Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T12:15:34.276Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

48 - Culture and Systems of Thought: Holistic versus Analytic Cognition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Richard E. Nisbett
Affiliation:
University of Michigan
Kaiping Peng
Affiliation:
University of California
Incheol Choi
Affiliation:
Seoul National University
Ara Norenzayan
Affiliation:
University of British Columbia
Jonathan E. Adler
Affiliation:
Brooklyn College, City University of New York
Lance J. Rips
Affiliation:
Northwestern University, Illinois
Get access

Summary

The British empiricist philosophers of the 18th and 19th centuries, including Locke, Hume, and Mill, wrote about cognitive processes as if they were the same for all normal adults. This assumption of universality was adopted by mainstream psychology of the 20th century, where it has been predominant from the earliest treatment of cognitive psychology by Piaget, to mid-century learning theorists, to modern cognitive science. The assumption of universality was probably strengthened by the analogy to the computer, which has been implicit and often explicit for the past 30 years (Block, 1995; Shweder, 1991). Brain equals hardware, inferential rules and data processing procedures equal the universal software, and output equals belief and behavior, which can, of course, be radically different given the different inputs possible for different individuals and groups. “Basic” processes such as categorization, learning, inductive and deductive inference, and causal reasoning are generally presumed to be the same among all human groups.

It appears, however, that fairly marked differences in knowledge about and use of inferential rules exist even among educated adults. Work by Nisbett and his colleagues (Larrick, Nisbett, & Morgan, 1993; Nisbett, 1993; Nisbett, Fong, Lehman, & Cheng, 1987; Smith, Langston, & Nisbett, 1992) shows that people can learn statistical, probabilistic, methodological, logical, deontic, cost-benefit, and other quite abstract rule systems and categorization procedures, and that training can affect their reasoning about everyday life events and even their behavior.

Type
Chapter
Information
Reasoning
Studies of Human Inference and its Foundations
, pp. 956 - 985
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abel, T. M., & Hsu, F. I. (1949). Some aspects of personality of Chinese as revealed by the Rorschach Test. Journal of Projective Techniques, 13, 285–301.Google ScholarPubMed
Adevai, G., Silverman, A. J., & McGough, W. E. (1970). Ethnic differences in perceptual testing. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 16, 237–239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allen, S. W., & Brooks, L. R. (1991). Specializing in the operation of an explicit rule. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 3–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asch, S. (1952). Social psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atran, S. (1990). Cognitive foundations of natural history. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Atran, S. (1995). Causal constraints on categories and categorical constraints on biological reasoning across cultures. In Sperber, D., Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (Eds.), Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 205–233). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bagley, C. (1995). Field independence in children in group-oriented cultures: Comparisons from China, Japan, and North America. The Journal of Social Psychology, 135, 523–525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R. (1995). Physical reasoning in infancy. In Gazzaniga, M. S. (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 181–204). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Baltes, P. B., & Staudinger, U. M. (1993). The search for a psychology of wisdom. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 75–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barry, H., Child, I., & Bacon, M. (1959). Relation of child training to subsistence economy. American Anthropologist, 61, 51–63.Google Scholar
Basseches, M. (1980). Dialectical schemata: A framework for the empirical study of the development of dialectical thinking. Human Development, 23, 400–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Basseches, M. (1984). Dialectical thinking and adult development. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Becker, C. B. (1986). Reasons for the lack of argumentation and debate in the Far East. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 10, 75–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlin, B. (1992). Ethnobiological classification: Principles of categorization of plants and animals in traditional societies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlin, B., Breedlove, D., & Raven, P. (1973). General principles of classification and nomenclature in folk biology. American Anthropologist, 74, 214–242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, J. W. (1967). Independence and conformity in subsistence-level societies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 415–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, J. W. (1976). Human ecology and cognitive style: Comparative studies in cultural and psychological adaptation. New York: Sage/Halsted.Google Scholar
