Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
  • Cited by 34
Publisher:
Cambridge University Press
Online publication date:
September 2014
Print publication year:
2014
Online ISBN:
9781139424608

Book description

Human possession psychology originates from deeply rooted experiential capacities shared with other animals. However, unlike other animals, we are a uniquely self-conscious species concerned with reputation, and possessions affect our perception of how we exist in the eyes of others. This book discusses the psychology surrounding the ways in which humans experience possession, claim ownership, and share from both a developmental and cross-cultural perspective. Philippe Rochat explores the origins of human possession and its symbolic development across cultures. He proposes that human possession psychology is particularly revealing of human nature, and also the source of our elusive moral sense.

Reviews

‘Throughout his work, Philippe Rochat attempts to identify what is uniquely human. In this book he yet again moves one step closer to a truly intellectually satisfying description. Origins of Possession is essential reading for all those of us who are in the profession of working with the human mind, particularly when it is troubled and is at risk of losing its essential qualities of self-awareness and symbolization. It is a highly significant contribution which builds on Rochat's unparalleled depth of understanding of child development to deliver a view of the nature and experience of possession that transcends development and individual differences - and illuminates both - whilst striking very close to what lies at the core of our experience of ourselves and of others as sentient beings.'

Peter Fonagy - Freud Memorial Professor of Psychoanalysis and Head, Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London

‘This is a rare, unique, and exciting book that could not have been written except by a scientist who, like Darwin, has travelled to diverse places and made the insightful observations that open a new perspective on everything of concern to human psychology. Rochat probes a concept so central and pervasive that it is commonplace: possession. He develops a psychology that penetrates deeply into philosophical and ethical realms, and that raises questions that to my knowledge have not been raised before about issues relevant to the development of the most important aspects of social life, issues that pertain to something so basic that it drives sex and violence and has the potential to realign our understanding of economics, law and social formation, as well as individual existence.'

Shaun Gallagher - Lillian and Morrie Moss Professor of Excellence, University of Memphis

‘Philippe Rochat has been studying children across cultures with a unique combination of curiosity, empathy, and insight. This important book challenges our understanding of their psychology - and of human psychology generally - and opens new directions of research. A highly pleasurable and stimulating read!'

Dan Sperber - author of Explaining Culture, and co-author of Meaning and Relevance

‘One might think that the topic of possession is clearly delimited and somewhat specialized. In this masterful work Rochat shows that nothing could be farther from the truth. Possession, as Rochat conceives it, is an integral part of all of the most important aspects of human experience. A most enlightening piece of work - and a joy to read.'

Michael Tomasello - Co-Director, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology

'Relying on Rousseau’s 'Second Discourse', Rochat shows how the idea of property led humans to abandon a natural state of sharing with all other animals to a state in which claims of ownership, combined with the acceptance of such claims by others, set a new standard for civil society based on possession. Rochat creates a fascinating discourse by integrating ideas from philosophy, biology, sociology, and anthropology to understand the development of laws of property in Western and non-Western cultures and the impact of these cultural systems on the psychological development of a sense of possession across the life span … Summing up: highly recommended. Upper-division undergraduates through faculty and professionals.'

R. B. Stewart, Jr Source: Choice

Refine List

Actions for selected content:

Select all | Deselect all
  • View selected items
  • Export citations
  • Download PDF (zip)
  • Save to Kindle
  • Save to Dropbox
  • Save to Google Drive

Save Search

You can save your searches here and later view and run them again in "My saved searches".

