Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 8
  • Print publication year: 2003
  • Online publication date: December 2009

22 - Self-consciousness: an integrative approach from philosophy, psychopathology and the neurosciences

from Part III - Disturbances of the self: the case of schizophrenia

Summary

Abstract

In this chapter, we want to try and integrate the divergent lines introduced in the other parts of this book. We propose a model of self-consciousness derived from phenomenology, philosophy, the cognitive and neurosciences. We will then give an overview of research data on self-processing from various fields and link it to our model. Some aspects of the disturbances of the self in pathological states such as brain lesions and schizophrenia will be discussed. Finally, the clinically important concept of insight into a disease and its neurocognitive origin will be introduced. We argue that self-consciousness is a valid construct and, as shown in this chapter, it is possible that it can be studied with the instruments of cognitive neuroscience.

Introduction

The self as an entity distinct from the other has entered western thought through Greek philosophy (see chapter 1, this volume, for details). Throughout history, a myriad of different notions, starting from theology, philosophy, psychoanalysis, to early psychological concepts, psychopathology, the social sciences, and, more recently, cognitive psychology, neurology and the neurosciences have been developed. With the advent of scientific interest in consciousness towards the end of the twentieth century, self-consciousness has also become a topic taken up by the neuroscientific community. As a first phenomenological approximation based on commonly shared experience, we know that we are the same person across time, that we are the author of our thoughts/actions, and that we are distinct from the environment.

REFERENCES
Allport, G. W. (1955). Becoming. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press
Amador, X. F. & David, A. S. (1997). Insight and Psychosis. New York: Oxford University Press
Amador, X. F., Strauss, D. H., Yale, S. A. et al. (1993). Assessment of insight in psychosis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 873–9
Anderson, J. R. & Reder, L. M. (1979). An elaborative processing explanation of depth of processing. In Levels of Processing in Human Memory, ed. L. S. Cermak & F. I. M. Craik, pp. 385–403. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Armstrong, D. (1980). The Nature of Mind and Other Essays. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press
Bentall, R. P., Kinderman, P. & Kaney, S. (1994). The self, attributional processes and abnormal beliefs: towards a model of persecutory delusions. Behavioral Research and Therapy, 32, 331–41
Bermudez, J. L., Marcel, A. & Eilan, N. (1995). The Body and the Self. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Berze, J. (1914). Die primäre Insuffizienz der psychischen Aktivität. Leipzig: Franz Deuticke
Blakemore, S. J., Goodbody, S. J. & Wolpert, D. M. (1998a). Predicting the consequences of our own actions: the role of sensorimotor context estimation. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 7511–18
Blakemore, S. J., Wolpert, D. M. & Frith, C. D. (1998b). Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation. Nature and Neuroscience, 1, 635–40
Bleuler, E. (1916). Lehrbuch der Psychiatrie. (Reprinted 1983.) Berlin: Springer
Block, N., Flanagan, O. & Güzeldere, G. (1997). The Nature of Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Chung, K. F., Chen, E. Y. H., Lam, L. C. W., Chen, R. Y. L. & Chan, C. K. Y. (1997). How are psychotic symptoms perceived? A comparison between patients, relatives and the general public. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 31, 756–61
Churchland, P. (1995). The Engine of Reason, the Seat of the Soul: A Philosophical Journey into the Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Collins, A. A., Remington, G. J., Coulter, K. & Birkett, K. (1997). Insight, neurocognitive function and symptom clusters in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 27, 37–44
Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human Nature and the Social Order. New York: Scribner
Craik, F. I. M., Moroz, T. M., Moscovitch, M. et al. (1999). In search of the self: a positron emission tomography study. Psychological Science, 10, 26–34
Cuesta, M. J. & Peralta, V. (1994). Lack of insight in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 20, 359–66