Block, N. (1995). The mind as the software of the brain. In Smith, E. E. & Osherson, D. N. (Eds.), Thinking: An invitation to cognitive science (pp. 377–425). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bond, M. H. (1996). Chinese values. In Bond, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of Chinese psychology (pp. 208–226). Hong Kong, China: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (1993). The naturalness of religious ideas.Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Briley, D. A., Morris, M., & Simonson, I. (2000). Reasons as carriers of culture: Dynamic versus dispositional models of cultural influence on decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 27, 157–178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, J. A. (1983). Go: Introduction. In Bramer, M. A. (Ed.), Computer game playing (pp. 136–140). Chicester, England: Ellis Horwood.Google Scholar
Carey, S., & Spelke, E. (1994). Domain-specific knowledge and conceptual change. In Hirschfeld, L. A. & Gelman, S. A. (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 169–200). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cha, J.-H., & Nam, K. D. (1985). A test of Kelley's cube theory of attribution: A cross-cultural replication of McArthur's study. Korean Social Science Journal, 12, 151–180.Google Scholar
Chalfonte, B. L., & Johnson, M. K. (1996). Feature memory and binding in young and older adults. Memory & Cognition, 24, 403–416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chan, W.-T. (1967). The story of Chinese philosophy. In Moore, C. A. (Ed.), The Chinese mind: Essentials of Chinese philosophy and culture (pp. 245–257). Honolulu, HI: East-West Center Press.Google Scholar
Chang, T.-S. (1939). A Chinese philosopher's theory of knowledge. Yen-ching Journal of Social Studies, 11(2).
Chavajay, P., & Rogoff, B. (2000). Cultural variation in management of attention by children and their caregivers. Unpublished manuscript. University of California at Santa Cruz.Google Scholar
Chiu, L.-H. (1972). A cross-cultural comparison of cognitive styles in Chinese and American children. International Journal of Psychology, 7, 235–242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, I. (1998). The cultural psychology of surprise: Holistic theories, contradiction, and epistemic curiosity. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Choi, I., Dalal, R., & Kim-Prieto, C. (2000). Information search in causal attribution: Analytic vs. holistic. Unpublished manuscript, University of Illinois.Google Scholar
Choi, I., & Nisbett, R. E. (1998). Situational salience and cultural differences in the correspondence bias and in the actor-observer bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 949–960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, I., & Nisbett, R. E. (2000). The cultural psychology of surprise: Holistic theories and recognition of contradiction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 890–905.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, I., Nisbett, R. E., & Norenzayan, A. (1999). Causal attribution across cultures: Variation and universality. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 47–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, I., Nisbett, R. E., & Smith, E. E. (1997). Culture, categorization and inductive reasoning. Cognition, 65, 15–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, S., & Gopnik, A. (1995). Early acquisition of verbs in Korean: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Child Language, 22, 497–529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, M. (1995). Socio-cultural-historical psycho-logy: Some general remarks and a proposal for a new kind of cultural-genetic methodology. In Wertsch, J. V., Rio, P. D., & Alvarez, A. (Eds.), Sociocultural studies of mind (pp. 113–144). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, M., & Scribner, S. (1974). Culture and thought: A psychological introduction. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cromer, A. (1993). Uncommon sense: The heretical nature of science. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
D'Andrade, R. (1987). A folk model of the mind. In Holland, D. & Quinn, N. (Eds.), Cultural models in language and thought (pp. 112–148). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darley, J. M., & Batson, C. D. (1973). From Jerusalem to Jericho: A study of situational and dispositional variables in helping behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 100–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, M. (2000). Responses to weak argument on the part of Asians and Americans. University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.Google Scholar
Davis, M., Nisbett, R. E., & Schwarz, N. (2000). Responses to weak arguments by Asians and Americans. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1995). Darwin's dangerous idea: Evolution and meanings of life. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Dershowitz, Z. (1971). Jewish subcultural patterns and psychological differentiation. International Journal of Psychology, 6, 223–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dyson, F. J. (1998, May 28). Is God in the lab?New York Review of Books, 8–10.Google Scholar
