Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T01:43:09.539Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

55 - Follow the Evidence, Ignore the Words

from Section B - Social/Personality Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 August 2016

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Susan T. Fiske
Affiliation:
Princeton University, New Jersey
Donald J. Foss
Affiliation:
University of Houston
Get access

Summary

Charles Darwin, not Albert Einstein, is the more appropriate role model for psychologists because he did not begin the Beagle voyage with a favorite idea he wished to prove. Rather, he was open to pursuing any puzzling observation without prejudice. Fortunately, nature furnished the evidence that his intelligence recognized as inconsistent with popular ideology. The rest is history.

I made the few modest discoveries that brought me a small measure of recognition because I adopted Darwin's strategy. I distrusted a priori hypotheses and remained vigilant for observations that were reliable and appeared to be related to a theoretically important issue. When chance presented such observations, I pursued them with the methods available at that time.

This approach generated several discoveries that, I hope, contributed to a deeper understanding of human development. The unexpected observation, in the 1970s, of an increase in attention to faces by infants of seven to nine months old implied an enhancement in working memory due to the maturation of a circuit connecting the temporal and frontal lobes. This discovery, which was later confirmed by others, was satisfying because it explained why seven to nine months is the interval when most infants cry when a stranger approaches them too quickly or their mother leaves them alone for a few minutes in an unfamiliar room. Infants cry because they compare the event with their acquired knowledge and cannot resolve the discrepancy. That comparison requires an improved working memory.

The research on temperament, conducted over the past thirty-five years, has brought more satisfaction, partly because most psychologists regard the discoveries as more significant. Hence, this short essay describes the events that led to the invention of the concepts of high- and low-reactive infants and inhibited and uninhibited children.

These ideas had their origin in the early 1960s, when Howard Moss and I were studying a large group of twenty- to thirty-year-olds on whom extensive observations from infancy to late adolescence were available. A small number of adults whose personality was marked by caution, uncertainty, and a risk-averse posture to challenge typically also had retreated from unfamiliar people or events during their first three years. A second, equally small group showed the contrasting profile of an exuberant approach to, rather than avoidance of, unfamiliar people, objects, or places.

Type
Chapter
Information
Scientists Making a Difference
One Hundred Eminent Behavioral and Brain Scientists Talk about Their Most Important Contributions
, pp. 260 - 263
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Kagan, J. (1994). Galen's prophecy. New York: Basic Books.
Kagan, J. (2010). The temperamental thread. New York: Dana Press.
Kagan, J., & Snidman, N. (2004). The long shadow of temperament. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×