Cuesta, M. J., Peralta, V., Caro, F. & Leon, J. (1995). Is poor insight in psychotic disorders associated with poor performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test? American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1380–2
Daprati, E., Franck, N., Georgieff, N. et al. (1997). Looking for the agent: an investigation into consciousness of action and self-consciousness in schizophrenic patients. Cognition, 65, 71–86
David, A. S. (1990). Insight and psychosis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 798–808
David, A., Os, J., Jones, P. et al. (1995). Insight and psychotic illness. Cross-sectional and longitudinal associations. British Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 621–8
David, A., Kemp, R., Smith, L. & Fahy, T. (1996). Multiple personality and schizophrenia. In Methods in Madness: Case Studies in Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, ed. P. Halligan & J. Marshall, pp. 123–46. East Sussex: Lawrence Erlbaum
Dehaene, S. & Naccache, L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition, 79, 1–37
Dennett, D. (1991). Consciousness Explained. Boston: Little, Brown
Dickerson, F. B., Boronow, J. J., Ringel, N. & Parente, F. (1997). Lack of insight among outpatients with schizophrenia. Psychiatric Services, 48, 195–9
Eysenck, M. W. & Eysenck, M. C. (1979). Processing depth, elaboration of encoding, memory stores, and expended processing capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 472–84
Feinberg, I. (1978). Efference copy and corollary discharge: implications for thinking and its disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 4, 636–40
Ferguson, T. J., Rule, G. R. & Carlson, D. (1983). Memory for personally relevant information. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 44, 251–61
Fletcher, P. C., Frith, C. D., Grasby, P. M. et al. (1995). Brain systems for encoding and retrieval of auditory-verbal memory. An in vivo study in humans. Brain, 118, 401–16
Fodor, J. (1983). The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Bradford Books
Fourneret, P. & Jeannerod, M. (1998). Limited conscious monitoring of motor performance in normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 36, 1133–40
Franck, N., Farrer, C., Georgieff, N. et al. (2001). Defective recognition of one's own actions in patients with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 454–9
Frith, C. D. (1992). The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum
Frith, C. D. & Done, D. J. (1989). Experiences of alien control in schizophrenia reflect a disorder in the central monitoring of action. Psychological Medicine, 19, 359–63
Gallois, P., Ovelacq, E., Hautecoeur, P. & Dereux, J. F. (1988). Disconnexion et reconnaissance des visages. [Disconnection and recognition of faces. A case with lesions of the left visual cortex and the splenium.] Revue Neurologique de Paris, 144, 113–19
Gallup, G. G. (1970). Chimpanzees: self-recognition. Science, 167, 86–7
Garety, P. A. & Hemsley, D. R. (1987). Characteristics of delusional experience. European Archives of Psychiatry and Neurological Science, 236, 294–8
Gruhle, H. W. (1932). Die Schizophrenie: Allgemeine Symptomatologie. Die Psychopathologie. In: Handbuch der Geisteskrankheiten, ed. O. Bumke, pp. 135–210. Berlin: Springer Verlag
Hattie, J. (1992). Self-concept. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Henry, M. (1963). L'essence de la manifestation. Paris: PUF
Henry, M. (1965). Philosophie et phénoménologie du corps. Paris: PFU
Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: a theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review, 94, 319–40
Hodges, J. R. & McCarthy, R. A. (1993). Autobiographical amnesia resulting from bilateral paramedian thalamic infarction. A case study in cognitive neurobiology. Brain, 116, 40
Hong, C. C., Gillin, J. C., Dow, B. M., Wu, J. & Buchsbaum, M. S. (1995). Localized and lateralized cerebral glucose metabolism associated with eye movements during REM sleep and wakefulness: a positron emission tomography (PET) study. Sleep, 18, 570–80
Husserl, E. (1922). Ideen zu einer reinen Phänomenologie. Augemeine Einf[w2]uhrung in die reine Phänomenologie, vol. 1, 2nd edn, p. ⅹ–323. Halle an der Saale: Max Niemeyer
Indefrey, P. & Levelt, W. J. M. (2000). The neural correlates of language production. In The New Cognitive Sciences, ed. M. S. Gazzaniga, Boston, MA: pp. 845–64. MIT Press
James, W. (1950). Principles of Psychology. New York: Dover Publications
Jaspers, K. (1913). Allgemeine Psychopathologie. Berlin: Springer