Fernald, A., & Morikawa, H. (1993). Common themes and cultural variations in Japanese and American mothers' speech to infants. Child Development, 64, 637–656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischhoff, B. (1975). Hindsight/foresight: The effect of outcome knowledge on judgment under uncertainty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1, 288–299.Google Scholar
Fiske, A. P., Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Nisbett, R. E. (1998). The cultural matrix of social psychology. In Gilbert, D. T., Fiske, S. T., & Lindzey, G. (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 915–981). Boston: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Freeman, N. H., & Habermann, G. M. (1996). Linguistic socialization: A Chinese perspective. In Bond, M. H. (Ed.), The handbook of Chinese psychology (pp. 79–92), Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fung, Y. (1983). A history of Chinese philosophy (D. Bodde, Trans.) (Vol. 1–2). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Galtung, J. (1981). Structure, culture, and intellectual style: An essay comparing saxonic, teutonic, gallic and nipponic approaches. Social Science Information, 20, 817–856.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia, J., McGowan, B. K., Ervin, F., & Koelling, R. (1968). Cues: Their relative effectiveness as reinforcers. Science, 160, 794–795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelman, S. A. (1988). The development of induction within natural kind and artifact categories. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 65–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelman, S. A., & Tardif, T. (1998). A cross-linguistic comparison of generic noun phrases in English and Mandarin. Cognition, 66, 215–248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. (1995). The correspondence bias. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 21–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glass, D. C., & Singer, J. E. (1973). Experimental studies of uncontrollable and unpredictable noise. Representative Research in Psychology, 4, 165–183.Google Scholar
Hadingham, E. (1994). The mummies of Xinjiang. Discover, 15, 68–77.Google Scholar
Hamilton, E. (1973). The Greek way. New York: Avon. (Original work published 1930)Google Scholar
Hampden-Turner, C., & Trompenaars, A. (1993). The seven cultures of capitalism: Value systems for creating wealth in the United States, Japan, Germany, France, Britain, Sweden, and the Netherlands. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Hansen, C. (1983). Language and logic in ancient China. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Heath, S. B. (1982). What no bedtime story means: Narrative skills at home and school. Language in Society, 11, 49–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hedden, T., Ji, L., Jing, Q., Jiao, S., Yao, C., Nisbett, R. E., & Park, D. C. (2000, April). Culture and age differences in recognition memory for social dimensions. Paper presented at the Cognitive Aging Conference, Atlanta, GA.Google Scholar
Hirschfeld, L. (1996). Race in the making: Cognition, culture, and the child's construction of human kinds. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture's consequences: International differences in work-related values. Beverley Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Hong, Y., Chiu, C., & Kung, T. (1997). Bringing culture out in front: Effects of cultural meaning system activation on social cognition. In Leung, K., Kashima, Y., Kim, U., & Yamaguchi, S. (Eds.), Progress in Asian social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 135–146). Singapore: Wiley.Google Scholar
Hong, Y.-y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C.-y., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55, 709–720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsu, F. L. K. (1981). Americans and Chinese: Passage to differences. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Huang, J., & Chao, L. (1995). Chinese and American students' perceptual styles of field independence versus field dependence. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 80, 232–234.Google ScholarPubMed
Huff, T. E. (1993). The rise of early modern science: Islam, China, and the West. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ji, L. (2001). Culture, language and relationships vs. categories in cognition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.Google Scholar
Ji, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (2001). Culture, language and categories. Unpublished manuscript, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Ji, L., Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (2000). Culture, control, and perception of relationships in the environment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 943–955.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, E. E., & Harris, V. A. (1967). The attribution of attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 3, 1–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kane, G. (2000). Culture and science. Unpublished manuscript, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Kemler-Nelson, D. G. (1984). The effect of intention on what concepts are acquired. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 734–759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitayama, S., & Masuda, T. (1997). Shaiaiteki ninshiki no bunkateki baikai model: taiousei bias no bunkashinrigakuteki kentou. [Cultural psychology of social inference: The correspondence bias in Japan]. In Kashiwagi, K., Kitayama, S., & Azuma, H. (Eds.), Bunkashinrigaju: riron to jisho [Cultural psychology:Theory and evidence] (pp. 109–127). Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press.Google Scholar
