Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 3
  • Print publication year: 2002
  • Online publication date: December 2009

7 - Kuhn, Conceptual Change, and Cognitive Science

Summary

INTRODUCTION

The research project outlined in Thomas Kuhn's The Structure of Scientific Revolutions seems intrinsically historical, philosophical, and psychological (Kuhn 1970). However, by and large, Kuhn never utilized research in the cognitive sciences that would have furthered his own paradigm in ways I think he would have found agreeable. Until his very last writings, psychology dropped out of Kuhn's post-Structure published articulations of his views just at the time that the cognitive revolution was beginning to provide accounts of representation, problem solving, and learning that I believe are pertinent to his intuitive insights. With hindsight one can construct significant parallels between the views of knowledge, perception, and learning developed in each. In what follows I will discuss in what ways some of Kuhn's insights might be furthered today in light of cognitive science research. Seen through a cognitive lens, Kuhn's little book seems all the more remarkable and insightful. Many of the issues with which he grappled have been the subject of entire areas of research in cognitive science, especially cognitive psychology. In the course of this essay I can only give brief indications of how Kuhn's thinking and research in areas of cognitive science have been running along parallel lines and of how one might, through cognitive-historical analysis, create some intersecting lines.

In his Presidential Address to the Philosophy of Science Association, Kuhn expressed his abiding interest as being in “incommensurability and the nature of the conceptual divide between the developmental stages separated by … ‘scientific revolutions’” (Conant and Haugeland 2000, p. 228).

References
Andersen, H., Chen, X., and Barker, P.. 1996. “Kuhn's Mature Philosophy of Science and Cognitive Psychology.” Philosophical Psychology 9: 347–63
Andersen, H., and Nersessian, N. J.. 2000. “Nomic Concepts, Frames, and Conceptual Change.” Philosophy of Science 67 (special Proceedings issue) S224–41
Armstrong, S. L., Gleitman, L., and Gleitman, H.. 1983. “What Some Concepts Might Not Be.” Cognition 13: 263–308
Barsalou, L. 1987. “The Instability of Graded Structure: Implications for the Nature of Concepts.” In: U. Neisser, ed. Concepts and Conceptual Development: Ecological and Intellectual Factors in Categorization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 101–40
Barsalou, L. 1992. “Frames, Concepts, and Conceptual Fields.” In: A. Lehrer and E. Kittay, eds. Frames, Fields, and Contrasts: New Essays in Semantical and Lexical Organization. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 325–40
Brown, J. S., Collins, A., and Duguid, P.. 1989. “Situated Cognition and the Culture of Learning.” Educational Researcher 18: 32–42
Carey, S. 1985. Conceptual Change in Childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Carey, S. 1991. “Knowledge Acquisition: Enrichment or Conceptual Change?” In: S. Carey and R. Gelman, eds. The Epigenesis of Mind: Essays on Biology and Cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 257–91
Carey, S., and Xu, Fei. 2001. “Infants' Knowledge of Objects: Beyond Files and Object Tracking.” Cognition 80: 179–213
Carey, S., and Smith, C.. 1993. “On Understanding the Nature of Scientific Knowledge.” Educational Psychologist 28: 235–51
Cartwright, N. 1983. How the Laws of Physics Lie. Oxford: Clarendon Press
Champagne, A. B., Klopfer, L. E., and Gunstone, R. F.. 1982. “Cognitive Research and the Design of Science Instruction.” Educational Psychologist 17: 31–53
Chen, X., Andersen, H., and Barker, P.. 1998. “Kuhn's Theory of Scientific Revolutions and Cognitive Psychology.” Philosophical Psychology 11: 5–28
Chi, M. T. H. 1992. “Conceptual Change Within and Across Ontological Categories; Examples from Learning and Discovery in Science.” In: R. Giere, ed. Cognitive Models of Science. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, pp. 129–86
Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P. J., and Glaser, R.. 1981. “Categorization and Representation of Physics Problems by Experts and Novices.” Cognitive Science 5: 121–52
Clement, J. 1982. “Students' Preconceptions in Elementary Mechanics.” AJP 50: 66–71