Jaspers, K. (1963). General Psychopathology. Manchester: Manchester University Press
Jeannerod, M. (1999). The 25th Bartlett lecture: to act or not to act: perspectives on the representation of actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52, A, 1–29
Kapur, S., Craik, F. I., Jones, C. et al. (1995). Functional role of the prefrontal cortex in retrieval of memories: a PET study. Neuroreport, 6, 1880–4
Keefe, R. S. E. (1998). The neurobiology of disturbances of self. In Insight and Psychosis, ed. X. F. Amador & A. S. David, pp. 142–73. New York: Oxford University Press
Keenan, J. M. (1993). An exemplar model can explain Klein and Loftus' results. In Advances in Social Cognition, ed. T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer, pp. 69–78. New York: Erlbaum
Keenan, J. M. & Baillet, S. D. (1983). Memory for personally and socially significant events. In Attention and Performance, ed. R. S. Nickerson, pp. 651–69. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Keith, L. K. & Bracken, B. A. (1996). Self-concept instrumentation: a historical and evaluative review. In Handbook of Self-Concept, ed. B. A. Bracken, pp. 91–171. New York: John Wiley
Kihlstrom, J. F. (1993). What does the self look like? In Advances in Social Cognition, ed. T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer, pp. 79–90. New York: Erlbaum
Kihlstrom, J. F. & Cantor, N. (1984). Mental representations of the self. Advances in Experimantal and Social Psychology, 17, 1–47
Kinderman, P. & Bentall, R. P. (1996). Self-discrepancies and persecutory delusions: evidence for a model of paranoid ideation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 106–13
Kinney, H. C., Brody, B. A., Kloman, A. S. & Gilles, F. H. (1988). Sequence of central nervous system myelination in human infancy. II. Patterns of myelination in autopsied infants. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology, 47, 217–34
Kinsbourne, M. (1995). Awareness of one's own body: an attentional theory of its nature, development and brain basis. In The Body and the Self, ed. J. L. Bermudez, A. Marcel & N. Eilan, pp. 205–23. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Kircher, T. T. J., Brammer, M. J., Simmons, A. & McGuire, P. K. (2000). Pausing for thought: Activation of left superior temporal sulcus when subjects pause during speech. submitted
Kircher, T. T. J., Liddle, P. F., Brammer, M. J. et al. (2001a). Neural correlates of formal thought disorder in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 769–74
Kircher, T., Baar, S., Plewnia, C. & Bartels, M. (2002a). Impaiment of facial self recognition in schizophrenia. in preparation
Kircher, T. T. J., Brammer, M. J., Bullmore, E. et al. (2002b). The neural correlates of intentional and incidental self processing. Neuropsychologia, 40, 683–92
Kircher, T. T. J., Senior, C., Phillips, M. L. et al. (2001b). Recognising one's own face. Cognition, 78, B1–15
Klatzky, R. L. & Forrest, F. H. (1984). Recognizing familiar and unfamiliar faces. Memory and Cognition, 12, 60–70
Klein, S. B. & Loftus, J. (1988). The nature of self-referent encoding: the contributions of elaborative and organizational processes. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 55, 5–11
Klein, S. B. & Loftus, J. (1993). The mental representation of trait and autobiographical knowledge about the self. In The Mental Representation of Trait and Autobiographical Knowledge about the Self. Advances in Social Cognition, ed, T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer, pp. 1–49. Hillsdale, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum
Knoblich, G. & Kircher, T. (2002). Deceiving oneself about control: sensitivity for mapping changes between movements and their consequences. submitted
Kraepelin, E. (1913). Ein Lehrbuch für Studierende und Ärzte. Leipzig: Barth
Krause, B. J., Schmidt, D., Mottaghy, F. M. et al. (1999). Episodic retrieval activates the precuneus irrespective of the imagery content of word pair associates. A PET study. Brain, 122, 255–63
Kronfeld, A. (1922). Ueber schizophrene Veränderungen des Bewutseins der Aktivität. Zeitschrift für die gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie, 74, 15–68
Lane, R. D., Reiman, E. M., Bradley, M. M. et al. (1997). Neuroanatomical correlates of pleasant and unpleasant emotion. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1437–44
Leicester, J., Sidman, M., Stoddard, L. T. & Mohr, J. P. (1969). Some determinants of visual neglect. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 32, 580–7