Please provide a title, maximum of 40 characters.
×

Contents

References

Addessi, E., Crescimbene, L., and Visalberghi, E. (2007). Do capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) use tokens as symbols?Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274(1625), 2579–2585.
Agnetta, B. and Rochat, P. (2004). Imitative games by 9-, 14-, and 18-month-old infants. Infancy, 6(1), 1–36.
Ainsworth, M. D. (1967). Infancy in Uganda. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Ainsworth, M. and Bowlby, J. (1965). Child Care and the Growth of Love. London: Penguin Books.
Ainsworth, M. D., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., and Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of Attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., and Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of Attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Alsaker, F. D. and Valkanover, S. (2001). Early diagnosis and prevention of victimization in kindergarten. In J. Juvonen and S. Graham (eds.), Peer Harassment in School: The Plight of the Vulnerable and Victimized (pp. 175–195). New York: Guilford Press.
Anand, K. J. S. and Hickey, P. R. (1987). Pain and its effects in the human neonate and fetus. New England Journal of Medicine, 317, 1321–1329.
Andonotopo, W. and Kurjak, A. (2006). The assessment of fetal behavior of growth restricted fetuses by 4D sonography. Journal of Perinatal Medicine, 34, 471–478.
Andonotopo, W., Stanojevic, M., Kurjak, A., Azumendi, G., and Carrera, J. M. (2004). Assessment of fetal behavior and general movements by four-dimensional sonography. Ultrasound Review of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 4, 103–114.
Aneci Rosa, C., Rute, S., de Sousa, E., Borba, R., and Ebrahim, G. J. (1992). The street children of Recife: a study of their background. Journal of Tropical Pediatrics, 38, 34–40.
Arnott, G. and Elwood, R. W. (2007). Fighting for shells: how private information about resource value changes hermit crab pre-fight displays and escalated fight behavior. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274(1628), 3011–3017.
Asma, S. T. (2012). Against Fairness. University of Chicago Press.
Atran, S. (2003). Genesis of suicide terrorism. Science, 299, 1534–1539.
Augé, M. (1994). Le sens des autres. Paris: Fayard.
Bakeman, R. and Adamson, L. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother–infant and peer–infant interactions. Child Development, 55, 1278–1289.
Banks, M. S. and Shannon, E. S. (1993). Spatial and chromatic visual efficiency in human neonates. In C. E. Granrud (ed.), Carnegie-Mellon Symposium on Cognitive Psychology (pp. 1–46). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Barclay, B. (2005). Manatuturu: Maori Treasures and Intellectual Property Rights. Auckland University Press.
Barsalou, L. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660.
Bates, E. (1990). Language about me and you: pronominal reference and the emerging concept of self. In D. Cicchetti and M. Beeghly (eds.), The Self in Transition: Infancy to Childhood (pp. 165–182). University of Chicago Press.
Bauer, P. (2006). Remembering Times of our Lives: Memory in Infancy and Beyond. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Baumard, N., André, J-B., and Sperber, D. (2013). A mutualistic approach to morality: the evolution of fairness by partner choice. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 59–122.
Belk, R. W. (1978). Assessing the effects of visible consumption on impression formation. Advances in Consumer Research, 5, 39–47.
Belk, R. W. (1991). The ineluctable mysteries of possessions. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 6(6), 17–55.
Belk, R. W. (1995). Collecting in a Consumer Society. New York: Routledge.
Belk, R. W. (1999). Shoes and self. Advances in Consumer Research, 30, 27–33.
Belk, R. W. (2000). Are we what we own? In A. L. Benson (ed.), I Shop, Therefore I Am: Compulsive Buying and the Search for Self (pp. 76–104). Oxford: Rowman and Littlefield.
Benedict, R. (1946). The Chrysanthemum and the Sword. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Benson, A. L. (ed.) (2000). I Shop, Therefore I Am: Compulsive Buying and the Search for Self. Oxford: Rowman and Littlefield.
Benson, J. B. (1993). Season of birth and onset of locomotion: theoretical and methodological implications. Infant Behavior and Development, 16(1), 69–81.
Berger, K. (2007). Update on bullying at school: science forgotten?Developmental Review, 27, 90–126.
Bertenthal, B. and Campos, J. J. (1990). A systems approach to the organizing effects of self-produced locomotion during infancy. In C. Rovee-Collier and L. P. Lipsitt (eds.), Advances in Infancy Research (vol. 6, pp. 1–60). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corp.
Binet, A. (1887). Du fétichisme dans l’amour (Fetishism in love). Revue Philosophique, 24, 143–167.
Birch, L. L. and Billman, J. (1986). Preschool children’s food sharing with friends and acquaintances. Child Development, 57, 387–395.
Blake, P. and Harris, P. L. (2009). Children’s understanding of ownership transfers. Cognitive Development, 24, 133–145.
Blake, P. and Harris, P. L. (2011). Early representations of ownership. In H. Ross and O. Friedman (eds.), Origins of Ownership of Property. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 132, 39–51.
Block, N. (2007). Consciousness, accessibility, and the mesh between psychology and neuroscience. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 30, 481–548.
Bourdieu, P. (1984). Distinction: A Social Critique of the Judgement of Taste. New York: Routledge.
Bourdieu, P. (1985). The social space and the genesis of groups. Theory and Society, 14(6), 723–744.
Bourdieu, P. (1986). The forms of capital. In J. Richardson (ed.), Handbook of Theory and Research for the Sociology of Education (pp. 241–258). New York: Greenwood.
Bourdieu, P. (1989). Social space and symbolic power. Sociological Theory, 7(1), 14–25.
Bourdieu, P. (1990). The Logic of Practice. Cambridge: Polity Press.
Bourdieu, P. (1994). Raisons pratiques (Practical Reasons). Paris: Editions du Seuil.
Bourdieu, P. (1997/2003). Méditations pascaliennes (Pascalian Meditations, French edition, author’s translation). Paris: Editions du Seuil.
Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and Loss. New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1988). A Secure Base: Parent–Child Attachment and Healthy Human Development. New York: Basic Books.
Breuer, J. and Freud, S. (2000). Studies on Hysteria. New York: Basic Books, first published in German in 1895.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The Ecology of Human Development: Experiments by Nature and Design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Brosnan, S. and DeWaal, F. (2001). A proximate perspective on reciprocal altruism. Human Nature, 13(1), 129–152.
Bruner, J. S. (1969). Eye, hand and mind. In D. Elkind and J. H. Flavell (eds.), Studies in Cognitive Development: Essays in Honor of Jean Piaget (pp. 223–236). Oxford University Press.
Bruner, J. S. (1972). Nature and uses of immaturity. American Psychologist, 27(8), 687–708.
Bruner, J. S. (1983). Child’s Talk. New York: Norton.
Brusseau, R. R. and Mashour, G. A. (2007). Subcortical consciousness: implications for fetal anesthesia and analgesia. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(1), 86–87.
Caillois, R. (1958/2001). Man, Play, and Games. Urbana-Champaign: University of Illinois Press.
