Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-20T00:29:04.378Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Implicit Language Aptitude

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 August 2020

Gisela Granena
Affiliation:
Universitat Oberta de Catalunya

Summary

It is a well-known fact that some adult second language learners learn more rapidly and/or to a higher level of proficiency than others. Some of these individual differences have been linked to differences in cognitive and perceptual abilities under the umbrella term of 'language aptitude'. The notion of language aptitude has undergone recent developments, one of which is the proposal that language aptitude includes cognitive abilities that involve implicit processes and that are advantageous in learning a language without awareness. This Element defines implicit language aptitude, examines tasks that can be used to measure implicit language aptitude, and provides an overview of relevant research in this area.
Get access
Type
Element
Information
Online ISBN: 9781108625616
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication: 06 August 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, J. R. (1987). Skill acquisition: Compilation of weak-method problem solutions. Psychological Review, 94, 192210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batterink, L., Reber, P. J., Neville, H. J., & Paller, K. A. (2015). Implicit and explicit contributions to statistical learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 83, 6278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berry, D. C., & Dienes, Z. (1993). Implicit learning: Theoretical and empirical issues. Hove, UK: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bertels, J., Franco, A., & Destrebecqz, A. (2012). How implicit is visual statistical learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 14251431.Google ScholarPubMed
Bley-Vroman, R. (1990). The logical problem of foreign language learning. Linguistic Analysis, 20, 349.Google Scholar
Bley-Vroman, R. (2009). The evolving context of the fundamental difference hypothesis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 31, 175198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, P. J., Kwoka, N., & Kempe, V. (2017). Distributional effects and individual differences in L2 morphology learning. Language Learning, 67, 171207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1958). A factor analysis of two foreign language aptitude batteries. Journal of General Psychology, 59, 39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carroll, J. B. (1962). The prediction of success in intensive foreign language training. In Glaser, R (ed.), Training research and education (pp. 87136). Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1981). Twenty-five years of research in foreign language aptitude. In Diller, K (ed.), Individual differences and universals in language learning aptitude (pp. 83118). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B., & Sapon, S. (1959). Modern Language Aptitude Test: Form A. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Chan, C. (1992). Implicit cognitive processes: Theoretical issues and applications in computer systems design. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Oxford.Google Scholar
Cheesman, J., & Merikle, P. M. (1984). Priming with and without awareness. Perception & Psychophysics, 36, 387395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Oxford, UK: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cleeremans, A., Allakhverdov, V., & Kuvaldina, M. (2019). Introduction. In Cleeremans, A, Allakhverdov, V, & Kuvaldina, M (eds.), Implicit learning: 50 years on (pp. 115). London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cleeremans, A., Destrebecqz, A., & Boyer, M. (1998). Implicit learning: News from the front. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 406416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conway, C. M., & Christiansen, M. H. (2005). Modality-constrained statistical learning of tactile, visual, and auditory sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 2439.Google ScholarPubMed
Conway, C. M., Bauernschmidt, A., Huang, S. S., & Pisoni, D. B. (2010). Implicit statistical learning in language processing: Word predictability is the key. Cognition, 114, 356371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Four ways five factors are basic. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 653665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cronbach, L. J. (1970). Essentials of psychological testing (3rd ed.). New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Cronbach, L. J., & Snow, R. E. (1977). Aptitudes and instructional methods: A handbook for research on interactions. New York: Irvington.Google Scholar
Defense Language Institute Foreign Language Center. (2009). Defense language proficiency testing system: Familiarization guide. www.dliflc.edu/wp-content/uploads/2014/04/DLPT-IV_Fam-guide-for-Web-based-test.pdf.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M. (2000). The robustness of critical period effects in second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22, 499533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R. (2007). Skill acquisition theory. In VanPatten, B & Williams, J (eds.), Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction (pp. 97112). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M. (2012). Interactions between individual differences, treatments, and structures in SLA. Language Learning, 62, 189200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M., & Koeth, J. (2011). Cognitive aptitudes for second language learning. In Hinkel, E (ed.), Handbook of research in second language teaching and learning (Vol. 2, pp. 395406). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Dennis, N. A., Howard, J. H., & Howard, D. V. (2006). Implicit sequence learning without motor sequencing in young and old adults. Experimental Brain Research, 175, 153164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Destrebecqz, A., & Cleeremans, A. (2001). Can sequence learning be implicit? New evidence with the process dissociation procedure. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, 343350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 880896.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dienes, Z. (1992). Connectionist and memory array models of artificial grammar learning. Cognitive Science, 16, 4179.Google Scholar
