Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T16:27:50.395Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Child Development in Evolutionary Perspective

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 October 2020

David F. Bjorklund
Affiliation:
Florida Atlantic University

Summary

Natural selection has operated as strongly or more so on the early stages of the lifespan as on adulthood. One evolved feature of human childhood is high levels of behavioral, cognitive, and neural plasticity, permitting children to adapt to a wide range of physical and social environments. Taking an evolutionary perspective on infancy and childhood provides a better understanding of contemporary human development, predicting and understanding adult behavior, and explaining how changes in the early development of our ancestors produced contemporary Homo sapiens.
Get access
Type
Element
Information
Online ISBN: 9781108866187
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication: 29 October 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alexander, R. D. (1989). Evolution of the human psyche. In Mellers, P and Stringer, C (Eds.), The Human Revolution: Behavioural and Biological Perspectives on the Origins of Modern Humans. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press, pp. 455513.Google Scholar
Allen, M., Perry, C., & Kaufman, J. (2018). Toddlers prefer to help familiar people. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 174, 90102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.05.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anzures, G., Wheeler, A., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., Slater, A. M., Heron-Delaney, M., Tanaka, J. W., & Lee, K. (2012). Brief daily exposure to Asian females reverses perceptual narrowing for Asian faces in Caucasian infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112, 484495. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.04.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arbiza, L., Gronau, I., Aksoy, B. A., Hubisz, M. J., Gulko, B., Keinan, A., & Siepel, A. (2013). Genome-wide inference of natural selection on human transcription factor binding sites. Nature Genetics, 45, 723729. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.2658Google Scholar
Baillargeon, R. (2008). Innate ideas revisited: For a principle of persistence in infants’ physical reasoning. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 213. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745–6916.2008.00056.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baillargeon, R., Scott, R., & Bian, L. (2016). Psychological reasoning in infancy. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 159186. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213–115033Google Scholar
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Pijlman, F. T., Mesman, J., & Juffer, F. (2008). Experimental evidence for differential susceptibility: Dopamine D4 receptor polymorphism (DRD4 VNTR) moderates intervention effects on toddlers’ externalizing behavior in a randomized controlled trial. Developmental Psychology, 44, 293300. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012–1649.44.1.293Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1896). A new factor in evolution. American Naturalist, 30, 441451; 536553. https://doi.org/10.1086/276408CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1902). Development and Evolution. New York, NY: McMillan.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York, NY: Freeman.Google Scholar
Barker, J. E., Semenov, A. D., Michaelson, L., Provan, L. S., Snyder, H. R., & Munakata, Y. (2014). Less-structured time in children’s daily lives predicts self-directed executive functioning. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 116. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00593Google Scholar
Barrett, T. M., Davis, E. F., & Needham, A. (2007). Learning about tools in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 43, 352368. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012–1649.43.2.352CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bateson, P. (2002). The corpse of a wearisome debate. Science, 297, 22122213. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1075989CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, P. J. (1993). Memory for gender-consistent and gender-inconsistent event sequences by twenty-five-month-old children. Child Development, 64, 285297. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131452CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, S. R., Apperly, I. A., Chappell, C., Guthrie, C., & Cutting, N. (2011). Making tools isn’t child’s play. Cognition, 119, 301306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beckett, C., Maughan, B., Rutter, M., Castle, J., Colvert, E., Groothues, C., Kreppner, J., Stevens, S., O’Connor, T. G., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2006). Do the effects of early severe deprivation on cognition persist into early adolescence? Findings from the English and Romanian Adoptee Study. Child Development, 77, 696711. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–8624.2006.00898.xGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J. (2005). Differential susceptibility to rearing influence. In Ellis, B. J. and Bjorklund, D. F. (Eds.), Origins of the Social Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and Child Development. New York, NY: Guilford, pp. 139163.Google Scholar
Belsky, J. (2019). Early-life adversity accelerates child and adolescent development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28, 241246. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419837670Google Scholar
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009) Beyond diathesis–stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influence.Psychological Bulletin, 135, 885908. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017376CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Development, 62, 647670. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131166Google Scholar
Bering, J. M., Bjorklund, D. F., & Ragan, P. (2000). Deferred imitation of object-related actions in human-reared juvenile chimpanzees and orangutans. Developmental Psychobiology, 36, 218232. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098–2302Google Scholar
Bick, J., Zeanah, C. H., Fox, N. A., & Nelson, C. A. (2018). Memory and executive functioning in 12-year-old children with a history of institutional rearing. Child Development, 89, 495508. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12952CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjorklund, D. F. (1987). A note on neonatal imitation. Developmental Review, 7, 8692. https://doi.org/10.1016/0273–2297(87)90006–2Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. (1997). The role of immaturity in human development. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 153169. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033–2909.122.2.153Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. (2003). Evolutionary psychology from a developmental systems perspective: Comment on Lickliter and Honeycutt (2003). Psychological Bulletin, 129, 836841. https//doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.6.836CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjorklund, D. F. (2006). Mother knows best: Epigenetic inheritance, maternal effects, and the evolution of human intelligence. Developmental Review, 26, 213–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2006.02.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. (2007). Why Youth is Not Wasted on the Young: Immaturity in Human Development. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. (2015). Developing adaptations. Developmental Review, 38, 1335. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2015.07.002Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. (2016). Prepared is not preformed: Comment on Witherington and Lickliter. Human Development, 59, 235241. https://doi:10.1159/000452289Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. (2018). A metatheory for cognitive development (or “Piaget is dead” revisited). Child Development, 89, 22882302. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjorklund, D. F. (2021). How Children Invented Humanity: The Role of Development in Human Evolution. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. (in press). Humans as a neotenous species. In Alemany-Oliver, M and Belk, R. W., (Eds.), “Like a Child Would Do”: A Multidisciplinary Approach to Childlikeness in Past and Current Societies. Montreal: Universitas Press.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., & Bering, J. M. (2003). Big brains, slow development, and social complexity: The developmental and evolutionary origins of social cognition. In Brüne, M, Ribbert, H, and Schiefenhövel, W (Eds.), The Social Brain: Evolutionary Aspects of Development and Pathology. New York, NY: Wiley, pp. 133151. https://doi.org/10.1002/0470867221.ch7Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., & Causey, K. (2018). Children’s Thinking: Cognitive Development and Individual Differences (6th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., Cormier, C., & Rosenberg, J. S. (2005). The evolution of theory of mind: Big brains, social complexity, and inhibition. In Schneider, W, Schumann-Hengsteler, R, and Sodian, B (Eds.), Young Children’s Cognitive Development: Interrelationships Among Executive Functioning, Working Memory, Verbal Ability and Theory of Mind. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 147174.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., & Ellis, B. J. (2005). Evolutionary psychology and child development: An emerging synthesis. In Ellis, B. J. and Bjorklund, D. F. (Eds.), Origins of the Social Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and Child Development. New York, NY: Guilford, pp. 318.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., & Ellis, B. J. (2014). Children, childhood, and development in evolutionary perspective. Developmental Review, 34, 225264. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2014.05.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., Ellis, B. J., & Rosenberg, J. S. (2007). Evolved probabilistic cognitive mechanisms: An evolutionary approach to gene × environment × development. In Kail, R. V. (Ed.), Advances in Child Development and Behavior, Vol. 35. Oxford: Elsevier, pp. 139. https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-0–12-009735–7.50006–2Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., Gaultney, J. F., & Green, B. L. (1993). “I watch, therefore I can do”: The development of meta-imitation over the preschool years and the advantage of optimism in one’s imitative skills. In Pasnak, R and Howe, M. L. (Eds.), Emerging Themes in Cognitive Development, Vol. 1. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag, pp. 79102. https://doi.org/10.1007/978–1-4613–9223-1_4Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., & Harnishfeger, K. K. (1995). The role of inhibition mechanisms in the evolution of human cognition and behavior. In Dempster, F. N. and Brainerd, C. J. (Eds.), New Perspectives on Interference & Inhibition in Cognition. New York, NY: Academic Press, pp 141173. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012208930–5/50006–4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., & Pellegrini, A. D. (2002). The Origins of Human Nature: Evolutionary Developmental Psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10425–000CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., Yunger, J. L., Bering, J. M., & Ragan, P. (2002). The generalization of deferred imitation in enculturated chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Animal Cognition, 5, 4958. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-001–0124-5Google Scholar