Callaghan, T., Rochat, P., Lillard, A., Claux, M. L., Odden, H., Itakura, S., Tapanya, S., and Singh, S. (2005). Synchrony in the onset of mental-state reasoning: evidence from five cultures. Psychological Science, 16(5), 378–384.
Carey, S. (2011). The origin of concepts: a précis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34, 113–167.
Carlson, B. M. (1994). Human Embryology and Developmental Biology. St. Louis: Mosby.
Carnevalli, B. (2011). Romantisme et reconnaissance: figures de la conscience chez Rousseau (Romantism and Recognition: Rousseau and the Figures of Consciousness). Geneva: Droz Publishers, Collection Bibliothèque des Lumières.
Carnevalli, B. (2013). “Glory”: la lutte pour la réputation dans le modèle hobbesien (“Glory”: the struggle for reputation in the Hobbesian model). Communications, Special issue on reputation, ed. G. Origgi.
Carpenter, M., Tomasello, M., and Striano, T. (2005). Role reversal imitation and language in typically developing infants and children with autism. Infancy, 8, 253–278.
Casanova, G. (1797/2006). History of My Life. New York: Everyman’s Library.
Charles, S. and Lipovetsky, G. (2006). Hypermodern Times. Cambridge: Polity Press.
Chen, X., Rubin, K. H., and Sun, Y. (1992). Social reputation and peer relationships in Chinese and Canadian children: a cross-cultural study. Child Development, 63, 1336–1343.
Clark, G. (2007). Specialisation, standardisation and Lapita ceramics. In S. Bedford, C. Sand, and S. P. Connaughton (eds.), Oceanic Explorations: Lapita and Western Pacific Settlement (pp. 289–296). Terra Australis 26. Australian National University.
Cohen, D. and Nisbett, R. E. (1994). Self-protection and the culture of honor: explaining southern violence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 551–567.
Cohen, D. and Nisbett, R. E. (1997). Field experiments examining the culture of honor: the role of institutions in perpetuating norms about violence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 1188–1199.
Cole, D. T. (1955). An Introduction of Tswana Grammar. Parktown: Longman Penguin South Africa.
Cole, M. and Hatano, G. (2007). Cultural-historical activity theory: integrating phylogeny, cultural history, and ontogenesis in cultural psychology. In S. Kitayama and D. Cohen (eds.), Handbook of Cultural Psychology (pp. 109–135). New York: Guilford Press.
Cyencek, D., Greenwald, A., and Meltzoff, A. (2011). Measuring implicit attitudes of 4-year old children: the preschool implicit association test. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109, 187–200.
Darwin, C. (1872/1965). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. University of Chicago Press.
Deacon, T. W. (1997). The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain. New York: Norton.
Debord, G. (1977/1967). The Society of the Spectacle. St. Petersburg, FL: Black and Red Publishers.
DeCasper, A. J. and Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: newborns prefer their mother’s voices. Science, 208, 1174–1176.
Dennett, D. C. (1981). Brainstorms: Philosophical Essays on Mind and Psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Dennett, D. C. (1987). The Intentional Stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
De Staël, Madame (1799). De la littérature considérée dans ses rapports avec les institutions sociales (On literature considered in relation to social institutions). Paris: Nabu.
deVries, J. I. P., Visser, G. H. A., and Prechtl, H. F. R. (1985). The emergence of fetal behavior. II. Quantitative aspects. Early Human Development, 12, 99–120.
De Waal, F. B. M. (1989). Food sharing and reciprocal obligations among chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 18, 433–459.
De Waal, F. B. M. (1997). The chimpanzee’s service economy: food for grooming. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18, 375–386.
De Waal, F. B. M. (2006). Primates and Philosophers: How Morality Evolved. Princeton University Press.
Dewey, J. (1922). Human Nature and Conduct: An Introduction to Social Psychology. New York: Carlton House.
Dittmar, H. (1992). The Social Psychology of Material Possessions: To Have Is to Be. New York: St. Martin’s Press.
Dittmar, H. (2000). The role of self-image in excessive buying. In A. L. Benson (ed.), I Shop, Therefore I Am: Compulsive Buying and the Search for Self (pp. 105–132). New York: Rowman and Littlefield.
Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the Modern Mind: Three Stages in the Evolution of Culture and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Dubreuil, D., Gentile, M. S., Visalberghi, E. (2008). Are capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) inequity averse?Animal Cognition, 11(2), 275–282.
Dunham, Y., Baron, A., and Banaji, M. (2008). The development of implicit intergroup cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(7), 248–253.
Dunn, J. (1988). The Beginnings of Social Understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Durkheim, E. (1893/1997). The Division of Labor in Society. New York: Free Press.
Ehrlich, P. R., Dobkin, D. S., Wheye, D., Pimm, S. L. (1994). The Birdwatcher’s Handbook (pp. 228–232). Oxford University Press.
Ekecrantz, L. and Rudhe, L. (1972). Transitional phenomena: frequency, forms and functions of specially loved objects. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavia, 48, 261–273.
Emery, N. J. (2000). The eyes have it: the neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 581–604.
Erikson, E. H. (1993). Childhood and Society. New York: W. W. Norton.
Faigenbaum, G. (2005). Children’s Economic Experience: Exchange. Buenos Aires: Libros En Red.
Faigenbaum, G. (2013). The underground economy of schoolyards (manuscript submitted).
Fair, D. A., Bathula, D., Mills, K. L., Costa Dias, T. G., Blythe, M. S., Zhang, D., Snyder, A. Z., Raichle, M. E., Stevens, A. A., Nigg, J. T., and Nagell, B. J. (2010). Maturing thalamocortical functional connectivity across development. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 4(10), 1–10.
Fehr, E., Bernhard, H., and Rockenbach, B. (2008). Egalitarianism in young children. Nature, 454, 1079–1084.
Field, T. M. et al. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by neonates. Science, 218(4568), 179–181.
Fiske, A. P. (1992). The four elementary forms of sociality: framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review, 99(4), 689–723.
Fonagy, P., Target, M., and Gergely, G. (2006). Psychoanalytic perspectives on developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti and D. J. Cohen (ed.), Developmental Psychopathology (2nd edn., pp. 701–749). New York: Wiley.
Freud, S. (1905/2000). Three Essays on the Theory of Sexuality. New York: Basic Books.
Friedman, O. (2008). First possession: an assumption guiding inferences about who owns what. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 15, 290–295.
Friedman, O. and Leslie, A. M. (2007). The conceptual underpinnings of pretense: pretending is not “behaving-as-if.” Cognition, 105, 103–124.
Friedman, O. and Neary, K. R. (2008). Determining who owns what: do children infer ownership from first possession?Cognition, 107, 829–849.
Friedman, O., Neary, K. R., Defeyter, M. A., and Malcolm, S. L. (2011). Ownership and object history. In H. Ross and O. Friedman (eds.), Origins of Ownership of Property. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 132, 79–89.
Furby, L. (1978). Possession in humans: an exploratory study of its meaning and motivation. Social Behavior and Personality 6(1), 49–65.
Gabalda, B. (2012). Development of the sense of ownership: social and moral evaluations. Doctoral dissertation, Université René Descartes Paris.