Dienes, Z., & Perner, J. (1999). A theory of implicit and explicit knowledge. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 735755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dienes, Z., Altmann, G., Kwan, L., & Goode, A. (1995). Unconscious knowledge of artificial grammars is applied strategically. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 21, 13221338.Google Scholar
Doughty, C. J. (2003). Instructed SLA: Constraints, compensation, and enhancement. In Doughty, C. J. & Long, M. H. (eds.), Handbook of Second Language Acquisition (pp. 256310). Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doughty, C. J., Campbell, S. G., Mislevy, M. A., et al. (2010). Predicting near-native ability: The factor structure and reliability of Hi-LAB. In Prior, M. T., Watanabe, Y, & Lee, S-K (eds.), Selected proceedings of the 2008 Second Language Research Forum (pp. 1031). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Dulany, D. E., Carlson, A., & Dewey, G. I. (1984). A case of syntactical learning and judgment: How conscious and how abstract? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 541555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (1996). Sequencing in SLA: Phonological memory, chunking and points of order. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 91126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2005). At the interface: Dynamic interactions of explicit and implicit language knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 305352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C., & Cadierno, T. (2009). Constructing a second language. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 7, 111139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emberson, L. L., Conway, C. M., & Christiansen, M. H. (2011). Timing is everything: Changes in presentation rate have opposite effects on auditory and visual implicit statistical learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 10211040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, S. (1990). Cognitive–experiential self-theory. In Pervin, L (ed.), Handbook of personality theory and research (pp. 165192). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49, 709724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, S. (2008). Intuition from the perspective of cognitive–experiential self-theory. In Plessner, H, Betsch, C, & Betsch, T (eds.), Intuition in judgment and decision making (pp. 2337). New York: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Evans, J. S. B. T., & Over, D. E. (1996). Rationality and reasoning. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Feldman, J., Kerr, B., & Streissguth, A. P. (1995). Correlational analyses of procedural and declarative learning performance. Intelligence, 20, 87114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foerde, K., Knowlton, B.J., & Poldrack, R.A. (2006). Modulation of competing memory systems by distraction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103, 1177811783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franco, A., Eberlen, J., Destrebecqz, A., Cleeremans, A., & Bertels, J. (2015). Rapid serial auditory presentation: A new measure of statistical learning in speech segmentation. Experimental Psychology, 62, 346351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fried, L. S., & Holyoak, K. J. (1984). Induction of category distributions: A framework for classification learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 234257.Google ScholarPubMed
Frost, R., Siegelman, N., Narkiss, A., & Afek, L. (2013). What predicts successful literacy acquisition in a second language? Psychological Science, 24, 12431252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gangopadhyay, I. McDonald, M., Ellis Weismer, S., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2018). Planning abilities in bilingual and monolingual children: Role of verbal mediation and inhibitory control. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gebauer, G. F. (2003). Implicit learning and intelligence. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Cambridge.Google Scholar
Gebauer, G. F., & Mackintosh, N. J. (2007). Psychometric intelligence dissociates implicit and explicit learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33, 3454.Google ScholarPubMed
Glicksohn, A., & Cohen, A. (2013). The role of cross-modal associations in statistical learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 11611169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gluck, M., Shohamy, D., & Myers, C. (2002). How do people solve the “weather prediction” task? Individual variability in strategies for probabilistic category learning. Learning & Memory, 9, 408418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graf, P., Mandler, G., & Haden, M. (1982). Simulating amnesic symptoms in normal subjects. Science, 218, 12431244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Granena, G. (2013a). Cognitive aptitudes for second language learning and the LLAMA Language Aptitude Test. In Granena, G & Long, M. H. (eds.), Sensitive periods, language aptitude, and ultimate L2 attainment (pp. 105129). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granena, G. (2013b). Individual differences in sequence learning ability and SLA in early childhood and adulthood. Language Learning, 63, 665703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granena, G. (2016). Cognitive aptitudes for implicit and explicit learning and information-processing styles: An individual differences study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37, 577600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granena, G. (2019). Cognitive aptitudes and L2 speaking proficiency: Links between LLAMA and Hi-LAB. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41, 313336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granena, G., & Yilmaz, Y. (2019a). Corrective feedback and the role of implicit sequence learning ability in L2 online performance. Language Learning, 69, S1, 127156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granena, G., & Yilmaz, Y. (2019b). Language aptitude profiles and the effectiveness of implicit and explicit corrective feedback. In Leow, R (ed.), The Routledge handbook of second language research in classroom learning (pp. 438451). New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gupta, P., & Cohen, N. J. (2002). Theoretical and computational analysis of skill learning, repetition priming, and procedural memory. Psychological Review, 109, 401448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamrick, P. (2015). Declarative and procedural memory abilities as individual differences in incidental language learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 44, 915.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heindel, W. C., Festa, E. K., Ott, B. R., Landy, K., & Salmon, D. P. (2013). Prototype learning and dissociable categorization systems in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 51, 16991708.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howard, J. H., Jr., & Howard, D. V. (1997). Age differences in implicit learning of higher order dependencies in serial patterns. Psychology and Aging, 12, 634656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoyer, W. J., & Lincourt, A. E. (1998). Ageing and the development of learning. In Stadler, M. A., & Frensch, P. A. (eds.), Handbook of implicit learning (pp. 445470). Thousand Oaks: Sage.Google Scholar