Bloom, P., & Markson, L. (1998). Capacities underlying word learning. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 6773. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01121–8Google Scholar
Bock, J., & Johnston, S. E. (2004). Play and subsistence ecology among the Okavango Delta Peoples of Botswana. Human Nature, 15, 6381. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-004–1004-xGoogle Scholar
Bogin, B. (1999). Patterns of Human Growth (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bogin, B. (2001). The Growth of Humanity. New York, NY: Wiley-Liss.Google Scholar
Bogin, B. (2003). The human pattern of growth and development in paleontological perspective. In Thompson, J. L., Krovitz, G. E., and Nelson, A. J. (Eds.), Patterns of Growth and Development in the Genus Homo. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 1544. https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511542565Google Scholar
Bolk, L. (1926). On the problem of anthropogenesis. Proc. Section Sciences Kon. Akad. Wetens. Amsterdam, 29, 465475.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (1989). Sensitive periods in development: Structural characteristics and causal interpretations. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 179197. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033–2909.105.2.179Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (2009). Toward a model of culture↔parent↔child transactions. In Sameroff, A (Ed.), The Transactional Model of Development: How Children and Contexts Shape Each Other. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 139161. https://doi.org/10.1037/11877–008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, O. M., O’Reilly, A. W., & Painter, K. M. (1996). Solitary and collaborative pretense play in early childhood: Sources of individual variation in the development of representational competence. Child Development, 6, 29102929. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131759CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., & Putnick, D. L. (2019). The Architecture of the Child Mind: g, Fs, and the Hierarchical Model of Intelligence. Abingdon: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429027307Google Scholar
Boulton, M. J., & Smith, P. K. (1990). Affective bias in children’s perceptions of dominance relations. Child Development, 61, 221229. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131061Google Scholar
Boyce, W. T. (2019). The Orchid and the Dandelion: Why Some Children Struggle and How All Can Thrive. New York, NY: Knopf.Google Scholar
Boyce, W. T., & Ellis, B. J. (2005). Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary-developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 271301. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579405050145CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2002). The importance of eyes: How infants interpret adult looking behavior. Developmental Psychology, 38, 958966. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012–1649.38.6.958CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bufill, E., Agustí, J., & Blesa, R. (2011). Human neoteny revisited: The case of synaptic plasticity. American Journal of Human Biology, 23, 729739. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.21225Google Scholar
Bushnell, I. W. R., Sai, F., & Mullin, J. T. (1989). Neonatal recognition of the mother’s face. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 315. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1989.tb00784.xGoogle Scholar
Buss, D. M., Haselton, M. G., Shackelford, T. K., Bleske, A. L., & Wakefield, J. C. (1998). Adaptations, exaptations, and spandrels. American Psychologist, 53, 533548. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.53.5.533Google Scholar
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Enculturated chimpanzees imitate rationally. Developmental Science, 10, F31F38. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–7687.2007.00630.xGoogle Scholar
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Rational tool use and tool choice in human infants and great apes. Child Development, 79, 609626. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–8624.2008.01146.xGoogle Scholar
Buttelmann, D., Zmyj, N., Daum, M., & Carpenter, M. (2013). Selective imitation of in-group over out-group members in 14-month-olds. Child Development, 84, 422428. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–8624.2012.01860.xGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W. (2005). Social cognition: Imitation, imitation, imitation. Current Biology, 15, R489R500. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2005.06.031Google Scholar
Byrnes, J. P., & Fox, N. A. (1998). The educational relevance of research in cognitive neuroscience. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 297342. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022145812276Google Scholar
Caldera, Y. M., Huston, A. C., & O’Brien, M. (1989). Social interactions and play patterns of parents and toddlers with feminine, masculine, and neutral toys. Child Development, 60, 7076. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131072Google Scholar
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1996). The effects of humans on the cognitive development of apes. In Russon, A. E., Bard, K. A., and Parker, S. T. (Eds.), Reaching into Thought: The Minds of the Great Apes. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press, pp. 371403.Google Scholar
Callaghan, T. C., Moll, H., Rakoczy, H., Warneken, F. L. U., Behne, T., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Early social cognition in three cultural contexts. Monographs of the Society of Research in Child Development, 76(2), 34104. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00606.xGoogle Scholar
Cao-Lei, L., Massart, R., Suderman, M. J., Machnes, Z., Elgbeli, G., Laplante, D. P., Szyf, M., & King, S. (2014). DNA methylation signatures triggered by prenatal maternal stress exposure to a natural disaster: Project Ice Storm. PLoS One, 9(9), e107653. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0107653Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., White, R. E., & Davis-Unger, A. C. (2014). Evidence for a relationship between executive function and pretense representation in preschool children. Cognitive Development, 29, 116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2013.09.001Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Tomasello, M., & Striano, T. (1995). Joint attention and imitative learning in children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees. Social Development, 4, 217237. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–9507.1995.tb00063.xGoogle Scholar
Carroll, S. B. (2005). Endless Forms Most Beautiful: The New Science of Evo Devo. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Casler, K., & Kelemen, D. (2005). Young children’s rapid learning about artifacts. Developmental Science, 8, 472480. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–7687.2005.00438.xGoogle Scholar
Chance, M. R. A. (1962). Social behaviour and primate evolution. In Montagu, M. F. A. (Ed.), Culture and the Evolution of Man. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, pp. 84130.Google Scholar
Chang, L., Lu, H. J., Lansford, J. E., Skinner, A. T., Bornstein, M. H., Steinberg, L., Dodge, K. et al. (2019). Environmental harshness and unpredictability, life history, and social and academic behavior of adolescents in nine countries. Developmental Psychology, 55, 890903. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000655CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chappell, J., Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A., & Beck, S. R. (2013). The development of tool manufacture in humans: What helps young children make innovative tools? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 368, 20120409. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0409Google Scholar
Charlesworth, W. R. (1992). Darwin and developmental psychology: Past and present. Developmental Psychology, 28, 516. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012–1649.28.1.5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charrier, C., Joshi, K., Coutinho-Budd, J., Kim, J-E., Lambert, N., de Marchena, J., Jin, W-L., Vanderhaeghen, P., Ghosh, A., Sassa, T., & Polleux, F. (2012). Inhibition of SRGAP2 function by its human-specific paralogs induces neoteny during spine maturation. Cell, 149, 923935. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2012.03.034Google Scholar
Clay, Z. & Tennie, C. (2018). Is overimitation a uniquely human phenomenon? Insights from human children as compared to bonobos. Child Development, 89, 15351544. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12857Google Scholar
Coen, E. (2000). The Art of Genes: How Organisms Make Themselves. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Conradt, E. (2017). Using principles of behavioral epigenetics to advance research on early-life stress. Child Development Perspective, 11, 107112. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12219Google Scholar
Cook, M., & Mineka, S. (1989). Observational conditioning of fear to fear-relevant versus fear-irrelevant stimuli in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 448459. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.98.4.448Google Scholar