Gaddini, R. and Gaddini, E. (1970). Transitional objects and the process of individuation: study in three different social groups. Journal of American Academic Child Psychiatry, 9, 347–365.
Gallagher, S. and Zahavi, D. (2008). The Phenomenological Mind: An Introduction to Philosophy of Mind and Cognitive Science. New York: Routledge.
Gazelle, H. and Ladd, G. W. (2003). Anxious solitude and peer exclusion: a diathesis stress model of internalizing trajectories in childhood. Child Development, 74, 257–278.
Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., and Kiraly, I. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415, 755.
Gergely, G. and Watson, J. S. (1999). Early socio-emotional development: contingency perception and the social-biofeedback model. In P. Rochat (ed.), Early Social Cognition: Understanding Others in the First Months of Life (pp. 101–136). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gibbon, E. (1776/2001). The History of the Decline and Fall of the Roman Empire. London: Penguin.
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.
Godbout, J. (1998). The World of the Gift. Montreal: McGill-Queen’s University Press.
Goffman, E. (1959). The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life. New York: Doubleday.
Gomes, C. M. and Boesch, C. (2009). Wild chimpanzees exchange meat for sex on a long-term basis. PLoSONE, 4, e5116.
Gomes, C. M., Mundry, R., and Boesch, C. (2009). Long-term reciprocation of grooming in wild West African chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276 (1657), 699–706.
Gottlieb, G. (1971). Ontogenesis of sensory functions in birds and mammals. In A. Tobach, L. R. Aronson, and E. Shaw (eds.), The Biopsychology of Development (pp. 67–128). New York: Academic Press.
Gould, S. J. (1977). Ontogeny and Phylogeny. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Graeber, D. (2011). Debt: The First 5000 Years. New York: Melville House.
Granrud, C. E. (1987). Size constancy in newborn human infants. Investigative Ophthamology and Visual Science, 28 (Supplement), 5.
Groos, K. (1898). The Play of Animals. New York: Appleton.
Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K. E., Spangler, G., Suess, G., and Unzner, L. (1985). Maternal sensitivity and newborns orientation responses as related to quality of attachment in northern Germany: growing points in attachment theory and research. Child Development, 50, 233–256.
Grossmann, K. E., Grossmann, K., Huber, F., Wartner, U.et al. (1981). German children’s behavior toward their mothers at 12 months and their fathers at 18 months in Ainsworth’s strange situation. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 4, 157–184.
Haith, M. M., Bergman, T., and Moore, M. J. (1977). Eye contact and face scanning in early infancy. Science, 198, 853–855.
Hamilton, W. (1963) The evolution of altruistic behavior. American Naturalist, 97, 354–356.
Hansell, M. (2000). Bird Nests and Construction Behaviour. Cambridge University Press.
Harbaugh, W. T., Krause, K. S., and Liday, S. J. (2003). Bargaining by children. Social Science Research Network, 1–40.
Hardin, G. (1968). The tragedy of the commons. Science, 162(3859), 1243–1248.
Harkness, S. and Super, C. M. (eds.) (1996). Parents’ Cultural Belief Systems: Their Origins, Expressions, and Consequences. New York: Guilford Press.
Harlow, H. F. (1958). The nature of love. American Psychologist, 13, 673–685.
Harris, P. L. and Kavanaugh, R. D. (1993). Young Children’s Understanding of Pretense. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58, 1.
Hart, S. and Carrington, H. (2002). Jealousy in 6-month-old infants. Infancy, 3(3), 395–402.
Hata, T., Dai, S. Y., and Marumo, G. (2010). Ultrasound for evaluation of fetal neurobehavioural development: from 2-D to 4-D ultrasound. Infant and Child Development, 19(1), 99–118. Special issue: Towards a fetal psychology.
Haun, D. and Tomasello, M. (2011). Conformity to peer pressure in preschool children. Child Development, 82(6), 1759–1767.
Haye, D. F. (2006). Yours and mine: toddlers talk about possessions with familiar peers. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 39–52.
Hebdige, D. (1994). Hiding in the Light. London: Routledge.
Heider, F. and Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57(2), 243–254.
Heine, B. (1997). Possession: Cognitive Sources, Forces, and Grammaticalization (pp. 1–44). Cambridge University Press.
Heller, M. C. (2012). Body Psychotherapy: History, Concepts and Methods. New York: Norton.
Henrich, J., Ensimger, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., Cardenas, J. C., Gurven, M., Gwako, E., Henrich, N., Lesorogol, C., Marlowe, F., Tracer, D., and J. Ziker (2010). Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science, 327, 1480–1484.
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., and Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world?Behavioral and Brain Science, 33, 1–75.
Henrich, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., Cardenas, J. C., Gurven, M., Gwako, E., Henrich, N., Lesorogol, C., Marlowe, F., Tracer, D., and Ziker, J. (2006). Costly punishment across human societies. Science, 312(5781), 1767–1770.
Hepper, P. G. (2002). Prenatal development. In A. Slater and M. Lewis (eds.), Introduction to Infant Development (pp. 39–60). Oxford University Press.
Hepper, P. G. and Shahidullah, S. (1994). Development of fetal hearing. Fetal and Maternal Medicine Review, 6, 167–179.
Hewlett, B., Lamb, M., Shannon, D., Leyendecker, P., and Schölmerich, A. (1998). Culture and early infancy among Central African foragers and farmers. Developmental Psychology, 34(4), 653–661.
Hinde, R. (1966). Animal Behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Hobara, M. (2003). Prevalence of transitional objects in young children of Tokyo and New York. Infant Mental Health Journal, 24(2), 174–191.
Hobson, R. P. (1989). On sharing experiences. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 97–203.
Hobson, R. P. (1995). Apprehending attitudes and actions: separable abilities in early development?Development and Psychopathology, 7, 171–82.
Hobson, R. P. (2002). The Cradle of Thought. London: Macmillan.
Hofeld, W. H. (1913). Some fundamental legal conceptions as applied to judicial reasoning. Yale Law Journal, 23, 16–59.
Hooker, D. (1952). The Prenatal Origin of Behavior. University of Kansas Press.
Huizinga, J. (1938/1955). Homo Ludens: A Study of the Play Element in Culture. Boston: Beacon Press.
Hume, D. (1739/1928). A Treatise of Human Nature. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Hustvedt, S. (2005). Mysteries of the Rectangle: Essays on Painting. Princeton Architectural Press.
Huttenlocher, P. R. and Dabholkar, A. S. (1997). Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 387(2), 167–178.
Ingold, T., Riches, D., and Woodburn, J. (eds.) (1988). Hunters and Gatherers, vol. 1: History, Evolution and Social Changes; vol. 2: Property, Power and Ideology. Oxford: Berg.
Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C., and Jasnow, M. D. (2001). Rhythms of dialogue in infancy. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 66(2), Serial No. 265.
James, W. (1884). What is an emotion?Mind, 9, 188–205.
James, W. (1890/1950). The Principles of Psychology. New York: Dover.
Jersky, J. and Kinsley, R. H. (1967). Lactose deficiency in the South African Bantu. South African Medical Journal, 41(Dec.), 1194–1196.