Lewis, A. (1934). The psychopathology of insight. Br. J. Med. Psychol., 14, 332–48
Linville, P. W. (1987). Self-complexity as a cognitive buffer against stress-related illness and depression. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 52, 663–76
Locke, J. (1959). An Essay Concerning Human Understanding. New York: Dover Publications
Lord, C. G. (1980). Schemas and images as memory aids: two models of processing social information. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 38, 257–69
Luria, A. R. (1969). Frontal lobe syndromes. In Handbook of Clinical Neurology, ed. P. J. Vinken & G. W. Bruyn, pp. 725–57. Amsterdam: North Holland
Lycan, W. G. (1997). Consciousness and Experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Lysaker, P. H., Bell, M. D., Bryson, G. & Kaplan, E. (1998). Neurocognitive function and insight in schizophrenia: support for an association with impairments in executive function but not with impairments in global function. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 97, 297–301
Maki, R. H. & Carlson, A. K. (1993). Knowledge of the self: is it special? In Advances in Social Cognition, ed. T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer, pp. 101–110. New York: Erlbaum
Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about the self. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 35, 63–78
Marsh, H. W. & Hattie, J. (1996). Theoretical perspectives on the structure of self-concept. In Handbook of Self-Concept, ed. B. A. Bracken. New York: John Wiley
McGlynn, S. M. & Schacter, D. L. (1989). Unawareness of deficits in neuropsychological syndromes. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11, 143–205
McGuire, P. K., Silbersweig, D. A., Wright, I. et al. (1995). Abnormal monitoring of inner speech: a physiological basis for auditory hallucinations. Lancet, 346, 596–600
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, Self and Society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1965). Phänomenologie der Wahrnehmung. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter
Metzinger, T. (1995). Conscious Experience. Paderborn: Schöningh-Verlag
Miller, L. A. (1992). Impulsivity, risk-taking, and the ability to synthesize fragmented information after frontal lobectomy. Neuropsychologia, 30, 69–79
Mohamed, S., Fleming, S., Penn, D. L. & Spaulding, W. (1999). Insight in schizophrenia: its relationship to measures of executive functions. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 187, 525–31
Neisser, U. (1988). Five kinds of self-knowledge. Philosophical Psychology, 1, 35–59
Parker, S. T., Mitchell, R. W. & Boccia, M. C. (1994). Self Awareness in Animals and Humans. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Parnas, J. (2000). The self and intentionality in the pre-psychotic stages of schizophrenia. In Exploring the Self, ed. D. Zahavi, pp. 115–47. Amsterdam: John Benjamin
Phillips, M. L., Young, A. W., Senior, C. et al. (1997). A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust. Nature, 389, 495–8
Pilowsky, T., Yirmiya, N., Arbelle, S. & Mozes, T. (2000). Theory of mind abilities of children with schizophrenia, children with autism, and normally developing children. Schizophrenia Research, 42, 145–55
Povinelli, D. J. & Preuss, T. M. (1995). Theory of mind: evolutionary history of a cognitive specialization. Trends in Neuroscience, 18, 418–24
Preilowski, B. (1979). Consciousness after complete surgical section of the forebrain commissures in man. In Structure and Function of Cerebral Commissures, ed. I. S. Russell, M. W. van Hoff & G. Berlucchi, pp. 411–20. London: Macmillan Press
Rees, G. (2001). Neuroimaging of visual awareness in patients and normal subjects. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11, 150–6
Rogers, T. B., Kuiper, N. A. & Kirker, W. S. (1977). Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 35, 677–88
Sanz, M., Constable, G., Lopez-Ibor, I., Kemp, R. & David, A. (1997). A comparative study of insight scales and their relationship to psychopathological and clinical variables. Psychological Medicine, 28, 437–46
Sarfati, Y. & Hardy Bayle, M. C. (1999). How do people with schizophrenia explain the behaviour of others? A study of theory of mind and its relationship to thought and speech disorganization in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 29, 613–20
Scharfetter, C. (1980). General Psychopathology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Scharfetter, C. (1981). Ego-psychopathology: the concept and its empirical evaluation. Psychological Medicine, 11, 273–80