Clement, J. 1983. “A Conceptual Model Discussed by Galileo and Used Intuitively by Physics Students.” In: D. Gentner and A. Stevens, ed. In Mental Models, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 325–40
Clement, J. 1989. “Learning Via Model Construction and Criticism.” In: G. Glover, R. Ronning and C. Reynolds, eds. Handbook of Creativity: Assessment, Theory, and Research. New York: Plenum, pp. 341–81
Collins, A., J. S. Brown, and P. Duguid. 1989. “Cognitive Apprenticeship: Teaching the Crafts of Reading, Writing, and Mathematics.” In: L. B. Resnick, ed. Knowing, Learning, and Instruction. Hillsdale: NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 453–94
Conant, James, and John Haugeland, eds. 2000. The Road Since Structure: Thomas S. Kuhn. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Darden, L. 1980. “Theory Construction in Genetics.” In: T. Nickles, ed. Scientific Discovery: Case Studies. Dordrecht: Reidel, pp. 151–70
Darden, L. 1991. Theory Change in Science: Strategies from Mendelian Genetics. New York: Oxford University
DeKleer, J., and J. S. Brown. 1983. “Assumptions and Ambiguities in Mechanistic Mental Models.” In: D. G. A. Stevens, ed. Mental Models. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 155–90
Driver, R., and Easley, J.. 1978. “Pupils and Paradigms: A Review of Literature Related to Concept Development in Adolescent Science Students.” Studies in Science Education 5: 61–84
Dunbar, K. 1995. “How Scientists Really Reason: Scientific Reasoning in Real-World Laboratories.” In: R. J. Sternberg and J. E. Davidson, eds. The Nature of Insight. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 365–95
Dunbar, K. 1999. “How Scientists Build Models: In Vivo Science as a Window on the Scientific Mind.” In: L. Magnani, N. J. Nersessian and P. Thagard, eds. Model-Based Reasoning in Scientific Discovery. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum, pp. 85–99
Gentner, D., and D. Gentner. 1983. “Flowing Waters and Teeming Crowds: Mental Models of Electricity.” In Gentner and Stevens, eds. (1983), pp. 99–129
Gentner, D., and Markman, A. B.. 1997. “Structure Mapping in Analogy and Similarity.” American Psychologist 52(1): 45–56
Gentner, D., and A. L. Stevens. 1983. Mental Models. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Giere, R. N. 1988. Explaining Science: A Cognitive Approach. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Giere, R. N. ed., 1992. Cognitive Models of Science. Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science, vol. 15. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press
Giere, R. N., 1994. “The Cognitive Structure of Scientific Theories.” Philosophy of Science 61: 276–96
Gooding, D. 1981. “Final Steps to the Field Theory: Faraday's Study of Electromagnetic Phenomena, 1845–1850.” Historical Studies in the Physical Sciences 11: 231–75
Gooding, D. 1990. Experiment and the Making of Meaning: Human Agency in Scientific Observation and Experiment. Dordrecht: Kluwer
Gooding, D. 1992. “The Procedural Turn: Or Why Did Faraday's Thought Experiments Work?” In Giere (1992), pp. 45–76
Gopnik, Alison, and Andrew N. Meltzoff. 1997. Words, Thought, and Theories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Griesemer, J. R. 1991a. “Material Models in Biology.” In: PSA 1990. East Lansing, MI: PSA
Griesemer, J. R., 1991b. “Must Scientific Diagrams Be Eliminable? The Case of Path Analysis.” Biology and Philosophy 6: 177–202
Griesemer, J. R., and W. Wimsatt. 1989. “Picturing Weismannism: A Case Study of Conceptual Evolution.” In: M. Ruse, ed. What the Philosophy of Biology Is: Essays for David Hull. Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 75–137
Halloun, I. A., and Hestenes, D.. 1985. “The Initial Knowledge State of College Physics Students.” AJP 53: 1043–55
Hegarty, M. 1992. “Mental Animation: Inferring Motion from Static Diagrams of Mechanical Systems.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 18(5): 1084–1102
Hegarty, M., and Sims, V. K.. 1994. “Individual Differences in Mental Animation from Text and Diagrams.” Journal of Memory and Language 32: 411–30
Hegarty, M., and K. Steinhoff. 1994. “Use of Diagrams as External Memory in a Mechanical Reasoning Task.” Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans
Heider, E. Rosch. 1972. “Universals in Color Naming and Memory.” Journal of Experimental Psychology 93: 10–20 (See also Rosch, E.)