Kagan, J. (1981). The Second Year: The Emergence of Self-awareness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Kanngiesser, P., Gjersoe, N., and Hood, B. (2010). The effect of creative labor on property-ownership transfer by preschool children and adults. Psychological Science, 21, 1236–1241.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1996). Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Keller, H. (2007). Cultures of Infancy. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Keller, H. (2013). Attachment and culture. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 44(2), 175–194.
Kellman, P. J. and Arterberry, M. E. (2006). Infant visual perception. In D. Kuhn, R. S. Siegler, W. Damon, and R. M. Lerner (eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology, vol. 2: Cognition, Perception, and Language (6th edn., pp. 109–160). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Kent, M. M. and Haub, C. (2005). Global demographic divide. Population Bulletin, 60(4), 1–24.
Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., and Spelke, E. S. (2007). The native language of social cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(30), 12577–12580.
Kinzler, K. D., Shutts, K., DeJesus, J., and Spelke, E. S. (2009). Accent trumps race in guiding children’s social preferences. Social Cognition, 27, 623–634.
Kisilevsky, B. S. and Hains, S. M. J. (2010). Exploring the relationship between fetal heart rate and cognition. Infant and Child Development, 19(1), 60–75. Special issue: Towards a Fetal Psychology.
Klein, Melanie (1932). The Psycho-analysis of Children. London: Hogarth Press.
Klein, R. G. (1999). The Human Career: Human Biological and Cultural Origins (2nd edn.). University of Chicago Press.
Klimach, V. J. and Cooke, R. W. (1988). Maturation of the neonatal somatosensory evoked response in preterm infants. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 30(2), 208–214.
Klopfer, P. H. (1969). Habitats and Territories: A Study of the Use of Space by Animals. New York: Basic Books.
Kobayashi, H. and Hashiya, K. (2011). The gaze that grooms: contribution of social factors to the evolution of primate eye morphology. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32, 157–165.
Kobayashi, H. and Kohshima, S. (1997). Unique morphology of the human eye. Nature, 387, 767–768.
Konner, M. (2010). The Evolution of Childhood. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Kretschmann, H. J., Kammradt, G., Krauthausen, I., Sauer, B., and Wingert, F. (1986). Brain growth in man. Bibliotheca Anatomica, 28, 1–26.
Kummer, H. (1971). Primate Societies: Group Techniques of Ecological Adaptation. Chicago: Aldine-Atherton.
Lakin, M., Lakin, M. G., and Costanzo, P. R. (1979). Group processes in early childhood: a dimension of human development. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2(2), 171–183.
Lamb, M. E., Sternberg, K. J., Broberg, A. G., and Hwang, P. (1992). Child Care in Context: Cross-Cultural Perspectives. Erlbaum.
Lancy, D. F. (1996). Playing on the Mother Ground: Cultural Routines for Children’s Development. New York: Guilford.
Lancy, D. F. (2008). The Anthropology of Childhood: Cherubs, Chattel, Changelings. Cambridge University Press.
Lecanuet, J.-P., Granier-Deferre, C., and Busnel, M.-C. (1988). Fetal cardiac and motor responses to octave-band noises as a function of central frequency, intensity and heartrate variability. Early Human Development, 18, 81–93.
Lecanuet, J.-P., Granier-Deferre, C., DeCasper, A. J., Maugeais, R., Andrieu, A.-J., and Busnel, M.-C. (1987). Perception et discrimination foetales de stimuli langagiers, mise en évidence à partir de la réactivité cardiaque, résultants préliminaires. Compte-Rendus de l’Académie des Sciences, Paris (III), 305, 161–164.
Lecanuet, J. P. and Schaal, B. (1996). Fetal sensory compentencies. European Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology and Reproductive Biology, 68, 1–23.
Lehman, E. B., Denham, S. A., Moser, M. H., and Reeves, S. L. (1992). Soft object and pacifier attachment in young children: the role of security attachment to the mother. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 33, 1205–1215.
Leroi-Gourhan, A. (1993). Gesture and Speech. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: the origins of “theory of mind.”Psychological Review, 94, 412–426.
LeVine, R. A. (1994). Child Care and Culture: Lessons from Africa. Cambridge University Press.
Lewis, M. (1992). Shame: The Exposed Self. New York: Free Press.
Lewis, M. and Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social Cognition and the Acquisition of Self. New York: Plenum Press.
Lewis, M., Sullivan, M., Stanger, C., and Weiss, M. (1989). Self development and self-conscious emotions. Child Development, 60, 146–156.
Lipovetsky, G. (2005). Hypermodern Times. Cambridge: Polity Press.
Litt, C. J. (1981). Children’s attachment to transitional objects: a study of two pediatric populations. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 51, 131–139.
Litt, C. J. (1986). Theories of transitional object attachment: an overview. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 9(3), 383–399.
Lizardo, O. (2004). The cognitive origins of Bourdieu’s habitus. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 34, 375, 401.
Lizardo, O. (2013). Habitus. In Byron Kaldis (ed.), Encyclopedia of Philosophy and the Social Sciences. London: Sage Publications.
Locke, J. (1689/1997). An Essay Concerning Human Understanding. Ed. Roger Woolhouse. New York: Penguin Books.
Locke, J. (1690/1980). The Second Treatise of Civil Government. Cambridge: Hackett.
Malinowski, B. (1932). Argonauts of the Western Pacific: An Account of Native Enterprise and Adventure in the Archipelagoes of Melanesian New Guinea. London: Routledge.
Malinowski, B. and Ellis, H. (1929). The Sexual Life of Savages in North-Western Melanesia: An Ethnographic Account of Courtship, Marriage, and Family Life among the Natives of the Trobriand Islands, British New Guinea. London: Routledge.
Marcuse, H. (1955/1987). Eros and Civilization (2nd edn.). London: Routledge.
Marlier, L., Schaal, B., and Soussignan, R. (1998a). Neonatal responsiveness to the odor of amniotic and lacteal fluids: a test of perinatal chemosensory continuity. Child Development, 69(3), 611–623.
Marlier, L. (1998b). Bottle-fed neonates prefer an odor experienced in utero to an odor experienced postnatally in the feeding context. Developmental Psychobiology, 33, 133–145.
Marlowe, F. W. (2010). The Hadza: Hunter-Gatherers of Tanzania. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Marr, D. (1982/2010). Vision: A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Marshall, L. (1961). Sharing, talking, and giving: relief of social tensions among ǃKung Bushmen. Africa: Journal of the International African Institute, 31(3), 231–249.
Marx, K. and Engels, F. (1888/1998). The Communist Manifesto. New York: Penguin.
Mauss, M. (1934). Le notion de technique du corps. Journal de Psychologie, 32, 3–4, reprinted in Sociologie et Anthropologie. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
Mauss, M. (1952/1967). The Gift: Forms and Functions of Exchange in Archaic Societies. New York: Norton.
Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838–850.
Meltzoff, A. N. and Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures. Science, 198(4312), 75–78.
Meltzoff, A. N. and Moore, M. K. (1992). Early imitation within a functional framework: the importance of person identity, movement, and development. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 479–505.
Merker, B. (2007). Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: a challenge for neuroscience and medicine. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 63–134.
