 2013. In addition, data from the Pew Research Center show that Twitter penetration among online US adults has remained around 20% for the last five years (Smith and Anderson Reference Smith and Anderson2018). The fact that Twitter has become more central in US politics (Gainous and Wagner Reference Gainous and Wagner2013), particularly after Trump was elected President, means that we would expect our findings to be more valid today and lead to more precise estimates. However, one result that may not hold today may be the notable partisan asymmetry we observe regarding the relationship between legislators and the media, which appears to be stronger for Republicans than for Democrats. This pattern could be a function of the political context, and in particular of which party is in the opposition, or it could be related to structural factors, such as the asymmetric fragmentation of the media system—currently larger on the left than on the right in the United States.

Overall, we illustrated how researchers can use social media communications to uncover agenda setting and responsiveness dynamics. Due to space constraints we had to limit the scope of our analysis, but other basic questions can be examined using this method. For example, is the President able to set political and public agendas? Previous research shows that the President’s ability is limited (Edwards and Wood Reference Edwards and Wood1999), but more recent studies argue that this pattern may have changed in the last few years (Lawrence and Boydstun Reference Lawrence and Boydstun2017; Wells et al. Reference Wells, Shah, Pevehouse, Yang, Pelled, Boehm, Lukito, Ghosh and Schmidt2016)—a finding that may be worth revisiting in the era of Trump’s presidency. Do politicians running in safe versus marginal districts respond to different types of constituents? Do politicians respond differently to constituents’ issue priorities depending on the issues they own? And how would these results differ across institutional or political contexts? In particular, we might have reason to expect higher levels of responsiveness in countries with higher levels of political contestation (Hobolt and Klemmensen Reference Hobolt and Klemmensen2008).

Another accessible topic of study is issue responsiveness at the state level. Existing responsiveness research in the United States studies how the issues discussed by Federal political elites are shaped by public issue and policy preferences (Erikson, Mackuen, and Stimson Reference Erikson, Mackuen and Stimson2002; Jones and Baumgartner Reference Jones and Baumgartner2004; Page and Shapiro Reference Page and Shapiro1983; Stimson, Mackuen, and Erikson Reference Stimson, Mackuen and Erikson1995). A relevant number of political decisions, however, are made at the state level. Are state policy makers responsive to their constituents? What type of constituents? Do federal agendas influence political discussions at the state level? Do we see differential responsiveness dynamics across states? And if so, why? In addition, given a longer time period, one could combine the fine-grained temporal measure that Twitter data offer with the curated topics of the Policy Agenda Project to determine who leads and who follows on each of the 19 issues the project has defined (Jones and Baumgartner Reference Jones and Baumgartner2004). Our hope is that both the findings and methods introduced here can serve as a springboard for research into these and other important topics related to political representation in the future.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055419000352.

Replication materials can be found on Dataverse at: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/AA96D2.

Footnotes

We thank Nick Beauchamp, Ken Benoit, Drew Dimmery, Andrew Eggers, Thorsten Faas, Michael Lewis-Beck, Jennifer Pan, Paul Quirk, Molly Roberts, Annelise Russell, Gaurav Sood, David Sontag, Dustin Tingley, and John Wilkerson for their helpful comments and suggestions to previous versions of this paper. We also gratefully acknowledge financial support for the NYU Social Media and Political Participation (SMaPP) lab from the INSPIRE program of the National Science Foundation (Award #1248077), the Bill and Melinda Gates Foundation, the William and Flora Hewlett Foundation, the Rita Allen Foundation, the John S. and James L. Knight Foundation, and Intel. In addition, we would like to thank the Gordon and Betty Moore Foundation and the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation for their support of the Moore Sloan Data Science Environment, which funded Casas’ time on the project. Replication files are available at the American Political Science Review Dataverse: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/AA96D2.

1 To be clear, we are not, however, addressing the downstream question of whether policy outcomes reflect the preferences of the public or particular groups of the public; see Gilens (Reference Gilens2012) and Gilens and Page (Reference Gilens and Page2014) for an analysis of the determinants of policy outcomes.