Hulstijn, J. H. (2005). Theoretical and empirical issues in the study of implicit and explicit second-language learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 129140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 306340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janacsek, K., Fiser, J., & Nemeth, D. (2012). The best time to acquire new skills: Age-related differences in implicit sequence learning across the human lifespan. Developmental Science, 15, 496505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jimenez, L. (2002). Attention in probabilistic sequence learning. In Jimenez, L (ed.), Attention and implicit learning (pp. 4367). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Kalra, P. (2015). Implicit learning: Development, individual differences, and educational implications. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Harvard University.Google Scholar
Kalra, P., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Finn, A. S. (2019). Evidence of stable individual differences in implicit learning. Cognition, 190, 199211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, S., DeYoung, C., Gray, J., Jiménez, L., Brown, J., & Mackintosh, N. (2010). Implicit learning as an ability. Cognition, 116, 321340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keri, S. (2003). The cognitive neuroscience of category learning. Brain Research Reviews, 43, 85109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, K., & Godfroid, A. (2019). Individual differences in cognitive aptitudes and implicit-explicit knowledge: An SEM approach. Paper presented at the American Association of Applied Linguistics annual conference, Atlanta, GA.Google Scholar
Kirkham, N. Z., Slemmer, J. A., & Johnson, S. P. (2002). Visual statistical learning in infancy: Evidence for a domain general learning mechanism. Cognition, 83, B35B42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knowlton, B., Squire, L. R., & Gluck, M. A. (1994). Probabilistic classification in amnesia. Learning and Memory, 1, 106120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knowlton, B. J., Mangels, J. A., & Squire, L. R. (1996). A neostriatal habit learning system in humans. Science, 273, 13991402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knowlton, B. J., Siegel, A. L., & Moody, T. D. (2017). Procedural learning in humans. In Byrne, J. H. (ed.), Learning and memory: A comprehensive reference (pp. 295312). Oxford: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linck, J. R., Hughes, M. M., Campbell, S. G., et al. (2013). Hi-LAB: A new measure of aptitude for high-level language proficiency. Language Learning, 63, 530566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, M. H. (2014). Second language acquisition and task-based language teaching. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Long, M. H. (2017). Instructed second language acquisition (ISLA): Geopolitics, methodological issues, and some major research questions. Instructed Second Language Acquisition, 1, 744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, M. H., & Doughty, C. (2009). The Handbook of language teaching. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, K. B. (2008). Effortful control, explicit processing, and the regulation of human evolved predispositions. Psychological Review, 115, 10121031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maie, R. (2019). Demystifying the complexity of individual differences under incidental conditions: A conceptual replication and extension. Paper presented at the American Association of Applied Linguistics annual conference, Atlanta, GA.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W., & Tyler, L. (1980). The temporal structure of spoken language processing. Cognition, 8, 171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGeorge, P., Crawford, J. R., & Kelly, S. W. (1997). The relationships between psychometric intelligence and learning in an explicit and an implicit task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 239245.Google Scholar
Meara, P. (2005). LLAMA language aptitude tests. Swansea, UK: Lognostics.Google Scholar
Meisel, J. (2009). Second language acquisition in early childhood. Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft, 28, 534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Misyak, J., & Christiansen, M. (2012). Statistical learning and language: An individual differences study. Language Learning, 62, 302331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan-Short, K., Faretta-Stutenberg, M., Brill-Schuetz, K., Carpenter, H., & Wong, P.C.M. (2014). Declarative and procedural memory as individual differences in second language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 5672.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan-Short, K., Faretta-Stutenberg, M., & Bartlett-Hsu, L. (2015). Contribution of event-related potential research into explicit and implicit second language acquisition. In Rebuschat, P (ed.), Investigating implicit and explicit language learning (pp. 349386). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myers, I., McCaulley, M. H., Quenk, N. L., & Hammer, A. L. (1998). Manual: A guide to the development and use of the Myers–Briggs Type Indicator (2nd ed.). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Newport, E. L., & Aslin, R. N. (2004). Learning at a distance: I. Statistical learning of nonadjacent dependencies. Cognitive Psychology, 48, 127162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pacini, R., & Epstein, S. (1999). The relation of rational and experiential information processing styles to personality, basic beliefs, and the ratio-bias phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 972987.