Merskey, H. and Bogduk, N. (eds.) (1994). Classification of Chronic Pain: Descriptions of Chronic Pain Syndromes and Definitions of Pain Terms (pp. 209–214). New York: IASP Press.
Metzinger, T. (2003). Being No One: The Self-Model Theory of Subjectivity. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Michotte, A. (1946/1962). The Perception of Causality. Andover, MA: Methuen.
Miller, J. G. (2006). Insight into moral development from cultural psychology. In M. Killen and J. Smetana (eds.), Handbook of Moral Development (pp. 375–398). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Mitani, J. C. and Watts, D. P. (2001). Why do chimpanzees hunt and share meat?Animal Behaviour, 61(5), 915–924.
Moessinger, P. (2000a). Le jeu de l’identité. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
Moessinger, P. (2000b). The Paradox of Social Order: Linking Psychology and Sociology. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Montagu, A. (1961). Neonatal and infant immaturity in man. Journal of the American Medical Association, 178(23), 56–57.
Morin, O. (2011). Comment les traditions naissent et meurent: la transmission culturelle (How traditions arise and die out: cultural transmission). Paris: Odile Jacob.
Morton, J. and Johnson, M. H. (1991). CONSPEC and CONLERN: a two-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review, 98(2), 164–181.
Murnighan, J. K. and Saxon, M. S. (1998). Ultimatum bargaining by children and adults. Journal of Economic Psychology, 19(4), 415–445.
Nijhuis, J. G., Prechtl, H. F. R., Martin, C. B., and Bots, R. (1982). Are there behavioral states in the human fetus?Early Human Development, 6, 177–195.
Nisbett, R. E. (2003). The Geography of Thought: How Asians and Westerners Think Differently … and Why. New York: The Free Press.
Nisbett, R. E. and Cohen, D. (1996). Culture of Honor: The Psychology of Violence in the South. Denver, CO: Westview Press.
Noles, N. S., Keil, F. C., Bloom, P., and Gelman, S. A. (2012). Children’s and adults’ intuitions about who can own things. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 12, 265–286.
Nordgreen, J., Joseph, P., Garner, J. P., Janczak, A. M., Ranheim, B., Muir, W. M., and Horsberg, T. E. (2009). Thermonociception in fish: effects of two different doses of morphine on thermal threshold and post-test behaviour in goldfish (Carassius auratus). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 119(1–2), 101–107.
Nozick, R. (1974). Anarchy, State, and Utopia. New York: Basic Books.
Odden, H. and Rochat, P. (2004). Observational learning and enculturation. Educational and Child Development, 21(2), 39–50.
O’Meara, T. (1990). Samoan Planters: Tradition and Economic Development in Polynesia. Fort Worth: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.
Oppenheim, R. W. (1991). Cell death during development of the nervous system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 14, 453–501.
Over, H. and Carpenter, M. (2009). Eighteen-month-old infants show increased helping following priming with affiliation. Psychological Science, 20, 1189–1193.
Papoušek, H. and Papoušek, M. (1995). Intuitive parenting. In Marc H. Bornstein (ed.) (1995). Handbook of Parenting, vol. 2: Biology and Ecology of Parenting (pp. 117–136). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Parnas, J. et al. (2005). EASE: Examination of Anomalous Self-Experience. Psychopathology, 38, 236–258.
Pasolini, P. P. (1962/1984). The Scent of India. London: Olive.
Passman, R. H. and Halonen, J. S. (1979). A developmental survey of young children’s attachment to inanimate objects. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 134, 165–178.
Piaget, J. (1932/1977). The Moral Judgment of the Child. Harmondsworth: Penguin.
Piaget, J. (1936/1952). The Origins of Intelligence in Children. New York: International Universities Press.
Piaget, J. (1938/1955). The Construction of Reality in the Child. New York: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, Dreams, and Imitation in Childhood. New York: Norton.
Pierce, J. L. and Jussila, I. (2011). Psychological Ownership and the Organizational Context: Theory, Research Evidence and Application. New Horizons in Management Series. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar.
Pilgrim, C. and Rueda-Riedle, A. (2002). The importance of social context in cross-cultural comparisons: first graders in Columbia and the United States. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 163(3), 283–295.
Polanyi, K. (1944/2001). The Great Transformation: The Political and Economic Origins of our Time. Boston: Beacon Press.
Porges, S. W. (2001). The polyvagal theory: phylogenetic substrates of a social nervous system. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 42, 123–146.
Porges, S. W. (2004). Neuroception: a subconscious system for detecting threats and safety. Zero to Three, May, 19–24.
Poss, S. and Rochat, P. (2003). Referential understanding of videos in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), and children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117(4), 420–428.
Prechtl, H. F. R. (1977). Assessment and significance of behavioural states. In S. R. Berenberg (ed.), Brain, Fetal and Infant: Current Research on Normal and Abnormal Development (pp. 79–90). The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.
Prechtl, H. F. R. (ed.) (1984). Continuity of Neural Functions: From Prenatal to Postnatal Life. Oxford: Blackwell.
Proctor, D., Williamson, R. A., de Waal, F. B. M., and Brosnan, S. F. (2013). Chimpanzees play the ultimatum game. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 110, 2070–2075.
Provence, S. and Lipton, R. C. (1962). Infants in Institutions. New York: International Universities Press.
Rakic, P. (1972). Model of cell migration to the superficial layers of the fetal monkey neocortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 145, 61–84.
Rakoczy, H., Tomasello, M., and Striano, T. (2004). Young children know that trying is not pretending: a test of the “behaving-as-if” construal of children’s understanding of pretense. Developmental Psychology, 40, 388–399.
Rakoczy, H., Warneken, F., and Tomasello, M. (2008). The sources of normativity: young children’s awareness of the normative structure of games. Developmental Psychology, 44(3), 875–881.
Rao, N. and Stewart, S. M. (1999). Cultural influences on sharer and recipient behavior sharing in Chinese and Indian preschool children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30(2), 219–241.
Ratner, N. and Bruner, J. (1978). Games, social exchange and the acquisition of language. Journal of Child Language, 5, 391–401.
Rawls, J. (1999). A Theory of Justice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Reddy, V. (2007). Getting back to the rough ground: deception and “social living.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1480), 621–637.
Reed, E. S. (1984). An outline of a theory of action systems. Journal of Motor Behavior, 14, 98–134.
Rigo, B. (2005). Conscience occidentale et fables océaniques: la dynamique de la contradiction. Paris: Editions L’Harmattan.
Robbins, E. and Rochat, P. (2011). Emerging signs of strong reciprocity in human ontogeny. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(353), 1–14.
Robbins, E. and Rochat, P. (2014, in preparation). Transitional objects in the South Pacific and the United States.
Rochat, M. J., Veroni, V., Brushweiler-Stern, N. et al. (2014, in press). Impaired vitality form recognition in autism. Neuropsychologica.