Corbit, J., Callaghan, T., & Svetlova, M. (2020). Toddlers’ costly helping in three societies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 195, 104841. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104841Google Scholar
Crabtree, J. W., & Riesen, A. H. (1979). Effects of the duration of dark rearing on visually guided behavior in the kitten. Developmental Psychobiology, 12, 291303. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.420120404Google Scholar
Cummins-Sebree, S. W., & Fragaszy, D. M. (2005). Choosing and using tools: Capuchins (Cebus apella) use a different metric than tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, 210219. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735–7036.119.2.210Google Scholar
Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A., Chappell, C., & Beck, S. R. (2014). The puzzling difficulty of tool innovation: Why can’t children piece their knowledge together? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 125, 110117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.11.010Google Scholar
de Beer, G. (1958). Embryos and Ancestors (3rd ed.). Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
de Klerk, C. C. J. M., Bulgarelli, C., & Hamilton, A. (2019). Selective facial mimicry of native over foreign speakers in preverbal infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 183, 3347. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.01.015Google Scholar
Del Giudice, M., & Ellis, E. J. (2016). Evolutionary foundations of developmental psychopathology In Cicchetti, D (Ed.), Developmental Psychopathology, Vol. 2: Developmental Neuroscience (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Wiley, pp. 158. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119125556.devpsy201Google Scholar
Del Giudice, M., Gangestad, S. W., & Kaplan, H. S. (2016). Life history theory and evolutionary psychology. In Buss, D (Ed.), Evolutionary Psychology Handbook (Vol. 2). New York, NY: Wiley, pp. 88114.Google Scholar
DeLoache, J. S., & LoBue, V. (2009). The narrow fellow in the grass: Human infants associate snakes and fear. Developmental Science, 12, 201207. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–7687.2008.00753.xGoogle Scholar
Denenberg, V. H., & Rosenberg, K. M. (1967). Non-genetic transmission of information. Nature, 216, 549550. https://doi.org/10.1038/216549a0Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1987). The Intentional Stance. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1990). The interpretation of texts, people, and other artifacts. Philosophy and Phenomenological Quarterly, 1 (Suppl.), 177194. https://doi.org/10.2307/2108038Google Scholar
Dennis, E. L., Jahanshad, N., McMahon, K. L., de Zubicaray, G. I., Martin, N. G., Hickie, I. B., Toga, A. W., Wright, M. J., and Thompson, P. M. (2013). Development of brain structural connectivity between ages 12 and 30: A 4-Tesladiffusion imaging study in 439 adolescents and adults. NeuroImage, 64, 671684. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeSilva, J. M. (2016). Brains, birth, bipedalism, and the mosaic evolution of the helpless infant. In Trevathan, W. R. and Rosenberg, K. R. (Eds.), Costly and Cute: Helpless Infants and Human Evolution. Santee Fe, MN: School for Advanced Research Press, pp. 6786.Google Scholar
Dobzansky, T. (1973). Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution. American Biology Teacher, 35, 125129. https://doi.org/10.2307/4444260Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1995). Neocortex size and group size in primates: A test of the hypothesis. Journal of Human Evolution, 28, 287296. https://doi.org/10.1006/jhev.1995.1021Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2003). The social brain: mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32, 163181. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.32.061002.093158Google Scholar
Duncker, K. (1945). On problem-solving. Psychological Monographs, 58, i113. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093599CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Carey, S. (2011). Consequences of “minimal” group affiliations in children. Child Development, 82, 793811. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–8624.2011.01577.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elias, C. L., & Berk, L. E. (2002). Self-regulation in young children: Is there a role for executive function and pretense representation in preschool children. Cognitive Development, 29, 116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2013.09.001Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Bianchi, J., Griskevicius, V., & Frankenhuis, W. E. (2017). Beyond risk and protective factors: An adaptation-based approach to resilience. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 561587. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617693054CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, B. J., Boyce, W. T., Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to the environment: An evolutionary- neurodevelopmental theory. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 728. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000611CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Del Giudice, M., Dishion, T. J., Figueredo, A. J., Gray, P., Griskevicius, V., Hawley, P. H., Jacobs, W. J., James, J., Volk, A. A., & Wilson, D. S. (2012). The evolutionary basis of risky adolescent behavior: Implications for science, policy, and practice. Developmental Psychology, 48, 598623. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026220Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Figueredo, A. J., Brumbach, B. H., & Schlomer, G. L. (2009). Fundamental dimensions of environmental risk: The impact of harsh versus unpredictable environments on the evolution and development of life history strategies. Human Nature, 20, 204268. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-009–9063-7Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Shirtcliff, E. A., Boyce, W. T., Deardorff, J., & Essex, M. J. (2011). Quality of early family relationships and the timing and tempo of puberty: Effects depend on biological sensitivity to context. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 8599. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000660Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Volk, A. A., Gonzalez, J.-M., Embry, D. D. (2016). The meaningful roles intervention: An evolutionary approach to reducing bullying and increasing prosocial behavior. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 26, 622637. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12243Google Scholar
Engelmann, J., Rapp, D., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2018). Concern for group reputation increases prosociality in young children. Psychological Science, 29, 181190. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617733830Google Scholar
Fabes, R. A., Fultz, J., Eisenberg, N., May-Plumlee, T., & Christopher, F. S. (1989). Effects of rewards on children’s prosocial motivation: A socialization study. Developmental Psychology, 25, 509515. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012–1649.25.4.509Google Scholar
Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. H. (2002). Eye contact detection in humans from birth. PNAS, 99, 96029605. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.152159999Google Scholar
Francis, D. D., Diori, J., Liu, D., & Meaney, M. J. (1999). Nongenomic transmission across generations in maternal behavior and stress response in the rat. Science, 286, 11551158. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.286.5442.1155Google Scholar
Frankenhuis, W. E., de Vries, S. A., Bianchi, J. M., & Ellis, B. J. (2019). Hidden talents in harsh conditions? A preregistered study of memory and reasoning about social dominance. Developmental Science, e12835. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12835Google Scholar
Franklin, P., & Volk, A. A. (2018). A review of infants’ and children’s facial cues’ influence on adults’ perceptions and behaviors. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 12, 296321. https://doi.org/10.1037/ebs0000108Google Scholar
Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 3160. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033–2909.134.1.31Google Scholar
Garstang, W. (1922). The theory of recapitulation: A critical restatement of the Biogenetic Law. Proceedings of the Linnean Society of London: Zoology 35, 81101. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1096–3642.1922.tb00464.xGoogle Scholar
Gava, L., Valenza, E., Turati, C., & de Schonen, S. (2008). Effect of partial occlusion on newborns’ face preference and recognition. Developmental Science, 11, 563574. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–7687.2008.00702.xGoogle Scholar
Geary, D. C. (1995). Reflections of evolution and culture in children’s cognition: Implications for mathematical development and instruction. American Psychologist, 50, 2437. https://doi.org/10.1037/10871-000Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2005). The Origin of Mind: Evolution of Brain, Cognition, and General Intelligence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10871–000Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2021). Male, Female: The Evolution of Human Sex Differences (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geary, D. C., & Berch, D. B. (Eds.). (2016). Evolutionary Perspectives on Education and Child Development. New York, NY: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978–3-319–29986-0Google Scholar
Geary, D. C., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2000). Evolutionary developmental psychology. Child Development, 71, 5765. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467–8624.00118Google Scholar
Gellén, K., & Buttelmann, D. (2019). Rational imitation declines within the second year of life: Changes in the function of imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 185, 148163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.04.019Google Scholar
German, T. P., & Johnson, S. C. (2002). Function and the origins of the design stance. Journal of Cognition and Development, 3, 279300. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327647JCD0303_2Google Scholar
Giedd, J. N. (2015). Risky teen behavior is drive by an imbalance in brain development. Scientific American, 312(6), 3337. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0615-32Google Scholar
