Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-jr42d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T13:55:39.117Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - Self-awareness, social intelligence and schizophrenia

from Part II - Cognitive and neurosciences

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 December 2009

Gordon G. Gallup Jr
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, State University of New York at Albany, Albany, NY, USA
James R. Anderson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Stirling, Scotland
Steven M. Platek
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Drexel University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
Tilo Kircher
Affiliation:
Eberhard-Karls-Universität Tübingen, Germany
Anthony David
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, London
Get access

Summary

Abstract

The purpose of this chapter is to review the evidence concerning mirror self-recognition as a measure of self-awareness and examine its applicability to schizophrenia. The evidence suggests that the ability to identify yourself correctly in a mirror is not only related to the capacity to conceive of yourself, but may also be related to your ability to take into account what other individuals may know, want or intend to do. This ability to make accurate inferences about mental states in others (known as mental state attribution, theory of mind or social intelligence) begins to emerge during childhood at the same point in time as mirror self-recognition. Species that fail to recognize themselves in mirrors fail to show any evidence that they can infer mental states in one another. Also consistent with the proposition that these phenomena go hand in hand, recent neuropsychological evidence shows that self-awareness and mental state attribution in humans appear to be a byproduct of brain activity that is related to the frontal cortex. As detailed here and elsewhere in this volume, there is growing evidence that both self-awareness and mental state attribution is impaired in schizophrenic patients and that schizophrenia may be related to frontal lobe dysfunction.