2 For example, an increase in public attention to the Dakota Access Pipeline followed by Congressional hearings on a fracking bill (e.g., S.785 of the 114th Congress) would be miscategorized as a case of agenda responsiveness by a commonly used issue classification in responsiveness research, the Policy Agendas Project issue classification (Jones and Baumgartner Reference Jones and Baumgartner2004): both actions would be categorized into Natural Gas & Oil (803) within the Energy category. Although both energy related, these two issues are distinct, and assuming that Congress is reacting and being responsive to a preceding public attention change can be misleading.

3 We further test this assumption in Online Appendix A.

4 There is a fourth main theoretical model that for data limitations we are unable to test in this paper: the argument that policy makers are responsive to wealthier constituents (Gilens Reference Gilens2012).

5 Delli Carpini and Keeter (Reference Delli Carpini and Keeter1996), for example, found that only 30% of Americans know the name of at least one of the two Senators representing their State.

6 The authors do find, however, that when discrepancies exist between the policy preferences of some groups, politicians do tend to be more responsive to some groups: those with a higher income, education, and political sophistication.

7 As will be explained below in the Issue Attention Congruence section, we measure the salience of an issue by calculating the average relative daily attention that different groups of the public paid to each political issue.

8 See Online Appendix F for information regarding the data collection process.

9 A description of the procedure we followed to build this sample can be found in Online Appendix B.

10 We conducted the data collection for the random sample (General Public group) in 2015 and were constrained by Twitter’s rate limits, which do not allow downloading all of user’s tweets. In this case, part of the tweets sent in 2013 by about 44% of the random users are not included in the sample.

11 There are two reasons for this decision. First, LDA assumes that each document is a mixture of topics, which is appropriate for our conceptualization of each day’s tweets as the political agenda that each party within each legislative chamber is trying to push for that specific day. Second, conducting an analysis at the tweet level is complex, given its very limited length. The existing literature on topic modeling of tweets has found that applications that aggregate tweets by author or day outperform those that rely on individual tweets (Hong and Davison Reference Hong and Davison2010).

12 Such as the ones from the Comparative Agendas Project or Manifesto Project.

13 For a general overview of the use of text-as-data methods in political science research, see Grimmer and Stewart (Reference Grimmer and Stewart2013) and Wilkerson and Casas (Reference Wilkerson and Casas2017).

14 To identify the list of relevant political topics, five coders used the information contained in our topic dashboard (pablobarbera.com/congress-lda) to classify each of them into three categories: nonpolitical topics (e.g., Valentine’s Day), political topics but not related to issues (e.g., public communication by House Republicans), and political issues (e.g., gun violence, government shutdown, obamacare). Average intercoder agreement was 83% and Cronbach’s alpha was 0.92. We chose as political issues those where the modal classification (three or more coders) agreed to classify as such.

15 In this paper, we do not control for the agenda of the President for two main reasons. First, Edwards and Wood (Reference Edwards and Wood1999) do not find the President to have a relevant agenda-setting capacity, and second, the method we employ in this paper to measure agendas (proportion of daily tweets on a set of issues) is well suited to measure group but not individual agendas, given that single individuals do not tweet frequently enough to build unbiased measures. Needless to say, revisiting the question of agenda-setting power of the President in the era of Trump would likely be a good idea for future research.

16 Democratic and Republican members of Congress, Democratic and Republican Supporters, Attentive Public, General Public, and Media.

17 We run Augmented Dickey–Fuller unit root tests and confirm that the series are stationary.

18 Autocorrelation and partial autocorrelation functions vary depending on the group and issue series one explores. However, on average we observe autocorrelations to go below 0.1 after 5–9 days, and partial autocorrelations to be below this level after 3–5 days; which indicates that by using a seven-lag structure we are accounting and controlling for the autocorrelation nature of these variables.

19 As we point out in the Discussion, we believe that future work should focus on studying how the effects presented here are conditional on the issue or issue type at hand. The methods advanced here can, for example, be used to clearly test hypotheses on the relationship between issue responsiveness and issue ownership. This is not, however, the immediate purpose of this study.

20 In Online Appendix D, we provide results based on shorter and longer term IRFs.

21 When calculating the IRFs for the permanent 10-percentage-point change, we insert a new increase of attention every day to the covariate of interest until the predicted attention for that group reaches 10% without the need of any extra shock. If we let ϕ represent the attention increase we introduce in the covariate, and $\hat{y}$ the resulting predicted value attention for that same covariate, then we can formally express ϕ during the 15 day period as follows: ${\phi _{i,t}} = \left\{ {\matrix{ {10,} \hfill &#x0026; {{\rm{if}}\;t\;{\rm{is}}\;0} \hfill \cr {10 - {{\hat{y}}_{i,t}}} \hfill &#x0026; {{\rm{if}}\;t\;{\rm{is}}\; > 0} \hfill \cr} } \right.$.