Giedd, J. N., Bluenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., Paus, T., Evans, A. C, & Rapoport, J. L. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 861863. https://doi.org/10.1038/13158Google Scholar
Gilad, Y., Oshlack, A., Smyth, G. K., Speed, T. P., & White, K. P. (2006). Expression profiling in primates reveals a rapid evolution of human transcription factors. Nature, 440, 242245. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature04559Google Scholar
Gilissen, R., Bakermans‐Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Linting, M. (2008). Electrodermal reactivity during the Trier Social Stress Test for children: Interaction between the serotonin transporter polymorphism and children’s attachment representation. Developmental Psychobiology, 50, 615625. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20314Google Scholar
Glocker, M. L., Langleben, D. D., Ruparel, K., Loughead, J. W., Gur, R. C., & Sachser, N. (2009). Baby schema in infant faces induces cuteness perception and motivation for caretaking in adults. Ethology, 115, 257263. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01603.xGoogle Scholar
Goldhaber, D. (2012). The Nature-Nurture Debates: Bridging the Gaps. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139022583Google Scholar
Gómez, J-C., & Martín-Andrade, B. (2005). Fantasy play in apes. In Pellegrini, A. D. and Smith, P. K. (Eds.), The Nature of Play: Great Apes and Humans. New York, NY: Guilford Press, pp. 139172.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1997). Words, Thoughts, and Theories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1976). The roles of experience in the development of behavior and the nervous system. In Gottlieb, G (Ed.), Neural and Behavioral Plasticity. New York, NY: Academic Press, pp. 2554. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0–12-609303–2.50008-XGoogle Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1987). The developmental basis of evolutionary change. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 101, 262271. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.101.3.262Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1992). Individual Development and Evolution: The Genesis of Novel Behavior. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1997). Synthesizing Nature-Nurture: Prenatal Roots of Instinctive Behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (2007). Probabilistic epigenesis. Developmental Science, 10, 111. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–7687.2007.00556.xGoogle Scholar
Gottlieb, G., Wahlsten, D., & Lickliter, R. (2006). The significance of biology for human development: A developmental psychobiological systems view. In Lerner, R. M. (Vol. Ed.), Theoretical Models of Human Development, Vol. 1, in Damon, W and Lerner, R. M. (Gen. Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology (6th ed.). New York, NY: Wiley, pp. 210257. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0105Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1977). Ontogeny and Phylogeny. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gray, P. (2016). Children’s natural ways of educating themselves still works: Even for the three Rs. In Geary, D. C. and Berch, D. B. (Eds.), Evolutionary Perspectives on Education and Child Development. New York, NY: Springer, pp. 6694. https://doi.org/10.1007/978–3-319–29986-0_3Google Scholar
Gredlein, J. M., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2005). Sex differences in young children’s use of tools in a problem-solving task: The role of object-oriented play. Human Nature, 16, 211232. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-005–1004-5Google Scholar
Greve, W., & Thomsen, T. (2016). Evolutionary advantages of free play during childhood. Evolutionary Psychology, 14, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704916675349Google Scholar
Greve, W., Thomsen, T., & Dehio, C. (2014). Does playing pay? The fitness-effect of free play during childhood. Evolutionary Psychology, 12, 434447. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491401200210Google Scholar
Griffey, J. A. F., & Little, A. C. (2014). Infants’ visual preferences for facial traits associated with adult attractiveness judgments: Data from eye-tracking. Infant Behavior and Development, 37 ¸ 268275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.03.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groos, K. (1898/1975). The Play of Animals. New York, NY: Appleton. https://doi.org/10.1037/12894–000Google Scholar
Gruber, T., Deschenaux, A., Frick, A., & Clément, F. (2019). Group membership influences more social identification than social learning or overimitation in children. Child Development, 90, 728745. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12931CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haith, M. M. (1966). The response of the human newborn to visual movement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 3, 235243. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022–0965(66)90067–1Google Scholar
Hamann, K., Warneken, F., Greenberg, J. R., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Collaboration encourages equal sharing in children but not in chimpanzees. Nature, 476, 328331. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature10278Google Scholar
Hamlin, J. K., Mahajan, N., Liberman, Z., & Wynn, K. (2013). Not like me = bad: Infants prefer those who harm dissimilar others. Psychological science, 24, 589594. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612457785Google Scholar
Hare, B. (2017). Survival of the friendliest: Homo sapiens evolved via selection for prosociality. Annual Review Psychology, 68, 155186. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010416–044201CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heimann, M. (1989). Neonatal imitation gaze aversion and mother-infant interaction. Infant Behavior and Development, 12, 495505. https://doi.org/10.1016/0163–6383(89)90029–5Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61135. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152XGoogle Scholar
Hepach, R. (2016). Prosocial arousal in children. Child Development Perspectives, 11, 5055. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12209Google Scholar
Hernández Blasi, C., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2003). Evolutionary developmental psychology: A new tool for better understanding human ontogeny. Human Development, 46, 259281. https://doi.org/10.1159/000071935Google Scholar
Hernández Blasi, C., Bjorklund, D. F., Agut, S., Lozano, F., & Martínez, M. A. (July, 2018). Vocal cues as signaling behavior in early childhood. 30th Annual Meeting of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
Hernández-Blasi, C., Bjorklund, D. F., & Ruiz Soler, M. (2015). Cognitive cues are more compelling than facial cues in determining adults’ reactions towards young children. Evolutionary Psychology, 13, 511530. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491501300212Google Scholar
Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernandez-Lloreda, M., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317, 13601366. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1146282Google Scholar
Hewlett, B. S., Berl, R. E. W., & Roulette, C. J. (2016). Teaching and overimitation among Aka hunter-gatherers. In Terashima, H and Hewlett, B (Eds.), Social Learning and Innovation in Contemporary Hunter-Gatherers. Tokyo: Springer, pp. 3545. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-55997-9_3Google Scholar
Hill, K., & Kaplan, H. (1999). Life history traits in humans: Theory and empirical studies. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 397430. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.28.1.397Google Scholar
Hoehl, S., Keupp, S., Schleihauf, H., McGuigan, N., Buttelmann, D., & Whiten, A. (2019). ‘Overimitation’: A review and appraisal of a decade of research. Developmental Review, 51, 90108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.12.001Google Scholar
Hood, B. (2014). The Domesticated Brain. London: Pelican.Google Scholar
Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8, 164181. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-004–0239-6Google Scholar
House, B. R., Silk, J. B., Henrich, J., Barrett, H. C., Scelza, B. A., Boyette, A. H., Hewlett, B. S., & Laurence, S. (2013). Ontogeny of prosocial behavior across diverse societies. PNAS, USA, 110, 1458614591. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.12.014Google Scholar
House, B. R., & Tomasello, M. (2018). Modeling social norms increasingly influences costly sharing in middle childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 171, 8498. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.12.014Google Scholar
Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1962). Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex. Journal of Physiology, 160, 106154. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1962.sp006837Google Scholar
Ingram, G. (2014). From hitting to tattling to gossip: An evolutionary rationale for the development of indirect aggression. Evolutionary Psychology, 12, 343363. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491401200205CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ingram, G., & Bering, J. M. (2010). Children’s tattling: A developmental precursor to gossip? Child Development, 81, 945957. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–8624.2010.01444.xGoogle Scholar
Joffe, T. H. (1997). Social pressures have selected for an extended juvenile period in primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 32, 593605. https://doi.org/10.1006/jhev.1997.0140Google Scholar
Jordan, J. J., McAuliffe, K., & Warneken, F. (2014). Development of in-group favoritism in children’s third-party punishment of selfishness. PNAS, 111, 1271012715. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1402280111Google Scholar
Keeley, L. H. (1996). War Before Civilization: The Myth of the Peaceful Savage. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kelly, D. J., Liu, S., Lee, K., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., Slater, A. M., & Ge, L. (2009). Development of the other-race effect in infancy: Evidence toward universality? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 105114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.006Google Scholar
Kenny, P., & Turkewitz, G. (1986). Effects of unusually early visual stimulation on the development of homing behavior in the rat pup. Developmental Psychobiology, 19, 5766. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.420190107Google Scholar
