Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-gtxcr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T15:52:45.033Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - Cognitive flexibility in autism: a social-developmental account

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 February 2011

Ilona Roth
Affiliation:
The Open University, Milton Keynes
Payam Rezaie
Affiliation:
The Open University, Milton Keynes
Get access

Summary

There are important social-developmental contributions to the acquisition of flexible, generative, context-sensitive thinking and engagement with the world. We highlight how among individuals with autism, limitations in the propensity to identify with the attitudes of other people might lead to a paucity of movement across alternative ‘takes’ on the world, and with this, specific forms of cognitive restriction and rigidity.

Introduction

Consider this condensed version of a classic description of a person with autism called L (Scheerer et al. 1945). L was first seen at the age of 11 with a history of severe learning difficulties. He was said never to have shown interest in his social surroundings. Although he had an IQ of only 50 on a standardised test of intelligence, L could recount the day and date of his first visit to a place, and could usually give the names and birthdays of all the people he met there. He could spell forwards and backwards.

L's background history included the fact that in his fourth and fifth years he rarely offered spontaneous observations or reasons for any actions or perceived event. Nor would he imitate an action of others spontaneously. He was unable to understand or create an imaginary situation. He did not play with toys, nor did he show any conception of make-believe games. He was unable to converse in give-and-take language. He barely noticed the presence of other children, and was said to have ‘little emotionality of normal depth and coherence’.