Knox, B. (1990). Introduction to Homer's The Iliad (Robert Fagles, Trans.). St. Paul, MN: Penguin-High Bridge.Google Scholar
Korzybyski, A. (1994). Science and sanity: An introduction to non-Aristotelian systems and general semantics. Englewood, NJ: Institute of General Semantics. (Original work published 1933)Google Scholar
Kühnen, U., Hannover, B., Röder, U., Schubert, B., Shah, A. A., & Zakaria, S. (2000). Cross-cultural variations in identifying embedded figures: Comparisons from the US, Germany, Russia and Malaysia. Unpublished manuscript, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Kühnen, U., Hannover, B., & Schubert, B. (2000). Procedural consequences of semantic priming: The role of self-knowledge for context-bounded versus context-independent modes of thinking. Unpublished manuscript, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Langer, E. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 311–328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larrick, R. P., Nisbett, R. E., & Morgan, J. N. (1993). Who uses the cost-benefit rules of choice? Implications for the normative status of microeconomic theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 56, 331–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice: Mind, mathematics, and culture in everyday life. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, F., Hallahan, M., & Herzog, T. (1996). Explaining real life events: How culture and domain shape attributions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 732–741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A. M. (1982). The perception of causality in infants. Perception, 11, 173–186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leslie, A. M. (1994). ToMM, ToBY, and agency: Core architecture and domain specificity. In Hirschfeld, L. A. & Gelman, S. A. (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leung, K., & Morris, M. W. (2001). Justice through the lens of culture and ethnicity. In Sanders, J. & Hamilton, V. L. (Eds.), Handbook of law and social science: Justice. (pp. 343–377) New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Lin, Y. (1936). My country and my people. London: William Heinemann.Google Scholar
Liu, S. H. (1974). The use of analogy and symbolism in traditional Chinese philosophy. Journal of Chinese Philosophy, 1, 313–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lloyd, G. E. R. (1990). Demystifying mentalities. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lloyd, G. E. R. (1991). The invention of nature. In Lloyd, G. E. R. (Ed.), Methods and problems in Greek Science (pp. 417–434). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Logan, R. F. (1986). The alphabet effect. New York: Morrow.Google Scholar
Lord, C., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. (1979). Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 2098–2109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucy, J. A. (1992). Language diversity and thought: A reformulation of the linguistic relativity hypothesis. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1931). Psychological expedition to Central Asia. Science, 74, 383–384.Google Scholar
Mao, T.-T. (1962). Four essays on philosophy. Beijing, China: People's Press. (Original work published 1937)Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masuda, T. (1996). Bunkashinrigakutekuteki appro-ach niyoru tashakou-dousuiron process soikou: correspondence bisas no hunensei no kento. [Rethinking the inference process about the other's behavior from a cultural psychogical approach.] Kyoto, Japan: Kyoto University.Google Scholar
Masuda, T., & Nisbett, R. E. (2001). Culture and attention to object vs. field. Unpublished manuscript, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Meizlik, F. (1973). Study of the effect of sex and cultural variables on field independence/dependence in a Jewish sub-culture. Unpublished master's thesis, City University of New York.Google Scholar
Menon, T., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C.-y., & Hong, Y.-y. (1999). Culture and the construal of agency: Attribution to individual versus group dispositions. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 76, 701–717.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, D. E. (1995). Adaptive executive control: Flexible multiple-task performance without pervasive immutable response-selection bottlenecks. Acta Psychologica, 90, 163–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, D. E., & Kieras, D. E. (1997a). A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: I. Basic mechanisms. Psychological Review, 104, 3–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, D. E., & Kieras, D. E. (1997b). A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: II. Accounts of psychological refractory-period phenomena. Psychological Review, 104, 749–791.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, D. E., & Kieras, D. E. (1999). Precis to a practical unified theory of cognition and action: Some lessons from EPIC computational models of human multiple-task performance. In Gopher, D. (Ed.), Attention and performance XVIIVIII: Cognitive regulation of performance: Interaction of theory and application (Vol. 17–88). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Miller, J. G. (1984). Culture and the development of everyday social explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 961–978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, M., Nisbett, R. E., & Peng, K. (1995). Causal understanding across domains and cultures. In Sperber, D., Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (Eds.), Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 577–612). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 949–971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moser, D. J. (1996). Abstract thinking and thought in ancient Chinese and early Greek. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.Google Scholar
Munro, D. J. (1969). The concept of man in early China. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Munro, D. J. (1985). Introduction. In Munro, D. (Ed.), Individualism and holism: Studies in Confucian and Taoist values (pp. 1–34). Ann Arbor: Center for Chinese Studies, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Nagashima, N. (1973). A reversed world: Or is it? In Horton, R. & Finnegan, R. (Eds.), Modes of thought (pp. 187–213). London: Faber and Faber.Google Scholar
Nakamura, H. (1985). Ways of thinking of eastern peoples. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press. (Original work published 1964)Google Scholar
Needham, J. (1954). Science and civilisation in China (Vol. 1). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Needham, J. (1962). Science and civilisation in China. Vol. 4: Physics and physical technology. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Needham, J. (1978). The history of Chinese science and technology. Chiu-lung, China: Chung Hua Shu Chu. (Original work published 1962)Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E. (1993). Rules for reasoning. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E. (1998). Essence and accident. In Cooper, J. & Darley, J. (Eds.), Attribution processes, person perception, and social interaction: The legacy of Ned Jones (pp. 169–200). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., Fong, G. T., Lehman, D. R., & Cheng, P. W. (1987). Teaching reasoning. Science, 238, 625–631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. D. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A. (1999). Rule-based and experience-based thinking: The cognitive consequences of intellectual traditions. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A., Choi, I., & Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Eastern and western perceptions of causality for social behavior: Lay theories about personalities and social situations. In Prentice, D. & Miller, D. (Eds.), Cultural divides: Understanding and overcoming group conflict (pp. 239–272). New York: Sage.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A., Choi, I., & Nisbett, R. E. (2001). Eastern and Western folk psychology and the prediction of behavior. Unpublished manuscript, University of Illinois.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A., Nisbett, R. E., Smith, E. E., & Kim, B. J. (2000). Rules vs. similarity as a basis for reasoning and judgment in East and West. Unpublished manuscript, University of Illinois.Google Scholar
Ohbuchi, K. I., & Takahashi, Y. (1994). Cultural styles of conflict management in Japanese and Americans: Passivity, covertness, and effectiveness of strategies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 24, 1345–1366.Google Scholar
On, L. W. (1996). The cultural context for Chinese learners: Conceptions of learning in the Confucian tradition. In Watkins, D. A. & Biggs, J. B. (Eds.), The Chinese learner: Cultural, psychological, and contextual influences (pp. 142–156). Hong Kong, China: Comparative Education.Google Scholar
Osherson, D. N., Smith, E. E., Wilkie, O., Lopez, A., & Shafir, E. (1990). Category-based induction. Psychological Review, 97, 185–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, D. C., Nisbett, R. E., & Hedden, T. (1999). Culture, cognition, and aging. Journal of Gerontology, 54B, 75–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peng, K. (1997). Naive dialecticism and its effects on reasoning and judgment about contradiction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. American Psychologist, 54, 741–754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (2000). Cross-cultural similarities and differences in the understanding of physical causality. Unpublished manuscript, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Resnick, L. B. (1994). Situated rationalism: Biological and social preparation for learning. In Hirschfeld, L. A. & Gelman, S. A. (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 474–494). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riegel, K. F. (1973). Dialectical operations: The final period of cognitive development. Human Development, 18, 430–443.Google Scholar
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rogoff, B., Mistry, J., Göncü, A., & Mosier, C. (1993). Guided participation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(236).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 173–220). New York: Academic Press.