Rochat, P. (1983). Oral touch in young infants: response to variations of nipple characteristics in the first months of life. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 6, 123–133.
Rochat, P. (1987). Mouthing and grasping in neonates: evidence for the early detection of what hard or soft substances afford for action. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 435–449.
Rochat, P. (ed.) (1999). Early Social Cognition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Rochat, P. (2001). The Infant’s World. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Rochat, P. (2009). Others in Mind: Social Origins of Self-consciousness. Cambridge University Press.
Rochat, P. (2011a). Possession and morality in early development. New Directions in Child and Adolescent Development, 11(132), 23–38.
Rochat, P. (2011b). The self as phenotype. Cognition and Consciousness, 20(1), 109–119.
Rochat, P. (2013). The gaze of others. In M. Banaji and S. Gelman (eds.), Navigating the Social World. Oxford University Press.
Rochat, P., Dias, M. D. G., Guo, L., Broesch, T., Passos-Ferreira, C., Winning, A., and Berg, B. (2009). Fairness in distributive justice by 3- and 5-year-olds across 7 cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40(3), 416–442.
Rochat, P., Goubet, N., and Shah, B. (1997). Enhanced sucking engagement by preterm infants during intermittent gavage feedings. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 18(1), 22–26.
Rochat, P. and Hespos, S. J. (1997). Differential rooting response by neonates: evidence of an early sense of self. Early Development and Parenting, 6(3–4), 105–112.
Rochat, P. and Passos-Ferreira, C. (2008). Becoming Homo negotiatus: ontogeny of the unique ways humans own and share. In S. Itakura and K. Fujita (eds.), Origins of the Social Mind: Evolutionary and Developmental Views (pp. 141–156). New York: Springer.
Rochat, P. and Senders, S. J. (1991). Active touch in infancy: action systems in development. In M. J. Weiss and P. R. Zelazo (eds.), Infant Attention: Biological Constraints and the Influence of Experience (pp. 412–442). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Rochat, P. and Striano, T. (1999a). Emerging self-exploration by 2 month-old infants. Developmental Science, 2(2), 206–218.
Rochat, P. and Striano, T. (1999b). Social cognitive development in the first year. In P. Rochat (ed.), Early Social Cognition (pp. 3–34). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Rochat, P., Querido, J., and Striano, T. (1999). Emerging sensitivity to the timing and structure of protoconversation in early infancy. Developmental Psychology, 35(4), 950–957.
Rochat, P., Robbins, E., Dias, M., and Passos-Ferreira, C. (2014). Coin versus food sharing in children growing up in highly contrasted socio-economical circumstances in North-eastern Brazil and the United States of America (in preparation).
Rogoff, B. (2003). The Cultural Nature of Human Development. Oxford University Press.
Rogoff, B., Baker-Sennett, J., Lacasa, P., and Goldsmith, D. (1995). Development through participation in sociocultural activity. In J. J. Goodnow, P. J. Miller, and F. Kessel (eds.), Cultural Practices as Contexts for Development (pp. 45–65). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Rose, C. M. (1985). Possession as the origin of property. University of Chicago Law Review, 52(1), 73–88.
Ross, H., Conant, C. M., and Vickar, M. (2011). Property rights and the resolution of social conflict. In H. Ross and O. Friedman (eds.), Origins of Ownership of Property. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 132, 53–64.
Ross, H. and Friedman, O. (eds.) (2011). Origins of Ownership of Property. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 132 (special issue).
Rossano, F., Rakoczy, H., and Tomasello, M. (2011). Young children’s understanding of violation of property rights. Cognition, 121, 219–227.
Rousseau, J.-J. (1755). Discours sur l’origine et les fondements de l’inégalité parmi les hommes. (Discourse on the origin and the foundations of inequality among men). Paris.
Rudmin, F. W. (1991). “To own is to be perceived to own”: a social cognitive look at the ownership of property. In To Have Possessions: A Handbook on Ownership and Property, special issue of Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 6(6–7), 85–104.
Sagi, A., Lamb, M. E., Lewkowicz, K. S., Shoham, R., Dvir, R., and Estes, D. (1985). Security of infant–mother, –father, and –metapelet attachments among kibbutz-reared Israeli children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(1–2, 209), 257–275.
Sahlins, M. (1963). Poor man, rich man, big man, chief: political types in Melanesia and Polynesia. Comparative Studies in Society and History, 5(3), 285–303.
Saks, E. (2007). The Center Cannot Hold: My Journey through Madness. New York: Hyperion.
Salas, C., Broglio, C., Durán, E., Gómez, A., Ocana, F. M., Jiménez-Moya, F., and Rodriguez, F. (2006). Neuropsychology of learning and memory in teleost fish. Zebrafish, 3(2), 157–171.
Sartre, J.-P. (1971/1981). The Family Idiot: Gustave Flaubert 1821–1857, vol. I. Trans. Carol Cosman. University of Chicago Press.
Saussure, F. de (2002). Ecrits de linguistique générale. Paris: Gallimard.
Saussure, F. de (2006). Writings in General Linguistics. Oxford University Press.
Scaife, M. and Bruner, J. S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature, 253, 265–266.
Schaal, B., Orgeur, P., and Rognon, C. (1995). Odor sensing in the human fetus: anatomical, functional, and chemio-ecological bases. In J.-P. Lecanuet, W. P. Fifer, N. A. Krasnegor, and W. P. Smotherman (eds.), Fetal Development: A Psychobiological Perspective (pp. 205–237). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Schachter, S. and Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–399.
Schweder, R. A. (1982). Beyond self-constructed knowledge: the study of culture and morality. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28(1), 41–69.
Seligmann, C. G. (1908). Notes on recent work among the Veddas. Journal of the Royal Asiatic Society (Ceylon), 21(61), 74.
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., and Marler, P. (1980). Vervet monkey alarm calls: semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Animal Behavior, 28(4), 1070–1094.
Sherrington, C. S. (1906). The Integrative Action of the Nervous System. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Shore, B. (1982). Sala’ilua: A Samoan Mystery. New York: Columbia University Press.
Shutts, K., Kinzer, K., Katz, R., Tredoux, C., and Spelke, E. (2011). Race preferences in children: insights from South Africa. Developmental Science, 14, 1–9.
Simoons, F. J. (1969). Primary adult lactose intolerance and the milking habit: a problem in biological and cultural interrelations. I. Review of the medical research. Digestive Diseases and Sciences, 14(12), 819–836.
Slater, A., Mattock, A., and Brown, E. (1990). Size constancy at birth: newborn infants’ responses to retinal and real size. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 314–322.
Slomp, G. (2000). Thomas Hobbes and the Political Philosophy of Glory. New York: St. Martin’s Press.
Slomp, G. (2007). Hobbes on glory and civil strife. In P. Springborg (ed.), The Cambridge Companion to Hobbes’s Leviathan (pp. 181–198). Cambridge University Press.
Smith, Adam (1759/2002). The Theory of Moral Sentiments, ed. Knud Haakonssen. Cambridge University Press.
Smith, S. (1996). Commission of Inquiry into Fetal Sentience. London: CARE.