22 See Online Appendix E.1.

23 In Online Appendix C, we show predicted effects for all issues.

References

REFERENCES

Adler, Scott, and Wilkerson, John. 2012. Congress and the Politics of Problem Solving. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aldrich, John H. 1995. Why Parties? The Origin and Transformation of Political Parties in America. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anand, N., and Peterson, Richard A.. 2000. “When Market Information Constitutes Fields: Sensemaking of Markets in the Commercial Music Industry.” Organization Science 11 (3): 270–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, R. Douglas. 1990. The Logic of Congressional Action. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Baumgartner, Frank R., and Jones, Bryan D.. 1993. Agendas and Instability in American Politics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bawn, Kathleen, Cohen, Martin, Karol, David, Masket, Seth, Noel, Hans, and Zaller, John. 2012. “A Theory of Political Parties: Groups, Policy Demands and Nominations in American Politics.” Perspectives on Politics 10 (3): 571–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birkland, Thomas A. 1998. “Focusing Events, Mobilization, and Agenda Setting.” Journal of Public Policy 18 (1): 53–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, Bill, and Cushing, Robert G.. 2008. The Big Sort: Why the Clustering of Like-Minded America Is Tearing Us Apart. New York: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Blei, David M., Ng, Andrew Y., and Jordan, Michael I.. 2003. “Latent Dirichlet Allocation.” The Journal of Machine Learning Research 3: 993–1022.Google Scholar
Boydstun, Amber E. 2013. Making the News: Politics, the Media, and Agenda Setting. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burstein, Paul. 2003. “The Impact of Public Opinion on Public Policy: A Review and an Agenda.” Political Research Quarterly 56 (1): 29–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burstein, Paul. 2014. American Public Opinion, Advocacy, and Policy in Congress: What the Public Wants and what it Gets. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Campbell, Angus, Converse, Philip E., Miller, Warren E., and Stokes, Donald E.. 1960. The American Voter. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Campbell, James E., Dettrey, Bryan J., and Yin, Hongxing. 2010. “The Theory of Conditional Retrospective Voting: Does the Presidential Record Matter Less in Open-Seat Elections?The Journal of Politics 72 (4): 1083–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Canes-Wrone, Brandice, and Shotts, Kenneth W.. 2004. “The Conditional Nature of Presidential Responsiveness to Public Opinion.” American Journal of Political Science 48 (4): 690–706.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casas, Andreu, and Morar, David. 2015. “Different Channel, Same Strategy? Filling Empirical Gaps in congress Literature.” In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the American Political Science Association, eds. Layna Mosley and Alvin Bernard Tillery. San Francisco: American Political Science Association. 121. Available at SSRN: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3395307.Google Scholar
Caughey, Devin, and Warshaw, Christopher. 2018. “Policy Preferences and Policy Change: Dynamic Responsiveness in the American States, 1936–2014.” American Political Science Review 112 (2): 249–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, Jonathan, Gerrish, Sean, Wang, Chong, Boyd-graber, Jordan L., and Blei, David M.. 2009. “Reading Tea Leaves: How Humans Interpret Topic Models.” In Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems. Vol. 22, eds.: Bengio, Yoshua, Schuurmans, Dale, Lafferty, John, Williams, Chris, and Culotta, Aaron. Vancouver, BC: Curran Associates, Inc., 288–96.Google Scholar
Chen, Kaiping, Lee, Nathan, and Marble, William. 2018. “Do Elected Officials Listen to Constituents on Social Media? Survey Evidence from Local Politicians in the United States.” Working paper. Available at SSRN: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3251651 or http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3251651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chi, Feng, and Yang, Nathan. 2011. “Twitter Adoption in congress.” Review of Network Economics 10 (1): 146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clinton, Joshua D. 2006. “Representation in congress: Constituents and Roll Calls in the 106th House.” The Journal of Politics 68 (2): 397–409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Converse, Philip E. 2006. “The Nature of Belief Systems in Mass Publics.” Critical Review 18 (1–3): 1–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delli Carpini, Michael X., and Keeter, Scott. 1996. What Americans Know about Politics and Why it Matters. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