Kenward, B. (2012). Over-imitating preschoolers believe unnecessary actions are normative and enforce their performance by a third party. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112, 195207. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.02.006Google Scholar
Kertes, D. A., Kamin, H. S., Hughes, D. A., Rodney, N. C., Bhatt, S., & Mulligan, C. J. (2016). Prenatal maternal stress predicts methylation of genes regulating the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical system in mothers and newborns in the Democratic Republic of Congo. Child Development, 87, 6172. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12487Google Scholar
Keupp, S., Behne, T., & Rakoczy, H. (2013). Why do children overimitate? Normativity is crucial. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 392406. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.002Google Scholar
Kochanska, G. (1993). Toward a synthesis of parental socialization and child temperament in early development of conscience. Child Development, 64, 325347. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131254Google Scholar
Konner, M. (2010). The Evolution of Childhood: Relationships, Emotions, Mind. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Koomen, R., Grueneisen, S., & Herrmann, E. (2020). Children delay gratification for cooperative ends. Psychological Science, 31, 139148. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619894205Google Scholar
Kringelbach, M. L., Stark, E. A., Alexander, C., Bornstein, M. H., & Stein, A. (2016). On cuteness: Unlocking the parental brain and beyond. Trends in Cognitive Science, 20, 545558. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.003Google Scholar
Lancy, D. (2015). The Anthropology of Childhood (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lancy, D. (2020). Child Helpers: A Multidisciplinary Perspective. Cambridge Elements (K. D. Keith, Ed.). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108769204Google Scholar
Langlois, J., Ritter, J., Casey, R., & Sawin, D. (1995). Infant attractiveness predicts maternal behavior and attitudes. Developmental Psychology, 31, 464472. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012–1649.31.3.464Google Scholar
Le Grand, R., Mondloch, C. J., Maurer, D., & Brent, H. P. (2001). Early visual experience and face processing. Nature, 410, 890. https://doi.org/10.1038/35073749Google Scholar
Legare, C. H., & Nielsen, M. (2015). Imitation and innovation: The dual engines of cultural learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 688699. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.005Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1943). Psychology and the process of group living. Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 113131. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1943.9712269Google Scholar
Lew-Levy, S., Kissler, S. M., Boyette, A. H., Crittenden, A. N., Mabulla, I. A., & Hewlett, B. S. (2020). Who teaches children to forage? Exploring the primacy of child-to-child teaching among Hadza and BaYaka hunter-gatherers of Tanzania and Congo. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41, 1222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2019.07.003Google Scholar
Lewkowicz, D. (2011). The biological implausibility of the nature–nurture dichotomy and what it means for the study of infancy. Infancy, 16, 331367. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532–7078.2011.00079.xGoogle Scholar
Lickliter, R. (1990). Premature visual stimulation accelerates intersensory functioning in bobwhite quail neonates. Developmental Psychobiology, 23, 1527. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.420230103Google Scholar
Lillard, A. S., Lerner, M. D., Hopkins, E. J., Dore, R. A., Smith, E. D., & Palmquist, C. M. (2013). The impact of pretend play on children’s development: A review of the evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 134. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029321Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M. (2006). 12- and 18-month-olds point to provide information for others. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7, 173187. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327647jcd0702_2Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Pointing out new news, old news, and absent referents at 12 months of age. Developmental Science, 10, F1F7. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–7687.2006.00552.xGoogle Scholar
Liu, X., Somel, M., Tang, L., Yan, Z., Jiang, X., Guo, S., Yuan, Y., He, L., Oleksiak, A. Zhang, Y., Li, N., Hu, Y., Chen, W., Qiu, Z., Pääbo, S., & Khaitovich, P. (2012). Extension of cortical synaptic development distinguishes humans from chimpanzees and macaques. Genome Research, 22, 611622. https://doi.org/10.1101/gr.127324.111Google Scholar
LoBue, V., & Adolph, K. E. (2019). Fear in infancy: Lessons from snakes, spiders, heights, and strangers. Psychological Bulletin, 55, 18891907. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000675Google Scholar
Lockhart, K. L., Chang, B., & Tyler, S. (2002). Young children’s belief about the stability of traits: Protective optimism. Child Development, 73, 14081430. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467–8624.00480Google Scholar
Lockhart, K. L., Goddu, M. K., & Keil, F. C. (2017). Overoptimism about future knowledge: Early arrogance? Journal of Positive Psychology, 12, 3646. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2016.1167939Google Scholar
Lockman, J. J. (2000). A perception-action perspective on tool use development. Child Development, 71, 137144. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00127Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1952). King Solomon’s Rings. New York, NY: Crowell. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1943.tb00655.xGoogle Scholar
Lorenz, K. Z. (1943). Die angeboren Formen moglicher Erfahrung [The innate forms of possible experience]. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 5, 233409. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439–0310.1943.tb00655.xGoogle Scholar
Lucion, M. K., Oliveira, V., Bizarro, L., Bischoff, R., Pelufo Silveria, P., & Kauer-Sant’Anna, M. (2017). Attentional bias toward infant faces: Review of the adaptive and clinical relevance. International Journal of Psychology, 114, 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.01.008Google Scholar
Lukianoff, G., & Haidt, J. (2018). The Coddling of the American Mind: How Good Intentions and Bad Ideas are Setting up a Generation for Failure. New York, NY: Penguin Press.Google Scholar
Lumey, L. H. (1992). Decreased birthweights in infants after maternal in utero exposure to the Dutch famine of 1944–45. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 6, 240253. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365–3016.1992.tb00764.xGoogle Scholar
Luo, L. Z., Li, H., & Lee, K. (2011). Are children´s faces really more appealing than those of adults? Testing the baby schema hypothesis beyond infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 115124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.04.002Google Scholar
Macchi Cassia, V., Turati, C., & Simion, F. (2004). Can a nonspecific bias toward top-heavy patterns explain newborns’ face preference? Psychological Science, 15, 379383. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956–7976.2004.00688.xGoogle Scholar
Machluf, K., & Bjorklund, D. F. (May, 2016). Physical immaturity in infants triggers greater empathy in adults. Paper presented at meeting of the Association of Psychological Science, Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
Manuck, S. B., Craig, A. E., Flory, J. D., Halder, I., & Ferrell, R. E. (2011). Reported early family environment covaries with menarcheal age as a function of polymorphic variation in estrogen receptor-a. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 6983. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000659Google Scholar
Marshall, P. J. (2015). Neuroscience, embodiment, and development. In Overton, W. F. and Molenaar, P. C. M. (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science (Lerner, R. M., Gen. Ed.), Vol. 1. New York, NY: Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy107Google Scholar
Martin, C. L., Ruble, D. N., & Szkrybalo, J. (2002). Cognitive theories of early gender development. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 903933. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033–2909.128.6.903Google Scholar
Maurer, D., & Lewis, T. L. (2013). Sensitive periods in visual development. In Zelazo, P. D. (Ed.), Oxford Handbook of Developmental Psychology, Vol. 1. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 202234. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199958450.013.0008Google Scholar
Maurer, D., Mondloch, C. J., & Lewis, T. L. (2007). Effects of early visual deprivation on perceptual and cognitive development. Progress in Brain Research, 164, 87104. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(07)64005–9Google Scholar
Mayr, E. (1982). The Growth of Biological Thought: Diversity, Evolution, and Inheritance. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Mayr, E., & Provine, W. B. (1980). The Evolutionary Synthesis: Perspectives on the Unification of Biology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. https://doi.org/10.4159/harvard.9780674865389Google Scholar
McAuley, T., & White, D. A. (2011). A latent variables examination of processing speed, response inhibition, and working memory during typical development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 152166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.08.009Google Scholar
McClean, C. Y., Reno, P. L., Pollen, A. A., Bassan, A. I., Capellini, T. D., Guenther, C.,… Kingsley, D. M. (2011). Human-specific loss of regulatory DNA and the evolution of human-specific traits. Nature, 471, 216219. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09774Google Scholar
McGuigan, N., Makinson, J., & Whiten, A. (2011). From over-imitation to super-copying: Adults imitate causally irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 118. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712610X493115CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKinney, M. L., & McNamara, K. (1991). Heterochrony: The Evolution of Ontogeny. New York, NY: Plenum. https://doi.org/10.1007/978–1-4757–0773-1Google Scholar