Holland, J. H., K. J. Holyoak, R. E. Nisbett, and P. R. Thagard. 1986. Induction: Processes of Inference, Learning, and Discovery. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Holmes, F. L. 1981. “The Fine Structure of Scientific Creativity.” History of Science 19: 60–70
Holmes, F. L. 1985. Lavoisier and the Chemistry of Life: An Exploration of Scientific Creativity. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press
Hoyningen-Huene, P. 1993. Reconstructing Scientific Revolutions: Thomas S. Kuhn's Philosophy of Science. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Jiminez, Gomez E., and Fernendaz, E. Duran. 1998. “Didactic Problems in the Concept of Electrical Potential Difference and an Analysis of Philogenesis.” Science and Education 7: 129–41
Johnson-Laird, P. N. 1983. Mental Models. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Johnson-Laird, P. N. 1989. “Mental Models.” In: M. Posner, ed. Foundations of Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 469–500
Keil, F. C. 1991. “The Emergence of Theoretical Beliefs as Constraints on Concepts.” In: S. Carey and R. Gelman, eds. The Epigenesis of Mind: Essays on Biology and Cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 237–56
Kosslyn, S. M. 1980. Image and Mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Kosslyn, S. M. 1994. Image and Brain. Cambridge MA: MIT Press
Kuhn, T. S. 1970. The Structure of Scientific Revolutions, 2nd ed. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Kuhn, T. S. 1977. The Essential Tension: Selected Studies in Scientific Tradition and Change. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Kuhn, T. S. 1979. “Metaphor in Science.” In: A. Ortony, ed. Metaphor and Thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 409–19
Kuhn, T. S., 1990. “The Road Since Structure. PSA 1990 2: 3–13
Kuhn, T. S. 1992. “The Trouble with the Historical Philosophy of Science.” Robert and Maurine Rothschild Lecture, Harvard University
Kuhn, T. S. 1993. “Afterwords.” In: P. Horwich, ed. World Changes: Thomas Kuhn and the Nature of Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 311–41
Latour, B. 1986. “Visualisation and Cognition: Thinking with Eyes and Hands.” Knowledge and Society 6: 1–40
Latour, B., and S. Woolgar. 1986. Laboratory Life: The Construction of Scientific Facts. Princeton: Princeton University Press
Lynch, M., and S. Woolgar, eds. 1990. Representation in Scientific Practice. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Magnani, L., N. J. Nersessian, and P. Thagard, eds. 1999. Model-Based Reasoning in Scientific Discovery. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum
McCloskey, M. 1983. “Naive Theories of Motion.” In: D. Gentner and A. L. Stevens, eds. Mental Models. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 229–33
McDermott, L. C. 1984. “Research on Conceptual Understanding in Mechanics.” Physics Today July: 24
Medin, D. L. 1989. “Concepts and Conceptual Structure.” American Psychologist 44: 1469–81
Morgan, M. S., and M. Morrison, eds. 1999. Models as Mediators. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Morrow, D. G., Bower, G. H., and Greenspan, S. L.. 1989. “Updating Situation Models During Narrative Comprehension.” Journal of Memory and Language 28: 292–312
Nersessian, N. J. 1984a. “Aether/Or: The Creation of Scientific Concepts.” Studies in the History and Philosophy of Science 15: 175–212
Nersessian, N. J., 1984b. Faraday to Einstein: Constructing Meaning in Scientific Theories. Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff/Kluwer Academic
Nersessian, N. J. 1985. “Faraday's Field Concept.” In: D. C. Gooding and F. A. J. L. James, eds. Faraday Rediscovered: Essays on the Life and Work of Michael Faraday. London: Macmillan, pp. 377–406
Nersessian, N. J. 1988. “Reasoning from Imagery and Analogy in Scientific Concept Formation.” In: A. Fine and J. Leplin, eds. PSA 1988. East Lansing, MI: Philosophy of Science Association, pp. 41–8
Nersessian, N. J., 1989. “Conceptual Change in Science and in Science Education.” Synthese 80 (Special Issue: Philosophy of Science and Science Education): 163–84
Nersessian, N. J. 1992a. “Constructing and Instructing: The Role of ‘Abstraction Techniques’ in Developing and Teaching Scientific Theories.” In: R. Duschl and R. Hamilton, eds. Philosophy of Science, Cognitive Science, and Educational Theory and Practice. Albany: SUNY Press, pp. 48–68
Nersessian, N. J. 1992b. “How Do Scientists Think? Capturing the Dynamics of Conceptual Change in Science.” In: Giere (1992), pp. 3–44