de Vreese, Claes H., and Boomgaarden, Hajo. 2006. “News, Political Knowledge and Participation: The Differential Effects of News media Exposure on Political Knowledge and Participation.” Acta Politica 41 (4): 317–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Downs, Anthony. 1957. An Economic Theory of Democracy. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Edwards, George C., and Wood, B. Dan. 1999. “Who Influences Whom? The President, congress, and the media.” American Political Science Review 93 (2): 327–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egan, Patrick J. 2013. Partisan Priorities: How Issue Ownership Drives and Distorts American Politics. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Enders, Walter, and Sandler, Todd. 1993. “The Effectiveness of Antiterrorism Policies: A Vector-Autoregression-Intervention Analysis.” American Political Science Review 87 (4): 829–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erikson, Robert S., Mackuen, Michael B., and Stimson, James A.. 2002. The Macro Polity. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Evans, Heather K., Cordova, Victoria, and Sipole, Savannah. 2014. “Twitter Style: An Analysis of How House Candidates Used Twitter in Their 2012 Campaigns.” PS: Political Science & Politics 47 (2): 454–62.Google Scholar
Feezell, Jessica T. 2018. “Agenda Setting through Social media: The Importance of Incidental News Exposure and Social Filtering in the Digital Era.” Political Research Quarterly 71 (2): 482–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenno, Richard F. 1978. Home Style: House Members in Their Districts. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Freeman, John R., Williams, John T., and Lin, Tse-min. 1989. “Vector Autoregression and the Study of Politics.” American Journal of Political Science 33 (4): 842–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gainous, Jason, and Wagner, Kevin M.. 2013. Tweeting to Power: The Social Media Revolution in American Politics. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geer, J.G. 1996. From Tea Leaves to Opinion Polls: A Theory of Democratic Leadership. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Gerber, Alan S., Karlan, Dean, and Bergan, Daniel. 2009. “Does the media Matter? A Field experiment Measuring the Effect of Newspapers on Voting Behavior and Political Opinions.” American Economic Journal: Applied Economics 1 (2): 35–52.Google Scholar
Gerber, Elisabeth R., and Morton, Rebecca B.. 1998. “Primary Election Systems and Representation.” Journal of Law, Economics, and Organization 14 (2): 304–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilens, Martin. 2012. Affluence and Influence: Economic Inequality and Political Power in America. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilens, Martin, and Page, Benjamin I.. 2014. “Testing Theories of American Politics: Elites, Interest Groups, and Average Citizens.” Perspectives on Politics 12 (3): 564–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gitlin, Todd. 1980. The Whole World Is Watching: Mass Media in the Making & Unmaking of the New Left. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Golbeck, Jennifer, Grimes, Justin M., and Rogers, Anthony. 2010. “Twitter Use by the US Congress.” Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology 61 (8): 1612–21.Google Scholar
Graber, Doris A. 1997. Mass Media and American Politics. Washington, D.C.: CQ Press.Google Scholar
Grimmer, Justin. 2013. “Appropriators Not Position Takers. the Distorting Effects of Electoral Incentives on Congressional Representation.” American Journal of Political Science. 57 (3): 624–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grimmer, Justin, and Stewart, Brandon M.. 2013. “Text as Data: The Promise and Pitfalls of Automatic Content Analysis Methods for Political Texts.” Political Analysis 21 (3): 267–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Habel, Philip D. 2012. “Following the Opinion Leaders? The Dynamics of Influence Among media Opinion, the Public, and Politicians.” Political Communication 29 (3): 257–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hemphill, Libby, Otterbacher, Jahna, and Shapiro, Matthew. 2013. “What’s Congress Doing on Twitter?” In Proceedings of the 2013 Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work, eds. Amy Bruckman, Scott Counts, Cliff Lampe, and Loren Terveen. New York: ACM, 877–86.Google Scholar
Hemphill, Libby, and Roback, Andrew J.. 2014. “Tweet Acts: How Constituents Lobby congress via Twitter.” In Proceedings of the 17th ACM Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work & Social Computing, eds. Susan Fussell, Wayne Lutters, Meredith Ringel Morris, and Madhu Reddy. New York: ACM, 1200–10.Google Scholar