Saul, J. R. (1992). Voltaire's bastards: The dictatorship of reason in the West. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (1970). On the generality of the laws of learning. Psychological Review, 77, 127–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shore, B. (1996). Culture in mind: Cognition, culture and the problem of meaning. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A. (1991). Cultural psychology: What is it? In Shweder, R. A. (Ed.), Thinking through cultures: Expeditions in cultural psychology (pp. 73–110). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Sloman, S. (1993). Feature-based induction. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 231–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloman, S. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 30–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, E. E., Langston, C., & Nisbett, R. E. (1992). The case for rules in reasoning. Cognition, 16, 1–40.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S. (1988). Where perceiving ends and thinking begins: The apprehension of objects in infancy. In Yonas, A. (Ed.), Perceptual development in infancy. Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology (Vol. 20, pp. 191–234). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S. (1990). Principles of object perception. Cognitive Science, 14, 29–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D. (1985). Anthropology and psychology: Towards an epidemiology of representations (The Malinowski Memorial Lecture 1984). Man (N.S.), 20, 73–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Stich, S. (1990). The fragmentation of reason. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tardif, T. (1996). Nouns are not always learned before verbs: Evidence from Mandarin-speakers early vocabularies. Developmental Psychology, 32, 492–504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tardif, T., Shatz, M., & Naigles, L. (1997). Caregiver speech and children's use of nouns versus verbs: A comparison of English, Italian and Mandarin. Journal of Child Language, 24, 535–565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toulmin, S., & Goodfield, J. (1961). The fabric of the heavens: The development of astronomy and physics. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1972). The analysis of subjective culture. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Tweed, R. G., & Lehman, D. (2000). Learning considered within a cultural context: Confucian and Socratic approaches. Unpublished manuscript, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky (J. S. Bruner, Trans.). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child's theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Whiting, B. B., & Whiting, J. W. M. (1975). Children of six cultures: A psycho-cultural analysis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiting, J. W. M., & Child, I. L. (1953). Child training and personality: A cross-cultural study. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Witkin, H. A., & Berry, J. W. (1975). Psychological differentiation in cross-cultural perspective. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 6, 4–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Witkin, H. A., Dyk, R. B., Faterson, H. F., Goodenough, D. R., & Karp, S. A. (1974). Psychological differentiation. Potomac, MD: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Witkin, H. A., & Goodenough, D. R. (1977). Field dependence and interpersonal behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 661–689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Witkin, H. A., Lewis, H. B., Hertzman, M., Machover, K., Meissner, P. B., & Karp, S. A. (1954). Personality through perception. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Witkin, H. A., Price-Williams, D., Bertini, M., Christiansen, B., Oltman, P. K., Ramirez, M., & Meel, J. (1974). Social conformity and psychological differentiation. International Journal of Psychology, 9, 11–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xia, C. (1997). Decision-making factors in Go expertise. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, New Mexico State University, Las Cruces.Google Scholar
Yamagushi, S., Gelfand, M., Mizuno, M., & Zemba, Y. (1997). Illusion of collective control or illusion of personal control: Biased judgment about a chance event in Japan and the U.S. Paper presented at the Second Conference of the Asian Association of Social Psychology, Kyoto, Japan.Google Scholar
Yang, S. (1988). History of Chinese thoughts on logic. Ganshu, China: People's Press of Ganshu.Google Scholar
Yates, J. F., & Curley, S. P. (1996). Contingency judgment: Primacy effects and attention decrement. Acta Psychologica, 62, 293–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yu, Y.-s. (1985). Individualism and the Neo-Taoist movement in Wei-Chin China. In Munro, D. (Ed.), Individualism and holism: Studies in Confucian and Taoist values (pp. 121–156). Ann Arbor: Center for Chinese Studies, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Zhang, D. L. (1985). The concept of “Tian Ren He Yi” in Chinese philosophy. Beijing University Journal, 1, 8.Google Scholar
Zhang, D. L., & Chen, Z. Y. (1991). Zhongguo Siwei Pianxiang (The orientation of Chinese thinking). Beijing: Social Science Press.Google Scholar
Zhou, G. X. (1990). Chinese traditional philosophy. Beijing: Beijing Normal University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×