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paciorek, A., & Williams, J. N. (2015). Implicit learning of semantic preferences of verbs. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37, 359382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peretti, C. H., Danion, J. M., Gierski, F., Grange, D. (2002). Cognitive skill learning and aging. A component process analysis. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17, 445459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, C. & Al-Haik, A. (1976). The development of the Defense Language Aptitude Battery (DLAB). Educational and Psychological Measurement, 36, 369380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, L. H., Wynn, V., Gilhooly, K. J., Della Sala, S., & Logie, R. H. (1999). The role of memory in the Tower of London task. Memory, 7, 209231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pimsleur, P. (1966). Pimsleur Language Aptitude Battery (PLAB). New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Poldrack, R. A., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2001). Characterizing the neural mechanisms of skill learning and repetition priming: Evidence from mirror-reading. Brain, 124, 6782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pretz, J. E., Totz, K. S., & Kaufman, S. B. (2010). The effects of mood, cognitive style, and cognitive ability on implicit learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 215219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Price, A. L. (2009). Distinguishing the contributions of implicit and explicit processes to performance of the weather prediction task. Memory and Cognition, 37, 210222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ranta, L. (2008). Aptitude and good language learners. In Griffiths, C (ed.), Lessons from good language learners (pp. 142154). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 855863.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reber, A. S. (1969). Transfer of syntactic structure in synthetic languages. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 115119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reber, A. S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 118, 219235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: An essay on the cognitive unconscious. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Reber, A. S., & Allen, R. (2000). Individual differences in implicit learning: Implications for the evolution of consciousness. In Kunzendorf, R. G. & Wallace, B (eds.), Individual differences in conscious experience (Vol. 20, pp. 227247). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reber, A. S., & Millward, R. B. (1965). Event memory in probability learning. Psychonomic Science, 3, 431432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reber, A. S., & Millward, R. B. (1968). Event observation in probability learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 317327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reber, A. S., Walkenfeld, F., & Hernstadt, R. (1991). Implicit and explicit learning: Individual differences and IQ. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 888896.Google ScholarPubMed
Reber, P., Gitelman, D., Parrish, T., & Mesulam, M. (2003). Dissociating explicit and implicit category knowledge with fMRI. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 574583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rebuschat, P. (2008). Implicit learning of natural language syntax. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Cambridge.Google Scholar
Rebuschat, P. & Williams, J. N. (2006). Dissociating implicit and explicit learning of natural language syntax. In Sun, R & Miyake, N (eds.) Proceedings of the annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, p. 2594. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rebuschat, P. & Williams, J. N. (2009). Implicit learning of word order. In Taatgen, N.A. & van Rijn, H (eds.), Proceedings of the 31th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 425430). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Reed, J., & Johnson, P. (1994). Assessing implicit learning with indirect tests: Determining what is learned about sequence structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 585594.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (1997). Individual differences and the fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit adult second language learning. Language Learning, 47, 4599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, P. (2005). Cognitive abilities, chunk-strength, and frequency effects in implicit artificial grammar and incidental L2 learning: Replications of Reber, Walkenfeld, and Hernstadt (1991) and Knowlton and Squire (1996) and their relevance for SLA. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 235268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saffran, J. R., Newport, E. L., Aslin, R. N., Tunick, R. A., and Barrueco, S. (1997). Incidental language learning. Psychological Science, 8, 101105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saint-Cyr, J. A., Taylor, A. E., Lang, A. E. (1988). Procedural learning and neostriatal dysfunction in man. Brain, 111, 941959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501518.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., & Graf, P. (1986). Effects of elaborative processing on implicit and explicit memory for new associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 432444.Google Scholar
Seger, C. A. (1994). Implicit learning. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 163196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 298, 199209.Google ScholarPubMed
Shanks, D. R., & Johnstone, T. (1999). Evaluating the relationship between explicit and implicit knowledge in a serial reaction time task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 25, 14351451.Google Scholar
Siegelman, N., & Frost, R. (2015). Statistical learning as an individual ability: Theoretical perspectives and empirical evidence. Journal of Memory and Language, 81, 105120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegelman, N., Bogaerts, L., & Frost, R. (2016). Measuring individual differences in statistical learning: Current pitfalls and possible solutions. Behavior Research Methods, 49, 115.Google Scholar