Nersessian, N. J. 1992c. “In the Theoretician's Laboratory: Thought Experimenting as Mental Modeling.” In: D. Hull, M. Forbes, and K. Okruhlik, eds. PSA 1992. East Lansing, MI: PSA, pp. 291–301
Nersessian, N. J. 1995a. “The Cognitive Sciences and the History of Science.” Paper presented at the Conference on Critical Problems and Research Frontiers in History of Science and Technology, Madison, WI
Nersessian, N. J. 1995b. “Opening the Black Box: Cognitive Science and the History of Science. Osiris 10 (Constructing Knowledge in the History of Science, A. Thackray, ed.): 194–211
Nersessian, N. J. 1995c. Should Physicists Preach What They Practice? Constructive Modeling in Doing and Learning Physics. “Science & Education 4: 203–26
Nersessian, N. J. in press-a. “Abstraction Via Generic Modeling in Concept Formation in Science.” Mind and Society. In: M. R. Jones and N. Cartwright, eds
Nersessian, N. J. in press-b. “Maxwell and the ‘Method of Physical Analogy.’” In: D. Malament, ed. Essays in the History and Philosophy of Science and Mathematics to Honor Howard Stein on his 70th Birthday. LaSalle, IL: Open Court
Nersessian, N. J., and L. B. Resnick. 1989. “Comparing Historical and Intuitive Explanations of Motion: Does ‘Naive Physics’ Have a Structure?” Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Ann Arbor, MI
Nickles, T. 1998. “Kuhn, Historical Philosophy of Science, and Case-Based Reasoning.” Configurations 6: 51–86
Perrig, W., and Kintsch, W.. 1985. “Propositional and Situational Representations of Text.” Journal of Memory and Language 24: 503–18
Pylyshyn, Z. 2001. Visual indexes, preconceptual objects and situated vision. Cognition 80: 127–58
Rosch, E. 1987. “Wittgenstein and Categorization Research in Cognitive Psychology.” In: M. Chapman and R. A. Dixon, eds. Meaning and the Growth of Understanding: Wittgenstein's Significance for Developmental Psychology. Berlin: Springer, pp. 151–66. (See also Heider, E.)
Rosch, E., and Mervis, C. B.. 1975. “Family Resemblance Studies in the Internal Structure of Categories.” Cognitive Psychology 7: 573–605
Rudwick, M. J. S. 1976. “The Emergence of a Visual Language for Geological Science.” History of Science 14: 149–95
Schwartz, D. L., and Black, J. B.. 1996. “Analog Imagery in Mental Model Reasoning: Depictive Models.” Cognitive Psychology 30: 154–219
Shelley, C. 1996. “Visual Abductive Reasoning in Archeology.” Philosophy of Science 63: 278–301
Shepard, R. N., and L. A. Cooper. 1982. Mental Images and Their Transformations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Smith, C., Snir, J., and Grosslight, L.. 1992. “Using Conceptual Models to Facilitate Conceptual Change: The Case of Weight–Density Differentiation.” Cognition and Instruction 9: 221–83
Smith, E. E., and D. L. Medin. 1981. Concepts and Categories. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Spelke, E. S., Phillips, A., and Woodward, A. L.. 1995. “Spatio-Temporal Continuity, Smoothness of Motion and Object Identity in Infancy.” British Journal of Developmental Psychology 13: 113–42
Thagard, P. 1991. “Conceptual Revolutions. Princeton: Princeton University Press
Trumpler, M. 1997. Converging Images: Techniques of Intervention and Forms of Representation of Sodium-Channel Proteins in Nerve Cell Membranes. Journal of the History of Biology 20: 55–89
Tweney, R. D. 1985. “Faraday's Discovery of Induction: A Cognitive Approach.” In: D. Gooding and F. A. J. L. James, eds. Faraday Rediscovered. New York: Stockton Press, pp. 189–201
Tweney, R. D. 1987. “What Is Scientific Thinking?” Unpublished manuscript
Tweney, R. D., 1992. “Stopping Time: Faraday and the Scientific Creation of Perceptual Order.” Physis 29: 149–64
Viennot, L. 1979. “Spontaneous Reasoning in Elementary Dynamics.” European Journal of Science Education 1: 205–21
Wiggins, D. 1980. Sameness and Substance. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Wiser, M. 1995. “Use of History of Science to Understand and Remedy Students' Misconceptions about Time and Temperature.” In: D. Perkins, ed. Software Goes to School. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 23–38
Wittgenstein, L. 1968. Philosophical Investigations. Translated by G. E. M. Anscombe. New York: Macmillan
Woods, D. D. 1995. “Towards a Theoretical Base for Representation Design in the Computer Medium: Ecological Perception and Aiding Human Cognition.” In: J. Flach, P. Hanosok, J. Caird, and K. Vicente, eds., Global Perspectives on the Ecology of Human–Machine Systems. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 157–88