Mirror self-recognition

Mirrors have a number of unique psychological properties. In principle, mirrors represent a means of seeing yourself as you are seen by others. In front of a mirror you are literally an audience to your own behaviour.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abley, P. (1930). Le signe du miroir dans les psychoses et plus specialement dans la demence precoce. Annales Medico-Psychologiques, 88, 28–36Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R.(1984a). Monkeys with mirrors: some questions for primate psychology. International Journal of Primatology, 5, 81–98Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1994). The monkey in the mirror: the strange conspecific. In Self-awareness in Animals and Humans: Developmental Perspectives, ed. S. T. Parker, R. W. Mitchell & M. L. Boccia, pp. 315–29. New York: Cambridge University Press
Anderson, J. R. (1984b). The development of self-recognition: a review. Developmental Psychobiology, 17, 35–49Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1999). Primates and representations of self. Current Psychology ofCognition, 18, 1005–29Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. & Gallup, G. G. Jr (1999). Self-recognition in nonhuman primates: past and future challenges. In Animal Models of Human Emotion andxs Cognition, ed. M. Haug & R. E. Whalen, pp. 175–94. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association
Anil, M. H., McKinstrey, J. L., Field, M. & Rodway, R. G. (1997). Lack of evidence for stress being caused to pigs by witnessing the slaughter of conspecifics. Animal Welfare, 6, 3–8Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). The cognitive neuroscience of autism: evolutionary approaches. In The New Cognitive Neurosciences, 2nd edn, ed. M. Gazzaniga, pp. 1249–57. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H. & Cohen, D. (1993). Understanding Other Minds: Perspective from Autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H., Moriarty, J. et al. (1994). Recognition of mental state terms: a clinical study of autism and a functional neuroimaging study of normal adults. British Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 640–9Google Scholar
Biringer, F. & Anderson, J. R. (1992). Self-recognition in Alzheimer's disease: a mirror and video study. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Science, 47, 385–8Google Scholar
Biringer, F. & Anderson, J. R. (1993). Self-recognition in Alzheimer's disease: use of mirror and video techniques and enrichment. In Recent Advances in Aging Science, vol. 1, ed. E. Beregi, I. A. Gergely & K. Rajczi, pp. 697–705. Bologna: Monduzzi Editore
Biringer, F., Anderson, J. R. & Strubel, D. (1988). Self-recognition in senile dementia. Experimental Aging Research, 14, 177–80Google Scholar
Blakemore, S. J., Smith, J., Steel, R., Johnstone, E. C. & Frith, C. (2000). The perception of self-produced sensory stimuli in patients with auditory hallucinations and passivity experiences: evidence for a breakdown in self-monitoring. Psychological Medicine, 30, 1131–9Google Scholar
Breen, N., Caine, D., Coltheart, M., Hendy, J. & Roberts, C. (2000). Towards an understanding of delusions of misidentification: four case studies. Mind and Language, 15, 74–110Google Scholar
Breen, N., Caine, D. & Coltheart, M. (2001). Mirrored-self misidentification: two cases of focal onset dementia. Neurocase, 7, 239–54Google Scholar
Buchanan, R. W., Vladar, K., Barta, P. E. & Pearlson, G. D. (1998). Structural evaluation of the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 121–7Google Scholar
Butterworth, G. (1994). Theory of mind and the facts of embodiment. In Children's Early Understanding of Mind: Origins and Development, ed. C. Lewis & P. Mitchell, pp. 115–32. Hove, UK: Erlbaum
Butterworth, G. & Jarret, N. L. M. (1991). What minds have in common space: spatial preverbal communication in human infancy. In Attention and Performance, ed. M. Jeannerod, pp. 605–24, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Carruthers, P. & Smith, P. K. (1996). Theories of Theories of Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Cheney, D. L. & Seyfarth, R. W. (1990). How Monkeys see the World: Inside the Mind of Another Species. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Corcoran, R., Cahill, C. & Frith, C. D. (1997). The appreciation of visual jokes in people with schizophrenia: a study of ‘mentalizing’ ability. Schizophrenia Research, 24, 319–27Google Scholar
Daprati, E., Frank, N., Georgieff, N. et al. (1997). Looking for the agent: an investigation into consciousness of action and self-consciousness in schizophrenic patients. Cognition, 65, 71–86Google Scholar
Dawson, G. & McKissick, F. C. (1984). Self-recognition in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 383–94Google Scholar
Dunn, J. (1994). Changing minds and changing relationships. In Children's Early Understanding of Mind: Origins and Development, ed. C. Lewis & P. Mitchell, pp. 297–310. Hove, UK: Erlbaum
Ferrari, M.&Matthews, W. S. (1983). Self-recognition deficits in autism: syndrome-specific or general developmental delay? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 13, 317–24Google Scholar
Fink, G. R., Markowitsch, H. J., Reinkemeier, M. et al. (1996). Cerebral representations of one's own past: neural networks in autobiographical memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 4257–82Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Everett, B. A., Croft, K. & Flavell, E. R. (1981). Young children's knowledge about visual perception: further evidence for the Level 1–Level 2 distinction. Developmental Psychology, 17, 99–103Google Scholar