Schneider, K. (1967). Klinische Psychopathologie, 8th edn. Stuttgart: Thieme Verlag
Shallice, T., Fletcher, P., Frith, C. D. et al. (1994). Brain regions associated with acquisition and retrieval of verbal episodic memory. Nature, 368, 633–5
Sims, A. (1988). Symptoms in the Mind. An Introduction to Descriptive Psychopathology. London: WB Saunders
Sirigu, A., Daprati, E., Pradat Diehl, P., Franck, N. & Jeannerod, M. (1999). Perception of self-generated movement following left parietal lesion. Brain, 122, 74
Slachevsky, A., Pillon, B., Fourneret, P. et al. (2001). Preserved adjustment but impaired awareness in a sensory-motor conflict following prefrontal lesions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 332–40
Smith, T. E., Hull, J. W., Israel, L. M. & Willson, D. F. (2000). Insight, symptoms, and neurocognition in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 26, 193–200
Snygg, D. & Combs, A. W. (1949). Individual Behaviour. New York: Harper & Row
Spence, S. A., Brooks, D. J., Hirsch, S. R. et al. (1997). A PET study of voluntary movement in schizophrenic patients experiencing passivity phenomena (delusions of alien control). Brain, 120, 1997–2011
Sperry, R. W. (1950). Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 43, 482–9
Sperry, R. W., Zaidel, E. & Zaidel, D. (1979). Self recognition and social awareness in the deconnected minor hemisphere. Neuropsychologia, 17, 153–66
Srinivasan, R., Russell, D. P., Edelman, G. M. & Tononi, G. (1999). Increased synchronization of neuromagnetic responses during conscious perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 5435–48
Stanton, B. & David, A. S. (2000). First person accounts of delusions. Psychiatric Bulletin, 24, 333–6
Startup, M. (1997). Awareness of own and others' schizophrenic illness. Schizophrenia Research, 26, 203–11
Strauss, J. S. (1969). Hallucinations and delusions as points on continua function. Archives of General Psychiatry, 21, 581–6
Swanson, C. L., Freudenreich, N. J., McEvoy, J. P. et al. (1995). Insight in schizophrenia and mania. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 183, 752–5
Symons, C. S. & Johnson, B. T. (1997). The self-reference effect in memory: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 371–94
Tulving, E. (1991). Concepts of human memory. In Memory: Organisation and Locus of Change, ed. G. Squire, G. Lynch, N. M. Weinberger & J. L. McGaugh. Oxford University Press, New York
Tulving, E. (1993a). Self-knowledge of an amnesic individual is represented abstractly. In The Mental Representation of Trait and Autobiographical Knowledge about the Self. Advances in social cognition, ed. T. Srull & R. S. Wyer, p. ⅷ. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Tulving, E. (1993b). Self-knowledge of an amnesic is represented abstractly. In The Mental Representation of Trait and Autobiographical Knowledge about the Self. Advances in Social Cognition, ed. T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer, pp. 147–56. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Valentine, T. & Bruce, V. (1986). Recognizing familiar faces: the role of distinctiveness and familiarity. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 40, 300–5
Vandenberghe, R., Price, C., Wise, R., Josephs, O. & Frackowiak, R. S. (1996). Functional anatomy of a common semantic system for words and pictures. Nature, 383, 254–6
Holst, E. & Mittelstaedt, H. (1950). Das Reafferenzprinzip (Wechselwirkungen zwischen Zentralnervensystem and Peripherie). Naturwissenschaften, 37, 464–76
Walsh, K. & Darby, D. (1999). Neuropsychology. A Clinical Approach, 4th edn. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone
Wiggs, C. L., Weisberg, J. & Martin, A. (1999). Neural correlates of semantic and episodic memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia, 37, 103–18
Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z. & Jordan, M. I. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science, 269, 1880–2
Wylie, R. C. (1989). Measures of Self-concept. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press
Yoon, H. U. (2001). Ich selbst oder ein anderer? Vertraut oder fremd? Untersuchungen zur Gesichtererkennung. Doctoral dissertation. University of Tübingen
Young, A. W., Hay, D. C., McWeeny, K. H., Flude, B. M. & Ellis, A. W. (1985). Matching familiar and unfamiliar faces on internal and external features. Perception, 14, 737–46
Young, D. A., Davila, R. & Scher, H. (1993). Unawareness of illness and neuropsychological performance in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 10, 117–24