Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T19:37:07.355Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

45 - The Importance of Developmental Plasticity

from Section A - Cognitive Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 August 2016

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Susan T. Fiske
Affiliation:
Princeton University, New Jersey
Donald J. Foss
Affiliation:
University of Houston
Get access

Summary

The Importance of Developmental Plasticity

What do I consider my most important scientific contribution? There are actually two important contributions that are intertwined – one mostly not my own, and one that is to some degree mine. The first – a contribution of great importance that came primarily from the work of Ursula Bellugi, Edward Klima, and William Stokoe – concerns the discovery that signed languages of Deaf communities around the world are natural languages, equal in expressiveness and in grammatical complexity to spoken languages. When I was in graduate school and before, most psychologists and linguists believed that human language was uniquely connected to speech – that all languages around the globe were produced with the mouth and processed with the ear.

Thanks to the groundbreaking work of Bellugi and Klima and of Stokoe, we now know that, when speech is unavailable, humans readily use their hands and eyes for communication and that signed languages can become as complex and elegantly structured as spoken languages. I am proud to have done some of my research on the structure and acquisition of American Sign Language, in collaboration with Ted Supalla, a Deaf native signer whose native language is ASL (and who is also my husband). Our work on ASL has been in the context of another passionate interest that has been the main focus of my scientific career.

My own most important contribution, I think, concerns developmental plasticity. I've long been interested in mechanisms of learning: How do we learn, what are we especially good at learning, what are the principles and constraints on the way we learn? I've been particularly interested in why infants and young children are better than adults at learning certain types of skills, especially language. This issue was first raised by Eric Lenneberg, who suggested that there may be a “critical period” for language acquisition in humans. I read Lenneberg's famous 1969 Science article in a first-year proseminar in graduate school, and then his 1967 book Biological Foundations of Language (the Science article is a précis of his book). One idea in that work is that there may be a critical period for human language learning – a window of time during which we are especially good at learning languages but not as good thereafter, even with the same amount and type of exposure.

Type
Chapter
Information
Scientists Making a Difference
One Hundred Eminent Behavioral and Brain Scientists Talk about Their Most Important Contributions
, pp. 210 - 213
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Lenneberg, E. H. (1969). On explaining language: The development of language in children can best be understood in the context of developmental biology. Science, 164, 635–643.Google Scholar
Newport, E. L. (1990). Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive Science, 14, 11–28.Google Scholar
Newport, E. L. (2016). Statistical language learning: Computational, maturational and linguistic constraints. Language and Cognition, in press.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×