Snarey, J. (1985). Cross-cultural universality of social-moral development: a critical review of Kohlbergian research. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 202–232.
Sneddon, L. U., Braithwaite, V. A., and Gentle, M. J. (2003). Do fish have nociceptors? Evidence for the evolution of a vertebrate sensory system. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B, 270, 1115–1121.
Spaepen, E., Coppola, M., Spelke. E., Carey, S., and Goldin-Meadow, S. (2011). Number without a language model. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(8), 3163–3168.
Sperber, D. (1974). Le symbolisme en général (Symbolism in general). Paris: Hermann.
Spitz, R. A. (1965). The First Year of Life: A Psychoanalytic Study of Normal and Deviant Development of Object Relations. New York: Basic Books.
Sroufe, A. (2005). Attachment and development: a prospective, longitudinal study from birth to adulthood. Attachment and Human Development, 7(4), 349–367.
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E., and Collins, W. A. (2005). The Development of the Person: The Minnesota Study of Risk and Adaptation from Birth to Adulthood. New York: Guilford.
Stern, D. N. (1985). The Interpersonal World of the Infant: A View from Psychoanalysis and Developmental Psychology. New York: Basic Books.
Stern, D. N. (1991). The Diary of a Baby: What Your Child Sees, Feels, and Experiences. London: Fontana Press.
Stern, D. N., Hofer, L., Haft. W., and Dore, J. (1985). Affect attunement: the sharing by means of intermodal fluency. In T. M. Field and N. A. Fox (eds.), Social Perception in Infants (pp. 249–286). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Stewart, S. M. and McBride-Chang, C. (2000). Influences on children’s sharing in multicultural setting. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 31, 333–348.
Stiles, D. (2001). Hunter-gatherer studies: the importance of context. African Study Monographs, Suppl. 26, 41–65.
Strathern, A. (1971). The Rope of Moka: Big-men and Ceremonial Exchange in Mount Hagen, New Guinea. Cambridge University Press.
Striano, T. and Rochat, P. (1999). Developmental link between dyadic and triadic social competence in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17(4), 551–562.
Striano, T. and Rochat, P. (2000). Emergence of selective social referencing. Infancy, 1(2), 253–264.
Striano, T., Tomasello, M., and Rochat, P. (2001). Social and object support for early symbolic play. Developmental Science, 4(4), 442–455.
Super, C. M. and Harkness, S. (1986). The developmental niche: a conceptualization at the interface of child and culture. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 9, 545–569.
Super, C. M. and Harkness, S. (2002). Culture structures the environment for development. Human Development, 45(4), 270–274.
Takahashi, K. (1986). Examining the strange-situation procedure with Japanese mothers and 12-month old infants. Developmental Psychology, 22(2), 265–270.
Taylor, C. (1989). Sources of the Self: The Making of the Modern Identity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Thomsen, L., Frankenhuis, W. E., Ingold-Smith, M., and Carey, S. (2011). Big and mighty: preverbal infants mentally represent social dominance. Science, 331, 477–480.
Tobin, J. J., Wu, D. Y. H., and Davidson, D. H. (1989). Preschools in Three Cultures: Japan, China, and the United States. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. J. Moore and P. Dunham (eds.), Joint Attention: Its Origins and Role in Development (pp. 103–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Tomasello, M. (1998). One child’s early talk about possession. In J. Newman (ed.), The Linguistic of Giving (pp. 349–373). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Tomasello, M. (1999). Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Tomasello, M. (2009). Why We Cooperate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., and Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Science, 28, 675–735.
Tomasello, M. and Haberl, K. (2003). Understanding attention: 12- and 18-month-olds know what is new for other persons. Developmental Psychology, 39, 906–912.
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., and Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 16, 495–552.
Trevarthan, W. R. (1987). Human Birth: An Evolutionary Perspective. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.
Trevarthen, C. (1980). The foundations of intersubjectivity: developments of interpersonal and cooperative understanding in infants. In D. R. Olson (ed.), The Social Foundations of Language and Thought: Essays in Honor of Jerome S. Bruner. New York: Norton.
Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts. Psychological Review, 96(3), 506–520.
Triandis, H. C. and Gelfand, M. J. (1998). Converging measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1), 118–128.
Tronick, E., Als, H., Adamson, L. B., Wise, S., and Brazelton, T. B. (1978). The infant’s response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face to face interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 17, 1–13.
United Nations Development Programme (UNDP) (2006). Human Development Report, 2006. New York: United Nations.
Veblen, T. (1899/1994). Theory of the Leisure Class: An Economic Study in the Evolution of Institutions. New York: Dover.
Veyne, P. (1976). Le pain et le cirque (Bread and Circuses). Paris: Le Seuil.
Veyne, P. (2006). The Roman Empire. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Vienne, B. (1984). Gens de Motlap (People of Motlap). Paris: Musée de L’Homme.
Volkmar, F., Chawarska, K., and Klin, A. (2005). Autism in infancy and early childhood. Annual Reviews of Psychology, 56, 315–336.
Von Hofsten, C. (1982). Eye–hand coordination in newborns. Developmental Psychology, 18, 450–461.
Walker, L. J. and Frimer, J. A. (2007). Moral personality of brave and caring exemplars. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 845–860.
Walker, L. J., Frimer, J. A., and Dunlop, W. L. (2010). Varieties of moral personality: beyond the banality of heroism. Journal of Personality, 78, 907–942.
Waters, E., Weinfield, N. S., and Hamilton, C. E. (2000). The stability of attachment styles from infancy to adolescence and early adulthood: general discussion. Child Development, 71, 703–706.
Weber, M. (1904/2009). The Protestant Ethic and the Spirit of Capitalism. New York: Norton.
Weiner, A. (1992). Inalienable Possessions: The Paradox of Keeping-while-Giving. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684.
Winnicott, D. (1953). Transitional objects and transitional phenomena. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 34, 229–242.
Wolff, P. H. (1987). The Development of Behavioral States and the Expression of Emotions in Early Infancy. University of Chicago Press.
Woodburn, J. (1982). Egalitarian societies. Man, 17(3), 431–451.
Yang, M. M. (2007). Ritual economy and rural capitalism with Chinese characteristics. In D. Held and H. Moore (eds.), Cultural Politics in a Global Age: Uncertainty, Solidarity and Innovation (pp. 226–233). Oxford: Oneworld Publications.
Zahavi, A. and Zahavi, A. (1997). The Handicap Principle: A Missing Piece of Darwin’s Puzzle. Oxford University Press.
Zebian, S. and Rochat, P. (2012). Judgment of land ownership by young refugee Palestinian and US children. International Journal of Behavioral Development November, 36(6), 449–456.

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Book summary page views

Total views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between #date#. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed.