Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T05:16:31.069Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

13 - RST and psychopathy: associations between psychopathy and the behavioral activation and inhibition systems

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 January 2011

John F. Wallace
Affiliation:
Professor, Department of Psychology University of Wisconsin-Madison, USA
Joseph P. Newman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, USA
Philip J. Corr
Affiliation:
University of Wales, Swansea
Get access

Summary

We review the experimental evidence regarding information processing anomalies that have been observed in psychopaths, and conclude that the body of evidence is in substantially better accord with the revised Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST), and, in particular, the current conceptualization of the BIS, than with the original version of RST. In addition, clear associations exist between psychopathy and self-report measures of the BAS and BIS constructs, and we discuss possible explanations for those associations that merit evaluation in future psychopathy research.

RST and psychopathy: associations between psychopathy and the BAS and BIS

Maladaptive behavior that is anti-social or impulsive in nature has been characterized as reflecting disinhibition – a decreased ability to regulate response inclinations in light of possible adverse consequences (e.g., Gorenstein and Newman 1980). Due to psychopaths' penchant for engaging in anti-social, maladaptive behavior, psychopathy is a prototypical example of the clinical syndromes that have disinhibited behavior as a prominent feature. For instance, although psychopaths make up only 1 per cent of the general population, they constitute 15 to 25 per cent of the prison population (Hare 1996), and psychopathic offenders are two to five times more likely to re-offend than are non-psychopaths (Hemphill, Hare and Wong 1998; Quinsey, Rice and Harris 1995; Serin 1996; Walters 2003).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arnett, P. A., Howland, E. W., Smith, S. S. and Newman, J. P. (1993), Autonomic responsivity during passive avoidance in incarcerated psychopaths, Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 173–185CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnett, P. A., Smith, S. S. and Newman, J. P. (1997), Approach and avoidance motivation in incarcerated psychopaths during passive avoidance, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1413–1428CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernstein, A., Newman, J. P., Wallace, J. F. and Luh, K. E. (2000), Left-hemisphere activation and deficient response modulation in psychopaths, Psychological Science, 11, 414–418CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blackburn, R. (1979), Cortical and autonomic arousal in primary and secondary psychopaths, Psychophysiology, 16, 143–150CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Book, A. S. and Quinsey, V. L. (2004), Psychopaths: cheaters or warrior-hawks?, Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 33–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carver, C. S. and White, T. L. (1994), Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the Behavioural Inhibition System/Behavioural Approach System scales, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cleckley, H. (1976), The Mask of Sanity (5th edn, St Louis, MO: Mosby)Google Scholar
Corr, P. J. (2000), J. A. Gray's reinforcement sensitivity theory: tests of the joint subsystems hypothesis of anxiety and impulsivity, Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 511–532CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowles, D. C. (1980), The three arousal model: implications of Gray's two-factor learning theory for heart rate, electrodermal activity, and psychopathy, Psychophysiology, 17, 87–104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gernsbacher, M. A. and Faust, M. E. (1991), The mechanism of suppression: a component of general comprehension skill, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 245–262Google ScholarPubMed
Gorenstein, E. E. and Newman, J. P. (1980), Disinhibitory psychopathology: a new perspective and a model for research, Psychological Review, 87, 301–315CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1975), Elements of a Two-Process Theory of Learning (London: Academic Press)Google Scholar
Gray, J. A.(1987), The Psychology of Fear and Stress (2nd edn, New York: Cambridge University Press)Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. and McNaughton, N. (2000), The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: an Enquiry into the Functions of the Septo-hippocampal System (2nd edn, New York: Oxford University Press)Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. and Smith, P. T. (1969), An arousal-decision model for partial reinforcement and discrimination learning in Gilbert, R. and Sutherland, N. S. (eds), Animal Discrimination Learning (New York: Academic Press), pp. 243–272Google Scholar
Hare, R. D. (1970), Psychopathy: Theory and Research (New York: Wiley)Google Scholar
Hare, R. D.(1996), Psychopathy: a clinical construct whose time has come, Criminal Justice and Behavior, 23, 25–54CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, R. D.(2003), Manual for the Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (2nd edn, Toronto: Multi-Health Systems)Google Scholar
Hemphill, J. F., Hare, R. D. and Wong, S. (1998), Psychopathy and recidivism: a review, Legal and Criminological Psychology, 3, 139–170CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hiatt, K. D., Schmitt, W. A. and Newman, J. P. (2004), Stroop tasks reveal abnormal selective attention in psychopathic offenders, Neuropsychology, 18, 50–59CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howland, E. W., Kosson, D. S., Patterson, C. M. and Newman, J. P. (1993), Altering a dominant response: performance of psychopaths and low socialization college students on a cued reaction time task, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 379–387CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kosson, D. S. (1998), Divided visual attention in psychopathic and nonpsychopathic offenders, Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 373–391CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lavie, N., Hirst, A., Fockert, J. W. and Viding, E. (2004), Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 339–354CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorenz, A. R. and Newman, J. P. (2002), Deficient response modulation and emotion processing in low-anxious Caucasian psychopathic offenders: results from a lexical decision task, Emotion, 2, 91–104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lykken, D. T. (1995), The Antisocial Personalities (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates)Google Scholar
MacCoon, D. G., Wallace, J. F. and Newman, J. P. (2004), Self-regulation: context-appropriate balanced attention in Baumeister, R. F. and Vohs, K. D. (eds), Handbook of Self-regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications (New York: Guilford Press), pp. 422–444Google Scholar
Most, S. B., Scholl, B. J., Clifford, E. R. and Simons, D. J. (2005), What you see is what you set: sustained inattentional blindness and the capture of awareness, Psychological Review, 112, 217–242CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, J. P. (2006), Correlations between measures of the Behavioural Approach System and Behavioural Inhibition System constructs and the PCL-R factors (unpublished raw data)
Newman, J. P. and Brinkley, C. A. (1997), Reconsidering the low-fear explanation for primary psychopathy, Psychological Inquiry, 8, 236–244CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, J. P. and Kosson, D. S. (1986), Passive avoidance learning in psychopathic and nonpsychopathic offenders, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 257–263CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman, J. P. and Lorenz, A. R. (2003), Response modulation and emotion processing: implications for psychopathy and other dysregulatory psychopathology in Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. and Goldsmith, H. H. (eds), Handbook of Affective Sciences (New York: Oxford University Press), pp. 1043–1067Google Scholar
Newman, J. P., MacCoon, D. G., Buckholtz, J. W., Bertsch, J. D., Hiatt, K. D. and Vaughn, L. J. (in press), Deficient integration of top-down and bottom-up influences on attention in psychopaths: potential contribution of the septo-hippocampal system in Barch, D. (ed.), Handbook of Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience of Psychopathology (New York: Oxford University Press)
Newman, J. P., MacCoon, D. G., Vaughn, L. J. and Sadeh, N. (2005), Validating a distinction between primary and secondary psychopathy with measures of Gray's Behavioural Inhibition System and Behavioural Approach System constructs, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 319–323CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, J. P., Patterson, C. M., Howland, E. W. and Nichols, S. L. (1990), Passive avoidance in psychopaths: the effects of reward, Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 1101–1114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, J. P., Patterson, C. M. and Kosson, D. S. (1987), Response perseveration in psychopaths, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 145–148CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman, J. P. and Schmitt, W. A. (1998), Passive avoidance in psychopathic offenders: a replication and extension, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 527–532CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, J. P., Schmitt, W. A. and Voss, W. (1997), The impact of motivationally neutral cues on psychopathic individuals: assessing the generality of the response modulation hypothesis, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 563–575CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman, J. P. and Wallace, J. F. (1993), Diverse pathways to deficient self-regulation: implications for disinhibitory psychopathology in children, Clinical Psychology Review, 13, 699–720CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patrick, C. J. (1994), Emotion and psychopathy: startling new insights, Psychophysiology, 31, 319–330CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patrick, C. J., Bradley, M. M. and Lang, P. J. (1993), Emotion in the criminal psychopath: startle reflex modulation, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 82–92CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patrick, C. J., Curtin, J. J. and Kreuger, R. F. (in press), The externalizing spectrum: structure and etiology in Barch, D. (ed.), Handbook of Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience of Psychopathology (New York: Oxford University Press)
Quinsey, V. L., Rice, M. E. and Harris, G. T. (1995), Actuarial prediction of sexual recidivism, Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 10, 85–105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmitt, W. A., Brinkley, C. A. and Newman, J. P. (1999), The application of Damasio's somatic marker hypothesis to psychopathic individuals: risk-takers or risk-averse?, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 538–543CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Serin, R. C. (1996), Violent recidivism in criminal psychopaths, Law and Human Behavior, 20, 207–217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torrubia, R., Ávila, C., Moltó, J. and Caseras, X. (2001), The sensitivity to punishment and sensitivity to reward questionnaire (SPSRQ) as a measure of Gray's anxiety and impulsivity dimensions, Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 837–862CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vitale, J. E., Brinkley, C. A., Hiatt, K. D. and Newman, J. P. (2007), Abnormal selective attention in psychopathic female offenders, Neuropsychology, 21, 301–312
Vitale, J. E., Newman, J. P., Bates, J. E., Goodnight, J., Dodge, K. A. and Petit, G. S. (2005), Deficient behavioral inhibition and anomalous selective attention in a community sample of adolescents with psychopathic and low-anxiety traits, Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 461–470CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallace, J. F., Schmitt, W. A., Vitale, J. E. and Newman, J. P. (2000), Experimental investigations of information-processing deficiencies in psychopaths: implications for diagnosis and treatment in Gacono, C. B. (ed.), The Clinical and Forensic Assessment of Psychopathy: a Practitioner's Guide (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), pp. 87–109Google Scholar
Walters, G. D. (2003), Predicting institutional adjustment and recidivism with the psychopathy checklist factor scores: a meta-analysis, Law and Human Behavior, 27, 541–558CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Welsh, G. S. (1956), Factor dimensions A and R in Welsh, G. S. and Dahlstrom, W. G. (eds), Basic Readings on the MMPI in Psychology and Medicine (Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press), pp. 264–281Google Scholar
Williamson, S., Harpur, T. J. and Hare, R. D. (1991), Abnormal processing of affective words by psychopaths, Psychophysiology, 28, 260–273CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×