Frith, C. D. (1992). The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Frith, C. D. (1997). Functional brain imaging and the neuropathology of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 23, 525–7Google Scholar
Frith, C. D. & Corcoran, R. (1996). Exploring ‘theory of mind’ in people with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 26, 521–30Google Scholar
Gallup, G. G. Jr (1968). Mirror-image stimulation. Psychological Bulletin, 70, 782–93Google Scholar
Gallup, G. G. Jr (1970). Chimpanzees: self-recognition. Science, 167, 86–7Google Scholar
Gallup, G. G. Jr (1982). Self-awareness and the emergence of theory of mind in primates. American Journal of Primatology, 2, 237–48Google Scholar
Gallup, G. G. Jr (1988). Towards a taxonomy of mind in primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 255–6Google Scholar
Gallup, G. G. Jr (1998). Self-awareness and the evolution of social intelligence. Behavior Processes, 42, 239–47Google Scholar
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. & Selemon, L. D. (1997). Functional and anatomical aspects of prefrontal pathology in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 23, 437–58Google Scholar
Happe, F. G., Brownell, H. & Winner, E. (1999). Acquired ‘theory of mind’ impairments following stroke. Cognition, 70, 211–40Google Scholar
Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B. & Tomasello, M. (2000). Chimpanzees know what conspecific's do and do not see. Animal Behavior, 59, 771–85Google Scholar
Harrington, A., Oepen, G. & Manfred, S. (1989). Disordered recognition and perception of human faces in acute schizophrenia and experimental psychosis. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 30, 376–84Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (1998). Theory of mind in non-human primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 101–48Google Scholar
Howe, M. L. & Courage, M. L. (1993). On resolving the enigma of infantile amnesia. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 305–26Google Scholar
Hsai, Y. F. & Tsai, N. (1981). Transcultural investigation of recent symptomatology of schizophrenia in China. American Journal of Psychiatry, 138, 1484–6Google Scholar
Keenan, J. P., McCutcheon, N. B., Freund, S. et al. (1999). Left hand advantage in a self-face recognition task. Neuropsychologia, 37, 1421–5Google Scholar
Keenan, J. P., Wheeler, M. A., Gallup, G. G. Jr & Pascual-Leone, A. (2000). Self-recognition and the right prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 338–44Google Scholar
Keenan, J. P., McCutcheon, N. B. & Pascual-Leone, A. (2001). Functional magnetic resonance imaging and event related potentials suggest right prefrontal activation for self-related processing. Brain and Cognition, 47, 87–91Google Scholar
Kircher, T. T. J., Senior, C., Phillips, M. L.et al. (2000). Towards a functional neuroanatomy of self-processing: effects of faces and words. Cognitive Brain Research, 10, 133–44Google Scholar
Kircher, T. T. J., Senior, C., Phillips, M. L.et al. (2001). Recognizing one's own face. Cognition, 78, B1–B15Google Scholar
Kremen, W. S., Faraone, S. V., Toomey, R., Seidman, L. J. & Tsuang, M. T. (1998). Sex differences in self-reported schizotypal traits in relatives of schizophrenic probands. Schizophrenia Research, 34, 27–37Google Scholar
Ledbetter, D. H. & Basen, J. (1982). Failure to demonstrate self-recognition in gorillas. American Journal of Primatology, 2, 307–10Google Scholar
Lempers, J. D., Flavell, E. R. & Flavell, J. H. (1977). The development in very young children of tacit knowledge concerning visual perception. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 95, 3–53Google Scholar
Lethmate, J. & Ducker, G. (1973). Untersuchungen zum Selbsterkennen im Spiegel bei Orang-utans and eingen anderen Affenarten. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 33, 248–69Google Scholar
Lewis, M. & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social Cognition and the Acquisition of Self. New York: Plenum Press
Lewis, M. & Mitchell, P. (eds) (1994). Children's Early Understanding of Mind: Origins and Development. Hove, UK: Erlbaum
Markowitsch, H. J. & Ewald, K. (1997). Right-hemispheric fronto-temporal injury leading to severe autobiographical retrograde and moderate anterograde episodic amnesia. Neurology, Psychiatry and Brain Sciences, 5, 71–8Google Scholar
Mayer, R. & Darby, S. J. (1991). Does a mirror deter wandering in demented older people?International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 6, 607–9Google Scholar
McDowell, J. E. & Clementz, B. A. (1997). The effect of fixation condition manipulations on anti-saccade performance in schizophrenics: studies of diagnostic specificity. Experimental Brain Research, 115, 333–44Google Scholar
Meador, K. J., Loring, D. W., Feinberg, T. E., Lee, G. P. & Nichols, M. E. (2000). Anosognosia and asomatognosia during intracarotid amobarbital inactivation. Neurology, 55, 816–20Google Scholar
Menzel, E. W., Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. & Lawson, J. (1985). Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes)spatial problem solving with the use of mirrors and televised equivalent mirrors. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 99, 211–17Google Scholar