Hibbing, John R., and Theiss-Morse, Elizabeth. 2002. Stealth Democracy: Americans’ Beliefs about How Government Should Work. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobolt, Sara, and Klemmensen, Robert. 2008. “Government Responsiveness and Political Competition in Comparative Perspective.” Comparative Political Studies 41 (3): 309–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holbrook, Thomas M., and McClurg, Scott D.. 2005. “The Mobilization of Core Supporters: Campaigns, Turnout, and Electoral Composition in United States Presidential Elections.” American Journal of Political Science 49 (4): 689–703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hong, Liangjie, and Davison, Brian D.. 2010. “Empirical Study of Topic Modeling in Twitter.” In Proceedings of the First Workshop on Social Media Analytics, eds. Prem Melville, Jure Leskovec, and Foster Provost. New York: ACM, 80–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iyengar, Shanto, and Kinder, Donald R.. 2010. News that Matters: Television and American Opinion. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, Lawrence R., and Shapiro, Robert Y.. 1997. “The Myth of the Pandering Politician.” The Public Perspective 8: 3–5.Google Scholar
Jacobs, Lawrence R., and Shapiro, Robert Y.. 2000. Politicians Don’t Pander: Political Manipulation and the Loss of Democratic Responsiveness: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Jones, Bryan D. 2004. Reconceiving Decision-Making in Democratic Politics: Attention, Choice, and Public Policy. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Jones, Bryan D., and Baumgartner, Frank R.. 2004. “Representation and Agenda Setting.” Policy Studies Journal 32 (1): 1–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, Bryan D., and Baumgartner, Frank R.. 2005. The Politics of Attention: How Government Prioritizes Problems. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kastellec, Jonathan P., Lax, Jeffrey R., Malecki, Michael, and Phillips, Justin H.. 2015. “Polarizing the Electoral Connection: Partisan Representation in Supreme Court Confirmation Politics.” The Journal of Politics 77 (3): 787–804.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katz, Elihu, and Lazarsfeld, Paul F.. 1955. Personal Influence: The Party Played by People in the Flow of Mass Communications. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
King, Gary, Schneer, Benjamin, and White, Ariel. 2017. “How the News media Activate Public Expression and Influence National Agendas.” Science 358 (6364): 776–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kingdon, John W. 2013. Agendas, Alternatives, and Public Policies. London, UK: Pearson Education Limited.Google Scholar
Ladd, Jonathan M., and Lenz, Gabriel S.. 2009. “Exploiting a Rare Communication Shift to Document the Persuasive Power of the News media.” American Journal of Political Science 53 (2): 394–410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lau, Richard R., and Redlawsk, David P.. 2006. How Voters Decide: Information Processing in Election Campaigns. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lawrence, Regina G., and Boydstun, Amber E.. 2017. “What We Should Really Be Asking about media Attention to Trump.” Political Communication 34 (1): 150–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lax, Jeffrey R., and Phillips, Justin H.. 2011. “The Democratic Deficit in the States.” American Journal of Political Science 56 (1): 148–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lupia, Arthur, McCubbins, Mathew D., and Arthur, Lupia. 1998. The Democratic Dilemma: Can Citizens Learn what They Need to Know? New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McCombs, Maxwell E., and Shaw, Donald L.. 1972. The Agenda-Setting Function of Mass media. Public Opinion Quarterly 36 (2): 176–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neundorf, Anja, and Adams, James. 2018. “The Micro-foundations of Party Competition and Issue Ownership: The Reciprocal Effects of Citizens’ Issue Salience and Party Attachments.” British Journal of Political Science 48 (2): 385–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Connor, Brendan, Balasubramanyan, Ramnath, Routledge, Bryan R., and Smith, Noah A.. 2010. “From Tweets to Polls: Linking Text Sentiment to Public Opinion Time Series.” In Fourth International AAAI Conference on Weblogs and Social Media, eds. Marti Hears, William Cohen, and Samuel Gosling. Menlo Park, CA: The AAAI Press, 122–8.Google Scholar
Page, Benjamin I. 1994. “Democratic Responsiveness? Untangling the Links between Public Opinion and Policy.” PS: Political Science and Politics 27 (1): 25–9.Google Scholar
Page, Benjamin I., and Shapiro, Robert Y.. 1983. “Effects of Public Opinion on Policy.” American Political Science Review 77 (1): 175–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Page, Benjamin I., and Shapiro, Robert Y.. 1992. The Rational Public: Fifty Years of Trends in American’s Policy Preferences. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quinn, Kevin M., Monroe, Burt L., Colaresi, Michael, Crespin, Michael H., and Radev, Dragomir R.. 2010. “How to Analyze Political Attention with Minimal Assumptions and Costs.” American Journal of Political Science 54 (1): 209–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, Annelise. 2018a. “The Politics of Prioritization: Senators’ Attention in 140 Characters.” The Forum 16 (2): 331–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, Annelise. 2018b. “US Senators on Twitter: Asymmetric Party Rhetoric in 140 Characters.” American Politics Research 46 (4): 695–723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schattschneider, Elmer E. 1975. The Semisovereign People: A Realist’s View of Democracy in America. New York: Dryden Press.Google Scholar
Schlozman, Kay Lehman, Verba, Sidney, and Brady, Henry E.. 2012. The Unheavenly Chorus: Unequal Political Voice and the Broken Promise of American Democracy. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Shapiro, Catherine R., Brady, David W., Brody, Richard A., and Ferejohn, John A.. 1990. “Linking Constituency Opinion and Senate Voting Scores: A Hybrid Explanation.” Legislative Studies Quarterly 15 (4): 599–621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shapiro, Matthew A., and Hemphill, Libby. 2017. “Politicians and the Policy Agenda: Does Use of Twitter by the US Congress Direct New York Times Content?Policy & Internet 9 (1): 109–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shapiro, Matthew A., Hemphill, Libby, and Otterbacher, Jahna. 2012. “Doing what I Say: Connecting Congressional Social media Behavior and Congressional Voting.” In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Midwest Political Science Association, eds. Rogowski, Jon, Sinclair, Betsy. Chicago: Midwest Political Science Association, 132.Google Scholar
Shoemaker, Stephen, and Reese, Pamela. 1996. Mediating the Message: Theories of Influences on Mass media Content. New York: Longman.Google Scholar
Sims, Christopher A. 1980. “Macroeconomics and Reality.” Econometrica 48 (1): 148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, Aaron, and Anderson, Monica. 2018. Social Media Use in 2018. Washington, DC: Pew Internet & American Life Project. Retrieved March 3, 2018.Google Scholar
Sniderman, Paul M., Brody, Richard A., and Tetlock, Phillip E.. 1993. Reasoning and Choice: Explorations in Political Psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Soroka, Stuart N. 2002. “Issue Attributes and Agenda-Setting by media, the Public, and Policymakers in Canada.” International Journal of Public Opinion Research 14 (3): 264–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soroka, Stuart N., and Wlezien, Christopher. 2009. Degrees of Democracy: Politics, Public Opinion, and Policy. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stimson, James A., Mackuen, Michael B., and Erikson, Robert S.. 1995. “Dynamic Representation.” American Political Science Review 89 (3): 543–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sulkin, Tracy. 2005. Issue Politics in Congress. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tausanovitch, Chris, and Warshaw, Christopher. 2014. “Representation in Municipal Government.” American Political Science Review 108 (3): 605–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walgrave, Stefaan, Soroka, Stuart, and Nuytemans, Michiel. 2008. “The Mass media’s Political Agenda-Setting Power: A Longitudinal Analysis of Media, Parliament, and Government in Belgium (1993 to 2000).” Comparative Political Studies 41 (6): 814–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walgrave, Stefaan, and Van Aelst, Peter. 2006. “The Contingency of the Mass Media’s Political Agenda Setting Power: Toward a Preliminary Theory.” Journal of Communication 56 (1): 88–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallis, Kenneth F. 1987. “Time Series Analysis of Bounded Economic Variables.” Journal of Time Series Analysis 8 (1): 115–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webster, James G., and Ksiazek, Thomas B.. 2012. “The Dynamics of Audience Fragmentation: Public Attention in an Age of Digital media.” Journal of Communication 62 (1): 39–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, Chris, Shah, Dhavan V., Pevehouse, Jon C., Yang, JungHwan, Pelled, Ayellet, Boehm, Frederick, Lukito, Josephine, Ghosh, Shreenita, and Schmidt, Jessica L.. 2016. “How Trump Drove Coverage to the Nomination: Hybrid Media Campaigning.” Political Communication 33 (4): 669–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkerson, John, and Casas, Andreu. 2017. “Large-scale Computerized Text Analysis in Political Science: Opportunities and Challenges.” Annual Review of Political Science 20 (1): 529–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wlezien, Christopher. 2004. “Patterns of Representation: Dynamics of Public Preferences and Policy.” The Journal of Politics 66 (1): 1–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, B. Dan, and Peake, Jeffrey S.. 1998. “The Dynamics of Foreign Policy Agenda Setting.” American Political Science Review 92 (1): 173–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wouters, Ruud, and Walgrave, Stefaan. 2017. “Demonstrating Power: How Protest Persuades Political Representatives.” American Sociological Review 82 (2): 361–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, Gerald C. 1989. “Policy Voting in the US Senate: Who Is Represented?Legislative Studies Quarterly 14 (4): 465–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zaller, John R. 1992. The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

TABLE 1. Description of the Tweets in the Dataset

Figure 1

TABLE 2. List of Political Issues

Figure 2

TABLE 3. Correlation in Issue Attention Between Members of Congress and Groups of the Public and the Media Over 46 Political Issues

Figure 3

FIGURE 1. Average Issue Attention by Groups of Politicians, the Public, and the MediaNote: Attention is represented as daily posterior LDA topic probabilities expressed in percentages. These are percentages based on all 100 topics of the LDA model.

Figure 4

FIGURE 2. 15 Day Cumulative IRFs: Predicted Issue Responsiveness Across GroupsNote: The coefficients (with 95% confidence intervals) indicate (in percentage points) how much more cumulative attention the groups in the panel titles paid to a given issue as a result of the groups in the y-axis increasing the attention to the same issue by 10 percentage points once (in gray) and permanently (in black) 15 days ago.

Figure 5

FIGURE 3. Politicians’ Ability to Set Public Agendas versus the Ability of the Public to Influence Political AgendasNote: The coefficients (with 95% confidence intervals) indicate (in percentage points) how much more cumulative attention the groups in the panel titles paid to a given issue as a result of the groups in the y-axis increasing the attention to the same issue by 10 percentage points (in black) 15 days ago. The gray coefficients indicate the vice versa effect.

Figure 6

FIGURE 4. Predicted Issue Responsiveness Across Issues and Groups (15 Day IRFs)Note: The coefficients (with 95% confidence intervals) indicate (in percentage points) how much more/less cumulative attention the groups in the panel titles paid to the issue in the y-axis as a result of a group (identified by the color) increasing the cumulative attention to the same issue by 10 percentage points 15 days ago. Only coefficients not crossing zero have been included. The two left-most panels show the influence of the public on Members of Congress. The four right-most panels show the influence of Democratic and Republican members of Congress on the public. Versions of this figure that also show the coefficients crossing zero are available in Online Appendix C.

Figure 7

FIGURE 5. Correlation Between Public Issue Relevance and the Ability of the Public to Set Political AgendasNote: The x-axis indicates the average attention the groups in the top panel titles paid to each political topic during the 113th Congress. The y-axis indicates how much more/less cumulative attention Democrats (top four panels) and Republicans in Congress (bottom four panels) paid to these topics as a result of the groups in the top panel titles increasing the attention to the topic by 10 percentage points 15 days ago. Each dot represents a different political issue and the lines around the dots represent 95% confidence intervals. Rows are sorted by the largest effect of Democrats in Congress (left panel).

Figure 8

TABLE 4. Correlation in Issue Attention Between Media Outlets and the Other Groups of Analysis

Figure 9

FIGURE 6. Predicted Media EffectsNote: The effects (with 95%) in the left panel indicate how much the media outlets increased their attention to a given issue (in percentage points) 15 days after the groups in the y-axis increased their attention to the same issue by 10 percentage points. The coefficients in the right panel indicates the vice versa effects, how much the groups in the y-axis increase their attention to an issue 15 days after the media increased the attention to the same issue by 10 percentage points.

Supplementary material: PDF

Barbera et al. supplementary material

Barbera et al. supplementary material 1

Download Barbera et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 1.2 MB