Siegelman, N., Bogaerts, L., Elazar, A., Arciuli, J., & Frost, R. (2018). Linguistic entrenchment: Prior knowledge impacts statistical learning performance. Cognition, 177, 198213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simon, H. A. (1975). The functional equivalence of problem solving skills. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 268288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skehan, P. (1989). Individual differences in second language learning. London: Arnold.Google Scholar
Snow, R. E. (1991). Aptitude-treatment-interaction as a framework for research on individual differences in psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 205216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squire, L. R. (1987). Memory and brain. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Squire, L. R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99, 195231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squire, L. R., & Zola-Morgan, S. (1991). The medial temporal lobe memory system. Science, 253, 13801386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squire, L. R., Knowlton, B. & Musen, G. (1993). The structure and organization of memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 453495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 645665.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stansfield, C., & Winke, P. (2008). Testing language aptitude. In Shohamy, E & Hornberger, N. H. (eds.), Encyclopedia of language and education: Language testing and assessment (2nd ed., Vol. 7, pp. 8194). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Suzuki, Y., & DeKeyser, R. M. (2015). Comparing elicited imitation and word monitoring as measures of implicit knowledge. Language Learning, 65, 860895.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suzuki, Y., & DeKeyser, R. M. (2017). The interface of explicit and implicit knowledge in a second language: Insights from individual differences in cognitive aptitudes. Language Learning, 67, 747790.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thakral, P. P., Kensinger, E. A., & Slotnick, S. D. (2016). Familiarity and priming are mediated by overlapping neural substrates. Brain Research, 1632, 107118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tunney, R. J., & Shanks, D. R. (2003). Subjective measures of awareness and implicit cognition. Memory & Cognition, 31, 10601071.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turk-Browne, N. B., Junge, J. A., & Scholl, B. J. (2005). The automaticity of visual statistical learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 134, 552564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2005a). Individual differences in working memory capacity and learning: Evidence from the serial reaction time task. Memory & Cognition, 33, 213220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vafaee, P., Suzuki, Y., & Kachinske, I. (2017). Validating grammaticality judgment tests: Evidence from two new psycholinguistic measures. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 39, 5995.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, W. C., & Yonelinas, A. P. (2012). Familiarity is related to conceptual implicit memory: An examination of individual differences. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 11541164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Was, C. A., & Woltz, D. J. (2007). Reexamining the relationship between working memory and comprehension: The role of available long-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 86102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. N. (1999). Memory, attention, and inductive learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21, 148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. N. (2005). Learning without awareness. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 269304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willingham, D. B., Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1989). On the development of procedural and declarative knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 10471060.Google Scholar
Winter, B., & Reber, A. S. (1994). Implicit learning and natural language acquisition. In Ellis, N.C. (ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages (pp. 115146). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Witteman, C., van den Bercken, J., Claes, L., & Godoy, A. (2009). Assessing rational and intuitive thinking styles. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 25, 3947.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woltz, D. J. (1990a). Decay of repetition priming effects and its relation to retention from text processing: A study of forgetting. Learning and Individual Differences, 2, 241261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woltz, D. J. (1990b). Repetition of semantic comparisons: Temporary and persistent priming effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 392403.Google Scholar
Woltz, D. J. (1999). Individual differences in priming: The roles of implicit facilitation from prior processing. In Ackerman, P. L., Kyllonen, P. C.,& Roberts, R. D. (eds.), Learning and individual differences: Process, trait, and content determinants (pp. 135156). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woltz, D. J. (2003). Implicit cognitive processes as aptitudes for learning. Educational Psychologist, 38, 95104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woolhouse, L., & Bayne, R. (2000). Personality and the use of intuition: Individual differences in strategy and performance on an implicit learning task. European Journal of Personality, 14, 157169.3.0.CO;2-L>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 441517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zaki, S. R., Nosofsky, R. M., Jessup, N. M., & Unverzagt, F. W. (2003). Categorization and recognition performance of a memory-impaired group: Evidence for single system models. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9, 394406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save element to Kindle

To save this element to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Implicit Language Aptitude
  • Gisela Granena, Universitat Oberta de Catalunya
  • Online ISBN: 9781108625616
Available formats
×

Save element to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Implicit Language Aptitude
  • Gisela Granena, Universitat Oberta de Catalunya
  • Online ISBN: 9781108625616
Available formats
×

Save element to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Implicit Language Aptitude
  • Gisela Granena, Universitat Oberta de Catalunya
  • Online ISBN: 9781108625616
Available formats
×