Type
Chapter
Information
Researching the Autism Spectrum
Contemporary Perspectives
, pp. 266 - 283
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Theory of mind and autism: a fifteen year review. In Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 3–20.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., Cohen, D.J. (2000). Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bishop, D.V.M., Norbury, C.F. (2005). Executive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symtomatology. I: Generativity. Autism, 9: 7–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, M., Hobson, R.P., Lee, A. (2005). Symbolic play in congenitally blind children. Development and Psychopathology, 17: 447–465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bretherton, I., McNew, S., Beeghly-Smith, M. (1981). Early person knowledge as expressed in gestural and verbal communication: When do infants acquire a ‘theory of mind’? In Infant Social Cognition: Empirical and Theoretical Considerations. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 333–373.Google Scholar
Brown, R., Hobson, R.P., Lee, A., Stevenson, J. (1997). Are there ‘autistic-like’ features in congenitally blind children? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38: 693–703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Volume 63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., et al. (1997). Infants with autism: An investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33: 781–789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, J., Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Creativity and imagination in autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29: 319–326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, U. (2003). Autism: Explaining the Enigma, 2nd edn. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Geurts, H.M., Corbett, B., Solomon, M. (2009). The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13: 74–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffith, E.M., Pennington, B.F., Wehner, E.A., Rogers, S.J. (1999). Executive functions in young children with autism. Child Development, 70: 817–832.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Happé, F. (1995). Autism: An Introduction to Psychological Theory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hill, E.L. (2004). Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Developmental Review, 24: 189–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, E.L., Frith, U. (2003). Understanding autism: insights from mind and brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Part B, 358: 281–289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, J.A., Harris, R., García-Pérez, R., Hobson, R.P. (2008). Anticipatory concern: A study in autism. Developmental Science, 12: 249–263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, J.A., Hobson, R.P. (2007). Identification: The missing link between joint attention and imitation? Development and Psychopathology, 19: 411–431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R.P. (1989). Beyond cognition: A theory of autism. In Autism: Nature, Diagnosis, and Treatment. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 22–48.Google Scholar
Hobson, R.P. (1990). On acquiring knowledge about people and the capacity to pretend: Response to Leslie. Psychological Review, 97: 114–121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R.P. (1991). Against the theory of ‘Theory of Mind’. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9: 33–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R.P. (1993). Autism and the Development of Mind. Hove, Sussex: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hobson, R.P. (2000). The grounding of symbols: A social-developmental account. In Reasoning and the Mind. Hove, UK: Psychology Press, pp. 11–35.Google Scholar
Hobson, R.P. (2002). The Cradle of Thought. London: Pan Macmillan, and New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hobson, R.P. (2008). Interpersonally situated cognition. International Journal of Philosophical Studies, 6: 377–397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R.P., Bishop, M. (2003). The pathogenesis of autism: insights from congenital blindness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Part B, 358: 335–344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R.P., Lee, A. (1999). Imitation and identification in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40: 649–659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R.P., Lee, A., Brown, R. (1999). Autism and congenital blindness. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29: 45–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R.P., Meyer, J.A. (2005). Foundations for self and other: A study in autism. Developmental Science, 8: 481–491.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R.P., Chidambi, G., Lee, A., Meyer, J. (2006). Foundations for self-awareness: An exploration through autism. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Serial number 284: 71.Google ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R.P, Lee, A., Hobson, J.A. (2007). Only connect? Communication, identification, and autism. Social Neuroscience, 2: 320–335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R.P., Lee, A., Hobson, J.A. (2009). Qualities of symbolic play among children with autism: A social-developmental perspective. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39: 12–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jarrold, C., Boucher, J., Smith, P. (1993). Symbolic play in autism: A review. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23: 281–307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J. (1982). The emergence of self. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 23: 363–381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2: 217–250.Google Scholar
Kaler, S.R., Kopp, C.B. (1990). Compliance and comprehension in very young toddlers. Child Development, 61: 1997–2003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenworthy, L., Yerys, B.E., Anthony, L.G., Wallace, G.L. (2008). Understanding executive control in autism spectrum disorders in the lab and in the real world. Neuropsychological Review, 18: 320–338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leslie, A.M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of ‘Theory of mind’. Psychological Review, 94: 412–426.CrossRef
Lewis, M., Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social Cognition and the Acquisition of Self. New York and London: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M., Ramsay, D. (2004). Development of self-recognition, personal pronoun use, and pretend play during the second year. Child Development, 75: 1821–1831.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, V., Boucher, J. (1988). Spontaneous, instructed and elicited play in relatively able autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6: 325–339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lind, S.E., Williams, D.M.Behavioural, biopsychosocial, and cognitive models of Autism Spectrum Disorders. In International Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders. New York: Springer, in press.
Mead, G.H. (1934). Mind, Self, and Society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Meyer, J.A., Hobson, R.P. (2004). Orientation in relation to self and other: The case of autism. Interaction Studies, 5: 221–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minshew, N.J., Meyer, J.A., Goldstein, G. (2002). Abstract reasoning in autism: A dissociation between concept formation and concept identification. Neuropsychology, 16: 327–334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scheerer, M., Rothmann, E., Goldstein, K. (1945). A case of ‘idiot savant’: an experimental study of personality organisation. Psychological Monographs, 58: 1–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sigman, M.D., Kasari, C., Kwon, J.H., Yirmiya, N. (1992). Responses to the negative emotions of others by autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Child Development, 63: 796–807.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sorce, J.F., Emde, R.N., Campos, J., Klinnert, M.D. (1985). Maternal emotional signaling: Its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 21: 195–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2000). Language and understanding minds: connections in autism. In Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 124–149.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: a description of primary intersubjectivity. In Before Speech: The Beginning of Human Communication. London: Cambridge University Press, pp. 321–347.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C., Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In Action, Gesture and Symbol: The Emergence of Language. London: Academic Press, pp. 183–229.Google Scholar
Ungerer, J.A., Sigman, M. (1981). Symbolic play and language comprehension in autistic children. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 20: 318–337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vygotsky, L.S. (1962). Thought and Language. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Internalization of higher psychological functions. In Mind in Society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, pp. 52–57.Google Scholar
Werner, H., Kaplan, B. (1984). Symbol Formation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.Google Scholar
Wing, L., Gould, J., Yeates, S.R., Brierly, L.M. (1977). Symbolic play in severely mentally retarded and in autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 18: 167–178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×