Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 6
  • Print publication year: 2013
  • Online publication date: March 2013

Chapter 17 - Neuroimaging of attention and alteration of processing capacity in sleep-deprived persons

from Section 3 - Neuroimaging, sleep loss, and circadian misalignment

Summary

Low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (LORETA) was one of the first attempts to solve both the inverse problem and the reference electrode problem. It is able to localize deep sources as well by minimizing the squared three-dimensional (3D) spatial Laplacian operator to determine the unique solution. In 2001, the authors published the first study that applied LORETA to sleep electroencephalogram (EEG) data. They selected artifact-free epochs with sleep spindles and determined LORETA power in the frequency domain via the EEG cross-spectral matrix. LORETA revealed cortical spindle sources predominantly medially in the frontal and parietal lobe. The cortical generators localized for delta waves in slow-wave sleep (SWS) showed considerable overlap with the spindle generators. LORETA was applied to reveal changes in brain activity due to chronic hypoxia in patients with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome (OSAS).

References

1. CorbettaM, ShulmanGL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3(3):201–15.
2. KastnerS, UngerleiderLG. Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:315–41.
3. DesimoneR, DuncanJ. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1995;18:193–222.
4. GazzaleyA, CooneyJW, McEvoyK, KnightRT, D’EspositoM. Top-down enhancement and suppression of the magnitude and speed of neural activity. J Cogn Neurosci. 2005;17(3):507–17.
5. DoranSM, Van DongenHP, DingesDF. Sustained attention performance during sleep deprivation: evidence of state instability. Arch Ital Biol. 2001;139(3):253–67.
6. GrawP, KrauchiK, KnoblauchV, Wirz-JusticeA, CajochenC. Circadian and wake-dependent modulation of fastest and slowest reaction times during the psychomotor vigilance task. Physiol Behav. 2004;80(5):695–701.
7. HorneJA, ReynerLA. Sleep related vehicle accidents. BMJ. 1995;310(6979):565–7.
8. LeproultR, ColecchiaEF, BerardiAM, et al. Individual differences in subjective and objective alertness during sleep deprivation are stable and unrelated. Am J Physiol. 2003;284(2):R280–90.
9. Van DongenHP, BaynardMD, MaislinG, DingesDF. Systematic interindividual differences in neurobehavioral impairment from sleep loss: evidence of trait-like differential vulnerability. Sleep. 2004;27(3):423–33.
10. HopfingerJB, BuonocoreMH, MangunGR. The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3(3):284–91.
11. KastnerS, PinskMA, De WeerdP, DesimoneR, UngerleiderLG. Increased activity in human visual cortex during directed attention in the absence of visual stimulation. Neuron. 1999;22(4):751–61.
12. LiuT, SlotnickSD, SerencesJT, YantisS. Cortical mechanisms of feature-based attentional control. Cereb Cortex. 2003;13(12):1334–43.
13. SerencesJT, SchwarzbachJ, CourtneySM, GolayX, YantisS. Control of object-based attention in human cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2004;14(12):1346–57.
14. WojciulikE, KanwisherN. The generality of parietal involvement in visual attention. Neuron. 1999;23(4):747–64.
15. CheeMW, ChuahYM. Functional neuroimaging and behavioral correlates of capacity decline in visual short-term memory after sleep deprivation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104(22):9487–92.
16. CheeMWL, ChooWC. Functional imaging of working memory after 24 hr of total sleep deprivation. J Neurosci. 2004;24(19):4560–7.
17. YiD-J, ChunMM. Attentional modulation of learning-related repetition attenuation effects in human parahippocampal cortex. J Neurosci. 2005;25(14):3593–600.
18. LimJ, TanJC, ParimalS, DingesDF, CheeMW. Sleep deprivation impairs object-selective attention: a view from the ventral visual cortex. PLoS One. 2010;5(2):e9087.
19. CheeMWL, TanJC, ParimalS, ZagorodnovV. Sleep deprivation and its effects on object-selective attention. Neuroimage. 2010;49(2):1903–10.
20. LangnerR, WillmesK, ChatterjeeA, EickhoffSB, SturmW. Energetic effects of stimulus intensity on prolonged simple reaction-time performance. Psychol Res. 2010;74(5):499–512.
21. AndersonC, HorneJA. Sleepiness enhances distraction during a monotonous task. Sleep. 2006;29(4):573–6.
22. KongD, SoonCS, CheeMW. Functional imaging correlates of impaired distractor suppression following sleep deprivation. Neuroimage. 2012;61(1):50–5.
23. GazzaleyA, CooneyJW, RissmanJ, D’EspositoM. Top-down suppression deficit underlies working memory impairment in normal aging. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(10):1298–300.
24. DarowskiES, HelderE, ZacksRT, HasherL, HambrickDZ. Age-related differences in cognition: the role of distraction control. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(5):638–44.
25. ChuahLY, CheeMW. Cholinergic augmentation modulates visual task performance in sleep-deprived young adults. J Neurosci. 2008;28(44):11369–77.
26. CheeMW, TanJC. Lapsing when sleep deprived: neural activation characteristics of resistant and vulnerable individuals. Neuroimage. 2010;51(2):835–43.
27. DrummondSP, MeloyMJ, YanagiMA, OrffHJ, BrownGG. Compensatory recruitment after sleep deprivation and the relationship with performance. Psychiatry Res. 2005;140(3):211–23.
28. LavieN. Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1995;21(3):451–68.
29. PessoaL, PadmalaS, MorlandT. Fate of unattended fearful faces in the amygdala is determined by both attentional resources and cognitive modulation. Neuroimage. 2005;28(1):249–55.
30. ForsterS, LavieN. High perceptual load makes everybody equal: eliminating individual differences in distractibility with load. Psychol Sci. 2007;18(5):377–81.
31. ReesG, FrithCD, LavieN. Modulating irrelevant motion perception by varying attentional load in an unrelated task. Science. 1997;278(5343):1616–19.
32. YiD-J, WoodmanGF, WiddersD, MaroisR, ChunMM. Neural fate of ignored stimuli: dissociable effects of perceptual and working memory load. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7(9):992–6.
33. KongD, SoonCS, CheeMW. Reduced visual processing capacity in sleep deprived persons. Neuroimage. 2011;55(2):629–34.
34. Turk-BrowneNB, YiDJ, ChunMM. Linking implicit and explicit memory: common encoding factors and shared representations. Neuron. 2006;49(6):917–27.
35. EpsteinRA, HigginsJS, Thompson-SchillSL. Learning places from views: variation in scene processing as a function of experience and navigational ability. J Cogn Neurosci. 2005;17(1):73–83.
36. LimJ, ChooWC, CheeMW. Reproducibility of changes in behaviour and fMRI activation associated with sleep deprivation in a working memory task. Sleep. 2007;30(1):61–70.
37. RypmaB, PrabhakaranV. When less is more and when more is more: the mediating roles of capacity and speed in brain-behavior efficiency. Intelligence. 2009;37(2):207–22.
38. ChuahLY, ChongDL, ChenAK, et al. Donepezil improves episodic memory in young individuals vulnerable to the effects of sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2009;32(8):999–1010.
39. MuQ, MishoryA, JohnsonKA, et al. Decreased brain activation during a working memory task at rested baseline is associated with vulnerability to sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2005;28(4):433–46.
40. CheeMWL, ChuahLYM, VenkatramanV, et al. Functional imaging of working memory following normal sleep and after 24 and 35 h of sleep deprivation: correlations of fronto-parietal activation with performance. Neuroimage. 2006;31(1):419–28.
41. ChunMM. Visual working memory as visual attention sustained internally over time. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(6):1407–9.
42. LuckSJ, VogelEK. The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature. 1997;390(6657):279–81.
43. EverittBJ, RobbinsTW. Central cholinergic systems and cognition. Annu Rev Psychol. 1997;48:649–84.
44. WeissmanDH, RobertsKC, VisscherKM, WoldorffMG. The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9(7):971–8.
45. CheeMWL, TanJC, ZhengH, et al. Lapsing during sleep deprivation is associated with distributed changes in brain activation. J Neurosci. 2008;28(21):5519–28.
46. PigarevIN, NothdurftHC, KastnerS. Evidence for asynchronous development of sleep in cortical areas. Neuroreport. 1997;8(11):2557–60.
47. MaroisR, ChunMM, GoreJC. A common parieto-frontal network is recruited under both low visibility and high perceptual interference conditions. J Neurophysiol. 2004;92(5):2985–92.
48. PortasCM, ReesG, HowsemanAM, et al. A specific role for the thalamus in mediating the interaction of attention and arousal in humans. J Neurosci. 1998;18(21):8979–89.
49. TomasiD, WangRL, TelangF, et al. Impairment of attentional networks after 1 night of sleep deprivation. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19(1):233–40.
50. LuckSJ, ChelazziL, HillyardSA, DesimoneR. Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1997;77(1):24–42.
51. Grent-’t-JongT, WoldorffMG. Timing and sequence of brain activity in top-down control of visual-spatial attention. PLoS Biol. 2007;5(1):e12.
52. SapirA, d’AvossaG, McAvoyM, ShulmanGL, CorbettaM. Brain signals for spatial attention predict performance in a motion discrimination task. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102(49):17810–15.
53. StokesM, ThompsonR, NobreAC, DuncanJ. Shape-specific preparatory activity mediates attention to targets in human visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106(46):19569–74.
54. PessoaL, KastnerS, UngerleiderLG. Neuroimaging studies of attention: from modulation of sensory processing to top-down control. J Neurosci. 2003;23(10):3990–8.
55. EicheleT, DebenerS, CalhounVD, et al. Prediction of human errors by maladaptive changes in event-related brain networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105(16):6173–8.
56. CheeMW, GohCS, NamburiP, et al. Effects of sleep deprivation on cortical activation during directed attention in the absence and presence of visual stimuli. Neuroimage. 2011;58(2):595–604.
57. VyazovskiyVV, OlceseU, HanlonEC, et al. Local sleep in awake rats. Nature. 2011;472:443–7.
58. KruegerJM, RectorDM, RoyS, et al. Sleep as a fundamental property of neuronal assemblies. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(12):910–19.
59. De HavasJA, ParimalS, SoonCS, CheeMW. Sleep deprivation reduces default mode network connectivity and anti-correlation during rest and task performance. Neuroimage. 2012;59(2):1745–51.
60. SämannPG, TullyC, Spoormaker VI, et al. Increased sleep pressure reduces resting state functional connectivity. MAGMA. 2010;23(5–6):375–89.
61. GujarN, YooSS, HuP, WalkerMP. The unrested resting brain: sleep deprivation alters activity within the default-mode network. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010;22(8):1637–48.
62. DrummondSP, Bischoff-GretheA, DingesDF, et al. The neural basis of the psychomotor vigilance task. Sleep. 2005;28(9):1059–68.