Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-zzh7m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T12:02:54.335Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - The continuing quest for psychological universals in categories, dimensions, taxonomies, and patterns of human behavior

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

Sevda Bekman
Affiliation:
Bogaziçi University, Istanbul
Ayhan Aksu-Koç
Affiliation:
Bogaziçi University, Istanbul
Get access

Summary

Cross-cultural psychological research has become increasingly influential. Çiğdem Kağıtçıbaşı's numerous contributions examining the human families and the explananda that can be used in that key area of understanding the rhythms and tempo of everyday life throughout the world has been exemplary. This is but one example of a multi-faceted career that we celebrate. Just as important is her friendship that we covet.

The International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology held its inaugural conference in 1972, at the University of Hong Kong. Jerome Bruner was chosen, honorifically and by acclamation, as its first president. He immediately yielded the position to Gustav Jahoda, who thus became the Association's first full-term president. Invited to discuss the presentations and reports he heard during the conference, Bruner characteristically made a number of pithy observations. Of the several comments he made – about thirty-five years ago, when many believe that “organized” cross-cultural psychology was born (see Segall et al. 1998) – one is particularly relevant for this chapter:

There has been one magnificent failure in cross-cultural studies and that is in the area of personality and culture. This is a splendid failure and good men and true have tried it. It is all premised on the idea that one can develop a cross-cultural index, pick out traits of the society and obtain some good chi squares that will tell about the kind of relationship between personality and culture. This has failed for the reason that if you were asked to quote the main finding from a period of twenty-five years of research that has studied the relationship between culture and personality, you would not only be tongue-tied but what you would say would eventually not be the same.

(Bruner 1974: 395–96)
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allik, J. and McCrae, R. R., “Toward a geography of personality traits: Patterns of profiles across 36 countries,” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35 (2004), 13–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ang, S., Dyne, L., and Koh, C., “Personality correlates of the four-factor model of cultural intelligence,” Group and Organization Management, 31 (2006), 100–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, J. W., “On cross-cultural comparability,” International Journal of Psychology, 4 (1969), 119–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, J. W., Human Ecology and Cognitive Style: Comparative Studies in Cultural and Psychological Adaptation (New York: Sage/Halsted, 1976).Google Scholar
Bond, M. H., “The third stage of cross-cultural psychology: Some personal Prescriptions for our future,” paper presented at the Seventeenth International Congress of the International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology. Xi'an, China, 2004a.
Bond, M. H., “Culture-level dimensions of social axioms and their correlates across 41 countries,”Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35 (2004b), 548–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J., “Concluding comments and summary of conference,” in Dawson, J. M. and Lonner, W. J. (eds.), Readings in Cross-cultural Psychology. Proceedings of the Inaugural Meeting of the International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology (Hong Kong: Hong Kong University Press, 1974), pp. 392–97.Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T.. and Fiske, D. W., “Convergent and discriminant validity by the multitrait–multimethod matrix,”Psychological Bulletin, 56 (1959), 81–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Georgas, J., Vijver, F. J. R., and Berry, J. W., “The ecocultural framework ecocultural indices, and psychological variables in cross-cultural research,”Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35 (2004), 74–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goleman, D., Social Intelligence: The New Science of Human Relationships (New York: Bantam, 2006).Google Scholar
Headland, T. N., Pike, K. L., and Harris, M., Emics and Etics: The Insider–outsider Debate (Newbury Park, CA: Sage, 1990).Google Scholar
Hofstede, G., Culture's Consequences: International Differences in Work-related Values (Beverly Hills, CA: Sage, 1980).Google Scholar
Hofstede, G., Culture's Consequences: Comparing Values, Behaviors, Institutions and Organizations across Nations (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 2001).Google Scholar
Kluckhohn, C. and Murray, H. A., Personality in Nature, Society, and Culture (New York: Alfred Knopf, 1949).Google Scholar
Kluckhohn, C. and Strodtbeck, F., Variations in Value Orientations (Evanston, Illinois: Row-Peterson, 1961).Google Scholar
Koltko-Rivera, M. E., “The psychology of worldviews,”Review of General Psychology, 8 (2004), 3–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leung, K., Bond, M., Carrasquel, S., Muñoz, C., Hernandez, M., Murakami, F., Yamaguchi, S., Bierbrauer, G., and Singelis, T. M., “Social axioms: The search for universal dimensions of general beliefs about how the world functions,”Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33 (2002), 286–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lonner, W. J., “An analysis of the prepublication evaluation of cross-cultural manuscripts: Implications for future research,” in Brislin, R. W., Bochner, S., and Lonner, W. J. (eds.), Cross-cultural Perspectives on Learning (New York: Halstead/Wiley, 1975), pp. 305–20.Google Scholar
Lonner, W. J., “The search for psychological universals,” in Triandis, H. C. and Lambert, W. W. (eds.), Handbook of Cross-cultural Psychology. Volume 1. Perspectives (Boston: Allyn & Bacon, 1980), pp. 143–204.Google Scholar
Lonner, W. J. and Hayes, S. A., Discovering Cultural Psychology: A Profile and Selected Readings of Ernest E. Boesch (Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing, 2007).Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D. and Yoo, S. H., “Toward a new generation of cross-cultural research,”Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(2006), 234–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCrae, R. R. and Costa, P. T., Personality in Adulthood (New York: Guilford, 1990).Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Pilar, G. H., Roland, J.-P., and Parker, W. D., “Cross-cultural assessment of the Five-Factor Model: The revised NEO Personality Inventory,”Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29 (1998), 171–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rotter, J. B., “Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement,”Psychological Monographs, 80 (1966), 1–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, S. H., “Cultural value differences: Some implications for work,”Applied Psychology: An International Review, 48 (1999), 23–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, S. H., “Mapping and interpreting cultural differences around the world,” in Vinken, H., Soeters, J., and Ester, P. (eds.), Comparing Cultures: Dimensions of Culture in a Comparative Perspective (Leiden, The Netherlands: Brill, 2004), pp. 43–73.Google Scholar
Schwartz, S. H., “A theory of cultural value orientations: Explication and applications,”Comparative Sociology, 5 (2006), 137–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, S., and Bilsky, J., “Towards a psychological structure of human values,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53 (1987), 550–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segall, M. H., Lonner, W. J., and Berry, J. W., “Cross-cultural psychology as a scholarly discipline: On the flowering of culture in behavioral research,”American Psychologist, 54(10) (1998), 1101–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, P. B., “Nations, cultures, and individuals: New perspectives and old dilemmas,” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35 (2004), 6–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, P. B. and Schwartz, S. H., “Values,” in Berry, J. W., Segall, M. H., and Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç. (eds.), Handbook of Cross-cultural Psychology. Volume 3. Social Behavior and Applications, Second edition (Boston: Allyn & Bacon, 1997), pp. 77–118.Google Scholar
Smith, P. B., Bond, M. H., and Ç. Kağıtçıbaşı, Understanding Social Behavior across Cultures: Living and Working in a Changing World (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 2006).Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C., “The psychological measurement of cultural syndromes,”American Psychologist, 51(4) (1996), 407–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×