Meaney, M. J. (2001). Maternal care, gene expression, and the transmission of individual differences in stress reactivity across generations. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 11611192. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.1161Google Scholar
Meaney, M. J. (2010). Epigenetics and the biological definition of gene environment interactions. Child Development, 81, 4179. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–8624.2009.01381.xGoogle Scholar
Meaney, M. J. (2013). Epigenetics and the environmental regulation of the genome and its function. In Narvaez, D, Panksepp, J, Schore, A. N., and Gleason, T. R. (Eds.), Evolution, Early Experience and Human Development: From Research to Practice and Policy. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 99128. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199755059.003.0006Google Scholar
Melas, P. A., Wei, Y., Wong, C. C., Sjöholm, L. K., Åberg, E., Mill, J., … & Lavebratt, C. (2013). Genetic and epigenetic associations of MAOA and NR3C1 with depression and childhood adversities. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 16, 15131528. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1461145713000102Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 7578. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.897687Google Scholar
Merz, E. C., Harlé, K. M., Noble, K. G., & McCall, R. B. (2016). Executive function in previously institutionalized children. Child Development Perspectives, 10, 105110. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12170CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, D. J., Duka, T., Stimpson, C. D., Schapiro, S. J., Baze, W. B., McArthur, M. J., Fobbs, A. J., Sousa, A. M. M., Šestan, N., Wildman, D. E., Lipovich, L., Kuzawa, C. W., Hof, P. R., & Sherwood, C. C. (2012). Prolonged myelination in human neocortical evolution. PNAS, 109, 1648016485. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1117943109Google Scholar
Mills, K. L., Goddings, A. L., Clasen, L. S., Giedd, J. N., & Blakemore, S. J. (2014). The developmental mismatch in structural brain maturation during adolescence. Developmental Neuroscience, 36, 147160. https://doi.org/10.1159/000362328Google Scholar
Mittal, C., Griskevicius, V., Simpson, J. A., Sung, S., & Young, E. S. (2015). Cognitive adaptations to stressful environments: When childhood adversity enhances adult executive function. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109, 604621. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000028Google Scholar
Mondloch, C. J., Lewis, T. M., Budreau, D. R., Maurer, D., Dannemiller, J. L., Benjamin, R., Stephens, B. R., & Kleiner-Gathercoal, K. A. (1999). Face perception during early infancy. Psychological Science, 10, 419422. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467–9280.00179Google Scholar
Montagu, A. (1989). Growing Young (2nd ed.). Grandy, MA: Bergin and Garvey.Google Scholar
Moore, D. D. (2015). The Developing Genome: An Introduction to Behavioral Epigenetics. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Morgan, D. K., & Whitelaw, E. (2008). The case for transgenerational epigenetic inheritance in humans, Mammalian Genome, 19, 394397. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00335-008–9124-yGoogle Scholar
Morss, J. R. (1990). The Biologising of Childhood: Developmental Psychology and the Darwinian Myth. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nagell, K., Olguin, K., & Tomasello, M. (1993). Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 107, 174186. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735–7036.107.2.174Google Scholar
Nagy, E. (2006). From imitation to conversation: The first dialogues with human neonates, Infant and Child Development, 15, 223232. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.460Google Scholar
Nelson, C. A. III, Zeanah, C. H., Fox, N. A., Marshall, P. J., Smuke, A. T., & Guthrie, D. (2007). Cognitive recovery in socially deprived young children: The Bucharest Early Intervention Program. Science, 318, 19371940. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1143921Google Scholar
Nielsen, M. (2012). Imitation, pretend play, and childhood: Essential elements in the evolution of human culture? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 170181. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025168Google Scholar
Nielsen, M. (2018). The social glue of cumulative culture and ritual behavior. Child Development Perspectives, 12, 264268. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12297Google Scholar
Nielsen, M., Haun, D., Kärtner, J., & Legare, C. H. (2017). The persistent sampling bias in developmental psychology: A call to action. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 162, 3138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.04.017Google Scholar
Nielsen, M., Mushin, I., Tomaselli, K., & Whiten, A. (2014). Where culture takes hold: “Overimitation” and its flexible deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen children. Child Development, 85, 21692184. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12265Google Scholar
Nielsen, M., Mushin, B., Tomaselli, K., & Whiten, A. (2016). Imitation, collaboration, and their interaction among Western and indigenous Australian preschool children. Child Development, 87, 755806. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12504Google Scholar
Noonan, J. P. (2003). Regulatory DNAs and the evolution of human development. Current Opinion in Genetics and Development, 19, 557564. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gde.2009.10.002Google Scholar
Nussey, D. H., Postma, E., Gienapp, P., & Visser, M. E. (2005). Selection on heritable phenotypic plasticity in a wild bird population. Science, 310, 304306. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1117004Google Scholar
Obradović, J., Bush, N. R., Stamperdahl, J., Adler, N. E., & Boyce, W. T. (2010). Biological sensitivity to context: The interactive effects of stress reactivity and family adversity on socioemotional behavior and school readiness. Child Development, 81, 270289. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–8624.2009.01394.xGoogle Scholar
Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 466478. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096–3445.130.3.466Google Scholar
Olson, K. R., & Spelke, E. S. (2008). Foundations of cooperation in young children. Cognition, 108, 222231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.003Google Scholar
Oostenbroek, J., Suddendorf, T., Nielsen, M., Redshaw, J., Kennedy-Costantini, S., Davis, J., … & Slaughter, V. (2016). Comprehensive longitudinal study challenges the existence of neonatal imitation in humans. Current Biology, 26, 13341338. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.03.047Google Scholar
Overton, W. F. (2015). Processes, relations, and relational-developmental-systems. In Overton, W. F. and Molenaar, P. C. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science. Vol. 1: Theory and method (7th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley, p. 962. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy102Google Scholar
Painter, R. C. Osmond, C., Gluckman, P. Hanson, M., Philips, D. I. W., & Roseboom, T. J. (2008). Transgenerational effects on prenatal exposure to the Dutch famine on neonatal adiposity and health in later life. BJOG, 115, 12431249. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471–0528.2008.01822.xGoogle Scholar
Parades, S. H., Ridout, K. K., Seifer, R., Armstrong, D. A., Marisit, C., McWilliams, M. A., & Tyrka, A. R. (2016). Methylation of the glucocorticoid receptor gene promoter in preschoolers: Link with internalizing behavior problems. Child Development, 87, 8697. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12484Google Scholar
Pascalis, O., de Haan, M., & Nelson, C. A. (2002). Is face processing species-specific during the first year of life? Science, 296, 13211323. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1070223Google Scholar
Pascalis, O., Scott, L. S., Kelly, D. J., Shannon, R. W., Nicholson, E., Coleman, M., & Nelson, C. A. (2005). Plasticity of face processing in infancy. PNAS, 102, 52975300. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0406627102Google Scholar
Patterson, M. M., & Bigler, R. S. (2006). Preschool children’s attention to environmental messages about groups: Social categorization and the origins of intergroup bias. Child Development, 77, 847860. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–8624.2006.00906.xGoogle Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D. (2013a). Object use in childhood: Development and possible functions. Behaviour, 150, 813843. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-00003086Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D. (2013b). Play. In Zelazo, P (Ed.), Oxford Handbook of Developmental Psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, pp. 276299.Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D., & Galda, L. (1982). The effects of thematic-fantasy play training on the development of children’s story comprehension. American Educational Research Journal, 19, 443452. https://doi.org/10.3102/00028312019003443Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (1998). Physical activity play: The nature and function of neglected aspect of play. Child Development, 69, 577598. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–8624.1998.tb06226.xGoogle Scholar
Periss, V., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2011). Trials and tribulations of childhood: An evolutionary perspective. In Salmon, C and Shackelford, T (Eds.), Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Family Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 149168. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195396690.013.0010Google Scholar
Petanjek, Z., Judaš, M., Šimić, G., Roko Rašin, M., Uylings, H. B. M., Rakic, P., & Kostović, I. (2011). Extraordinary neoteny of synaptic spines in the human prefrontal cortex. PNAS, 108, 1328113286. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1105108108Google Scholar
Plötner, M., Over, H., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2015). The effects of collaboration and minimal-group membership on children’s prosocial behavior, liking, affiliation, and trust. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 139, 161173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.05.008Google Scholar
Portmann, A. (1990). A Zoologist Looks at Humankind. Judith Schaefer (trans.). New York, NY: Columbia University Press. (Originally published in 1944).Google Scholar