Milner, D. (1997). Human Neural and Behavioral Development. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas
Neuman, C. J. & Hill, S. D. (1978). Self-recognition and stimulus preference in autistic children. Developmental Psychobiology, 11, 571–8Google Scholar
Nyberg, L., McIntosh, A. R., Cabeza, R. et al. (1996). Network analysis of positron emission tomography regional cerebral blood flow data: ensemble inhibition during episodic memory retrieval. Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 3753–9Google Scholar
Orbach, J., Traub, A. C. & Olson, R. (1966). Psychological studies of body-image II: normative data on the adjustable body-distorting mirror. Archives of General Psychiatry, 14, 41–7Google Scholar
Patterson, F. G. P. & Cohn, R. H. (1994). Self-recognition and self-awareness in lowland gorillas. In Self-awareness in Animals and Humans: Developmental Perspectives, ed. S. T. Parker, R. W. Mitchell & M. L. Boccia, pp. 273–90. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Pierrot-Deseilligny, C., Gaymard, B., Muri, R.& Rivaud, S. (1997). Cerebral ocular motor signs. Journal of Neurology, 244, 65–70Google Scholar
Platek, S. M. & Gallup, G. G. Jr(2002). Self-face recognition is affected by schizoptypal personality traits. Schizophrenia Research, 57, 311–15Google Scholar
Potterat, E., Perry, W. & Braff, D. L. (1997). Measuring the density of executive functioning in schizophrenic patients. Biological Psychiatry, 41, 86SGoogle Scholar
Povinelli, D. J. & Eddy, T. J. (1996). Factors influencing young chimpanzees' (Pan troglodytes)recognition of attention. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 110, 336–45Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., Parks, K. A. & Novak, M. A.(1991). Do rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta)attribute knowledge and ignorance to others?Journal of Comparative Psychology, 105, 318–25Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., Rulf, A. B., Landau, K. R.&Bierschwale, D. T.(1993). Self-recognition in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): distribution, ontogeny, and patterns of emergence. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 107, 347–72Google Scholar
Rochat, P. (1995). Perceived reachability for self and others by 3–5-year-old children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 317–33Google Scholar
Rosenzweig, S. & Shakow, D. (1937). Mirror behavior in schizophrenic and normal individuals. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 86, 166–74Google Scholar
Schultz, T. R. & Wells, D. (1985). Judging the intentionality of action-outcomes. Developmental Psychology, 21, 83–9Google Scholar
Semendeferi, K. (1999). The frontal lobes of the great apes with a focus on the gorilla and the orangutan. In The Mentalities of Gorillas and Orangutans, ed. S. Parker, R. Mitchell & H. Miles, pp. 70–95. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Shammi, P. & Stuss, D. T. (1999). Humor appreciation: a role of the right frontal lobe. Brain, 122, 657–66Google Scholar
Shillito, D. J., Gallup, G. G. Jr & Beck, B. B. (1999). Factors effecting mirror behavior in western lowland gorillas, Gorilla gorilla.Animal Behavior, 57, 999–1004Google Scholar
Siegal, M., Carrington, J. & Radel, M. (1996). Theory of mind and pragmatic understanding following right hemisphere damage. Brain and Language, 53, 40–50Google Scholar
Spangenberg, K., Wagner, M. T. & Bachman, D. L. (1998). Neuropsychological analysis of a case of abrupt onset following a hypotensive crisis in a patient with vascular dementia. Neurocase, 4, 149–54Google Scholar
Spiker, D.&Ricks, M. (1984). Visual self-recognition in autistic children: developmental relationships. Child Development, 55, 214–25Google Scholar
Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S. & Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 640–56Google Scholar
Stuss, D. T., Gallup, G. G. Jr & Alexander, M. P. (2001). The frontal lobes are necessary for theory of mind. Brain, 124, 279–86Google Scholar
Suarez, S. D. & Gallup, G. G. Jr (1981). Self-recognition in chimpanzees and orangutans, but not gorillas. Journal of Human Evolution, 10, 175–88Google Scholar
Swartz, K. B. (1998). Self-recognition in nonhuman primates. In Comparative Psychology: A Handbook, ed. G. Greenberg & M. Haraway, pp. 849–55. New York: Garland
Traub, A. C. & Orbach, J. (1964). Psychophysical studies of body image I: The adjustable body-distorting mirror. Archives of General Psychiatry, 11, 53–66Google Scholar
Does, A. W. & Bosch, R. J. (1992). What determines Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizophrenics? Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 567–83Google Scholar
Walraven, V., Elsacker, L. & Verheyen, R. (1995). Reactions of a group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) to their mirror images: evidence for self-recognition. Primates, 36, 145–50Google Scholar
Westergaard, G. C. & Hyatt, C. W. (1994). The responses of bonobos (Pan paniscus) to their mirror images: evidence of self-recognition. Human Evolution, 9, 323–9Google Scholar
Wheeler, M. A., Stuss, D. & Tulving, E. (1997). Toward a theory of episodic memory: the frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 331–54Google Scholar
Zahn-Waxler, C. & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1982). The development of altruism: alternative research strategies. In The Development of Prosocial Behavior, ed. N. Eisenberg, pp. 107–30. New York: Academic Press

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×