Prabhakar, S., Noonan, J. P., Pääbo, S., & Rubin, E. M. (2006). Accelerated evolution of conserved noncoding sequences in humans. Science, 314, 786. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1130738Google Scholar
Quinn, P. C., Yahr, J., Kuhn, A., Slater, A. M., & Pascalis, O. (2002). Representation of the gender of human faces by infants: A preference for female. Perception, 31, 11091121. https://doi.org/10.1068/p3331Google Scholar
Raff, R. A. (1996). The Shape of Life: Genes, Development, and the Evolution of Animal Form. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226256573.001.0001Google Scholar
Rakoczy, H., & Haun, D. (2020). Comparative cognition between. Children and animals. In Hupp, S and Jewell, J (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Child and Adolescent Development. New York, NY: Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119171492.wecad109Google Scholar
Redshaw, J., Nielsen, M., Slaughter, V., Kennedy‐Costantini, S., Oostenbroek, J., Crimston, J., & Suddendorf, T. (2019). Individual differences in neonatal ‘imitation’ fail to predict early social cognitive behavior. Developmental Science, 23, e12892. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12892Google Scholar
Ressler, R. H. (1966). Inherited environmental influences on the operant behavior of mice. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 61, 264267. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0023140Google Scholar
Rheingold, H. (1982). Little children’s participation in the work of adults: A nascent prosocial behavior. Child Development, 53, 114125. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129643Google Scholar
Riedl, K., Jensen, K., Call, J. M., & Tomasello, M. (2012). No third-party punishment in chimpanzees. PNAS, 109, 1482414829. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1203179109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riedl, K., Jensen, K., Call, J. M., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Restorative justice in children. Current Biology, 25, 17311735. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.014Google Scholar
Robbins, E., & Rochat, P. (2011). Emerging signs of strong reciprocity in human ontogeny. Frontiers in Psychology, 353, 114. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00353Google Scholar
Rochat, P. M., Dias, D. G., Guo, L., Broesch, T., Passos-Ferreira, C. Winning, A., & Berg, B. (2009). Fairness in distributive justice by 3- and 5-year-olds across seven cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40, 416442. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022109332844Google Scholar
Romens, S. E., McDonald, J., Svaren, J., & Pollak, S. D. (2015). Associations between early Life stress and gene methylation in children. Child Development, 86, 303309. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12270Google Scholar
Ruff, H. A., & Birch, H. G. (1974). Infant visual fixation: The effect of concentricity, curvilinearity, and number of directions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 17, 460473. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022–0965(74)90056–3Google Scholar
Ruiz, A. M., & Santos, L. R. (2013). Understanding differences in the way human and non-human primates represent tools: The role of teleological-intentional information. In Sanz, C. M., Call, J, and Boesch, C (Eds.), Tool Use in Animals: Cognition and Ecology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 119133. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511894800.008Google Scholar
Rutter, M., Beckett, C. Castle, J., Colvert, E., Kreppner, J., Mehta, M. et al. (2007). Effects of profound early institutional deprivation: An overview of findings from a UK longitudinal study of Romanian adoptee. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 332350. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405620701401846Google Scholar
Salapatek, P., & Kessen, W. (1966). Visual scanning of triangles by the human newborn. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 3, 155167. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022–0965(66)90090–7Google Scholar
Samek, A., Cowell, J. M., Cappelen, A. W., Cheng, Y., Contreras-Ibáñez, C., Gomez-Sicard, N., Gonzalez-Gadea, M. L., Huepe, D., Ibáñez, A., Lee, K., Malcolm-Smith, S., Salas, N., Selcuk, B., Tungodden, B., Wong, A., Zhou, X., & Decety, J. (2020). The development of social comparisons and sharing behavior across 12 countries. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 192, 104778. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104778Google Scholar
Sanefuji, W., Wada, K., Yamamoto, T., Mohri, I., & Taniike, M. (2014). Development of preference for conspecific faces in human infants. Developmental Psychology, 50, 979985. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035205Google Scholar
Schmidt, M. F., Rakoczy, H., Mietzsch, T., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Young children understand and defend the role of agreement in establishing arbitrary norms – but unanimity is key. Child Development, 87, 612626. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12510Google Scholar
Schneider, W. (1985). Developmental trends in the metamemory-memory behavior relationship: An integrative review. In Forrest-Pressley, D. L., MacKinnon, G. E., and Waller, T. G. (Eds.), Cognition, Metacognition, and Human Performance, Vol. 1. New York, NY: Academic Press, pp. 57109.Google Scholar
Schneider, W. (1998). Performance prediction in young children: Effects of skill, metacognition and wishful thinking. Developmental Science, 1, 291297. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467–7687.00044Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (1991). Learned Optimism: How to Change Your Mind and your Life (1st ed.). New York, NY: Free Press.Google Scholar
Senese, V. P., De Falco, S., Bornstein, M. H., Caria, A., Buffolino, S., & Venutti, P. (2013). Human infant faces provoke implicit positive affective responses in parents and non-parents alike. PLoS One, 8, e80379. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080379Google Scholar
Shepperd, J. A., Klein, W. M. P., Waters, E. A., & Weinstein, N. D. (2013). Taking stock of unrealistic optimism. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 395411. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691613485247Google Scholar
Shin, H.-E., Bjorklund, D. F., & Beck, E. F. (2007). The adaptive nature of children’s overestimation in a strategic memory task. Cognitive Development, 22, 197212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.10.001Google Scholar
Shutts, K., Kinzler, K. D., McKee, C., & Spelke, E. (2009). Social information guides infants’ selection of foods. Journal of Cognition and Development, 10, 117. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248370902966636Google Scholar
Simpson, E. A., Miller, G. M., Ferrari, P. F., Suomi, S. J., & Paukner, A. (2016). Neonatal imitation and early social experience predict gaze following abilities in infant monkeys. Scientific Reports, 6, 20233. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep20233Google Scholar
Simpson, J. A., Griskevicius, V., Kuo, S., Sung, S., & Collins, W. A. (2012). Evolution, stress, and sensitive periods: The influence of unpredictability in early versus late childhood on sex and risky behavior. Developmental Psychology, 48, 674686. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027293Google Scholar
Skulachev, V. P., Holtze, S., Vyssokikh, M. Y., Bakeeva, L. E., Skulachev, M. V., Markov, A. V., Hildebrandt, T. B., & Sadovnichii, V. A. (2017). Neoteny, prolongation of youth: From naked mole rats to “naked ape” (humans). Physiological Review, 97, 699720. https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00040.2015Google Scholar
Slagt, M., Dubas, J. S., Deković, M., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2016). Differences in sensitivity to parenting depending on child temperament: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 10681110. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000061Google Scholar
Smilansky, S. (1968). The Effects of Sociodramatic Play on Disadvantaged Preschool Children. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Smith, P. K. (1982). Does play matter? Functional and evolutionary aspects of animal and human play. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5, 139184. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0001092XGoogle Scholar
Somel, M., Rohlfs, R., & Liu, X. (2014). Transcriptomic insights into human brain evolution: acceleration, neutrality, heterochrony. Genetics & Development, 29, 110119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gde.2014.09.001Google Scholar
Spear, L. P. (2000). Neurobehavioral changes in adolescence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 111114. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467–8721.00072Google Scholar
Spear, L. P. (2007). Brain development and adolescent behavior. In Coch, D, Fischer, K. W., and Dawson, G (Eds.), Human Behavior, Learning, and the Developing Brain: Typical Development. New York, NY: Guilford, pp. 362396.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S., & Kinzler, K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10, 8996. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–7687.2007.00569.xGoogle Scholar
Stearns, S. (1992). The Evolution of Life Histories. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stengelin, R., Hepach, R., & Haun, D. (2020a). Being observed increases overimitation in three diverse cultures. Developmental Psychology, 55, 26302636. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000832Google Scholar
Stengelin, R., Hepach, R., & Haun, D. (2020b). Cross-cultural variation in how much, but not whether, children overimitate. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 193, 104796. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104796Google Scholar
Stenhouse, D. (1974). The Evolution of Intelligence: A General Theory and Some of its Implications. London: Allen and Unwin.Google Scholar
Stipek, D. (1981). Children’s perceptions of their own and their classmates’ ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 73, 404410. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022–0663.73.3.404Google Scholar
Stipek, D. (1984). Young children’s performance expectations: Logical analysis or wishful thinking? In Nicholls, J. G. (Ed.), Advances in Motivation and Achievement: Vol. 3. The Development of Achievement Motivation. Greenwich, CT: JAI, pp. 107136.Google Scholar
Stright, A. D., Gallagher, K. C., & Kelley, K. (2008). Infant temperament moderates relations between maternal parenting in early childhood and children’s adjustment in first grade. Child Development, 79, 186200. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–8624.2007.01119.xGoogle Scholar
Sumner, J. A., McLaughlin, K. A., Walsh, K., Sheridan, M. A., & Koenen, K. C. (2015). Caregiving and 5-HTTLPR genotype predict adolescent physiological stress reactivity: Confirmatory tests of gene x environment interactions. Child Development, 86, 985994. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12357Google Scholar
Szepsenwol, O., Griskevicius, V., Simpson, J. A., Young, E. S., Fleck, C., & Jones, R. E. (2018). The effect of predictable early childhood environments on sociosexuality in early adulthood. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 11, 131145. https://doi.org/10.1037/ebs0000082CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H., Billig, M. G., Bundy, R. P., & Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization and intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 149178. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420010202Google Scholar
Tasimi, A., & Wynn, K. (2016). Costly rejection of wrongdoers by infants and children. Cognition, 151, 7679. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.03.004Google Scholar
Tobi, E. W., Slieker, R. C., Stein, A. D., Suchiman, H. E. D., Slagboo, P. E., van Zwet, E. W., Heijmans, B. T., & Lumey, L. H. (2015). Early gestation as the critical time-window for changes in the prenatal environment to affect the adult human blood methylome. International Journal of Epidemiology, 44, 12111223. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyv043Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2019). Becoming Human: A Theory of Ontogeny. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press. https://doi.org/10.4159/9780674988651Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2005). The emergence of social cognition in three young chimpanzees. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 70(1), vii132. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.2005.00324.xGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Savage-Rumbaugh, S., & Kruger, A. C. (1993). Imitative learning of actions on objects by children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees. Child Development, 64, 16881705. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131463Google Scholar
Tomonaga, M., Tankak, M., Matsuzawa, T., Myowa-Yamakoshi, M., Kosugi, D., Mizuno, Y., Okamoto, S., Yamaguchi, M. K., & Bard, K. (2004). Development of social cognition in infant chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Face recognition, smiling, gaze, and the lack of triadic interactions. Japanese Psychological Research, 46, 227235. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468–5584.2004.00254.xGoogle Scholar
Tottenham, N., Hare, T. A., Quinn, B. T., McCarry, T. W., Nurse, M., Gilhooly, T., Galvan, A., Davidson, M. C,… & Casey, B. J. (2010). Prolonged institutional rearing is associated with atypically large amygdala volume and difficulties in emotional regulation. Developmental Science, 13, 4661. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–7687.2009.00852.xGoogle Scholar
Tremblay, R. E., & Szyf, M. (2010). Developmental origins of chronic physical aggression and epigenetics. Epigenomics, 2, 495499. https://doi.org/10.2217/epi.10.40Google Scholar
Trevathan, W. R., & Rosenberg, K. R. (2016). Human evolution and the helpless infant. In Trevathan, W. R. and Rosenberg, K. R. (Eds.), Costly and Cute: Helpless Infants and Human Evolution. Santee Fe, MN: School for advanced Research Press, pp. 128.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In Campbell, B (Ed.), Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man. New York, NY: Aldine de Gruyter, pp. 136179. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315129266–7Google Scholar
Trut, L. (1999). Early canid domestication: The Farm-Fox Experiment. American Scientist, 87, 160. https://doi.org/10.1511/1999.2.160Google Scholar
Trut, L., Oskina, I., & Kharlamova, A. (2009). Animal evolution during domestication: The domesticated fox as a model. BioEssays, 31, 349360. https://doi.org/10.1002/bies.200800070Google Scholar
Ulber, J., Hamann, K., & Tomasello, M. (2015). How 18- and 24-month-old peers divide resources among themselves. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140, 228244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.009Google Scholar
Ulber, J., Hamann, K., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Extrinsic rewards diminish costly sharing in 3-year-olds. Child Development, 87, 11921203. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12534Google Scholar
Vaish, A., Hepach, R., & Tomasello, M. (2018). The specificity of reciprocity: Young children reciprocate more generously to those who intentionally benefit them. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 167, 336353. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.11.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Ebstein, R. P. (2011). Methylation matters in child development: Toward developmental behavioral epigenetics. Child Development Perspectives, 5, 305310. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750–8606.2011.00202.xGoogle Scholar
Volk, A. A., & Atkinson, J. A. (2013). Infant and child death in the human environment of evolutionary adaptation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 182192. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.11.007Google Scholar
Waddington, C. H. (1975). The Evolution of an Evolutionist. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Waller, K. L., Volk, A., & Quinsey, V. L. (2004). The effect of infant fetal alcohol syndrome facial features on adoption preferences. Human Nature, 15, 101117. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-004–1006-8Google Scholar
Warneken, F. (2015). Precocious prosociality: Why do young children help? Child Development Perspectives, 9, 16. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12101Google Scholar
Warneken, F., Chen, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Cooperative activities in young children and chimpanzees. Child Development, 77, 640663. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–8624.2006.00895.xGoogle Scholar
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311, 13011303. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1121448Google Scholar
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Extrinsic rewards undermine altruistic tendencies in 20-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 44, 17851788. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013860Google Scholar
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Parental presence and encouragement do not influence helping in young children. Infancy, 18, 345368. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532–7078.2012.00120.xGoogle Scholar
Washburn, S. (1960). Tools and human evolution. Scientific American, 203, 315. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0960-62Google Scholar
West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003). Developmental Plasticity and Evolution. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. (2019). Conformity and over-imitation: An integrative review of variant forms of hyper-reliance on social learning. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 51, 3175.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., & Erdal, D. (2014). The human socio-cognitive niche and its evolutionary origins. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 367, 21192129. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0114Google Scholar
Wilson, D. S., Kauffman, R. A., & Purdy, M. S. (2011). A program for at-risk high school students informed by evolutionary science. PLoS One, 6(11), e27826. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027826Google Scholar
Witelson, S. F. (1987). Neurobiological aspects of language in children. Child Development, 58, 653688. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130205Google Scholar
Witherington, D. C., & Lickliter, R. (2016). Integrating development and evolution in psychological science: Evolutionary developmental psychology, developmental systems, and explanatory pluralism. Human Development, 59, 200234. https://doi.org/10.1159/000450715Google Scholar
Wobber, V., Herrmann, E., Hare, B., Wrangham, R., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Differences in the early cognitive development of children and great apes. Developmental Psychobiology, 56, 547573. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21125Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. (2019). Goodness Paradox: The Strange Relationship Between Virtue and Violence in Human Evolution. New York, NY: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Wray, G. A. (2007). The evolutionary significance of cis-regulatory mutations. Nature Review Genetics, 8, 206216. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg2063Google Scholar
Yang, F., Choi, Y-J., Misch, A., Yang, X., & Dunham, Y. (2018). In defense of the commons: Young children negatively evaluate and sanction free riders. Psychological Science, 29, 15981611. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797618779061Google Scholar
Yeager, D. S., Dahl, R. E., & Dweck, C. S. (2018). Why interventions to influence adolescent behavior often fail but could succeed. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13, 101122. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617722620Google Scholar
Yeats, K. O., & Taylor, G. H. (2005). Neurobehavioral outcomes of mild head injury in children and adolescents. Pediatric Rehabilitation, 8, 516. https://doi.org/10.1080/13638490400011199Google Scholar
Zell, E., Strickhouser, J. E., Sedikides, C., & Alicke, M. D. (2020). The better-than-average effect in comparative self-evaluation: A comprehensive review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146, 118149. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000218Google Scholar

Save element to Kindle

To save this element to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Child Development in Evolutionary Perspective
Available formats
×

Save element to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Child Development in Evolutionary Perspective
Available formats
×

Save element to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Child Development in Evolutionary Perspective
Available formats
×