Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T23:04:07.848Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - Neural networks for perceptual processing: from simulation tools to theories

from Part I - General themes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 July 2011

Kevin Gurney
Affiliation:
University of Sheffield
Colin R. Tosh
Affiliation:
University of Leeds
Graeme D. Ruxton
Affiliation:
University of Glasgow
Get access

Summary

Introduction

This paper has two main aims. First, to give an introduction to some of the construction techniques – the ‘nuts-and-bolts’ as it were – of neural networks deployed by the authors in this book. Our intention is to emphasise conceptual principles and their associated terminology, and to do this wherever possible without recourse to detailed mathematical descriptions. However, the term ‘neural network’ has taken on a multitude of meanings over the last couple of decades, depending on its methodological and scientific context. A second aim, therefore, given that the application of the techniques described in this book may appear rather diverse, is to supply some meta-theoretical landmarks to help understand the significance of the ensuing results.

In general terms, neural networks are tools for building models of systems that are characterised by data sets which are often (but not always) derived by sampling a system input-output behaviour. While a neural network model is of some utility if it mimics the behaviour of the target system, it is far more useful if key mechanisms underlying the model functionality can be unearthed, and identified with those of the underlying system. That is, the modeller can ‘break into’ the model, viewed initially as an input-output ‘black box’, and find internal representations, variable relationships, and structures which may correspond with the underlying target system. This target system may be entirely non-biological (e.g. stock market prices), or be of biological origin, but have nothing to do with brains (e.g. ecologically driven patterns of population dynamics).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adelson, E. H. & Movshon, J. A. 1982. Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns. Nature 300, 523–525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andersen, R. A., Essick, G. K. & Siegel, R. M. 1985. Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. Science 230, 456–458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barto, A. G. 1985. Learning by statistical cooperation of sele-interested neuron-like computing elements. Hum Neurobiol 4, 229–256.Google ScholarPubMed
Barto, A. G. & Anandan, P. 1985. Pattern-recognizing stochastic learning automata. IEEE Syst Man CyberneticsSMC-15, 360–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barto, A. G., Sutton, R. S. & Anderson, C. W. 1983. Neuronlike elements that can solve difficult learning control problems. IEEE Trans Systems Man Cybernetics 13, 835–846.Google Scholar
Bishop, C. M. 1996. Neural Networks for Pattern Recognition. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cheng, B. & Titterington, D. M. 1994. Neural networks: a review from a statistical perspective. Statist Sci 9, 2–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K. & McClelland, J. L. 1990. On the control of automatic processes – a parallel distributed-processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychol Rev 97, 332–361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crick, F. 1989. The recent excitement about neural networks. Nature 337, 129–132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dayan, P. 2002. Levels of analysis in neural modeling. In Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science (ed. L. Nadel). Nature Publishing Group; John Wiley & Sons Ltd.Google Scholar
Dror, I. E. & Gallogly, D. P. 1999. Computational analyses in cognitive neuroscience: in defense of biological implausibility. Psychon Bull Rev 6, 173–182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elman, J. L. 1990. Finding structure in time. Cogn Sci 14, 179–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitzsimonds, R. M., Song, H. J. & Poo, M. M. 1997. Propagation of activity-dependent synaptic depression in simple neural networks. Nature 388, 439–448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, C. D. 2001. Scientific models, connectionist networks, and cognitive science. Theory Psychol 11, 97–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gurney, K. 1997. An Introduction to Neural Networks. UCL Press (Taylor and Francis group).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gurney, K., Prescott, T. J., Wickens, J. R. & Redgrave, P. 2004. Computational models of the basal ganglia: from robots to membranes. Trends Neurosci 27, 453–459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gurney, K. N. & Wright, M. J. 1992. A self-organising neural network model of image velocity encoding. Biol Cybernetics 68, 173–181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haykin, S. 1999. Neural Networks: A Comprehensive Foundation. Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Hebb, D. 1949. The Organization of Behaviour. John Wiley.Google Scholar
Hinton, G. E. & Shallice, T. 1991. Lesioning an attractor network – investigations of acquired dyslexia. Psychol Rev 98, 74–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodgkin, A. L. & Huxley, A. F. 1952. A quantitative description of membrane current and its application to conduction and excitation in nerve. J Physiol 117, 500–544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopfield, J. J. 1982. Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 79, 2554–2558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hubel, D. H. & Wiesel, T. N. 1968. Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. J Physiol 195, 215–243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobs, R. A., Jordan, M. I. & Barto, A. G. 1991. Task decomposition through competition in a modular connectionist architecture. Cogn Sci 15, 219–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jordan, M. I. 1986. Serial Order: a Parallel Distributed Approach. University of California, Institute for Cognitive Science.
Koch, C. 1999. The Biophysics of Computation: Information Processing in Single Neurons. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Koch, C., Mo, C. & Softky, W. 2002. Single-cell models. In The Handbook of Brain Theory and Neural Networks (ed. Arbib, M.). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kohonen, T. 1984. Self-organization and Associative Memory. Springer Verlag.Google Scholar
Lippmann, R. 1987. An introduction to computing with neural nets. ASSP Magazine, IEEE 4, 4–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacLeod, C. M. 1991. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect – an integrative review. Psychol Bull 109, 163–203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Makhoul, J., El-Jaroudi, A. & Schwartz, R. 1989. Formation of disconnected decision regions with a single hidden layer. In International Joint Conference on Neural Networks, Vol. 1, pp. 455–460.
Marr, D. 1982. Vision: A Computational Investigation into Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information. W.H. Freeeman and Co.Google Scholar
Marr, D. & Poggio, T. 1976. From Understanding Computation to Understanding Neural Circuitry. MIT AI Laboratory.
McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L. & O' Reilly, R. C. 1995. Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychol Rev 102, 419–457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, J. L. & Rumelhart, D. E. 1985. Distributed memory and the representation of general and specific information. J Exp Psychol Gen 114, 159–188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCloskey, M. 1991. Networks and theories – the place of connectionism in cognitive science. Psychol Sci 2, 387–395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCulloch, W. S. & Pitts, W. 1943. A logical calculus of the ideas immanent in nervous activity. Bull Math Biophysics 7, 115–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minsky, M. & Papert, S. 1969. Perceptrons. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Movshon, J. A., Adelson, E. H., Gizzi, M. S. & Newsom, W. T. 1985. The analysis of moving visual patterns. In Pattern Recognition Mechanisms (ed. Chagas, C., Gattas, R. & Gross, C. G.), pp. 117–151. Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Parker, D. B. 1982. Learning-logic. Office of Technology Licensing, Stanford University.
Price, D. & Willshaw, D. 2000. Mechanisms of Cortical Development. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rieke, F., Warland, D., Ruyter van Steveninck, R. & Bialek, W. 1996. Spikes. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rosenblatt, F. 1958. The perceptron: a probabilistic model for information storage and organization in the brain. Psychol Rev 65, 386–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosin, P. L. & Fierens, F. 1995. Improving neural net generalisation. In Proceedings of IGARSS'95. Firenze, Italy.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L. & ,The PDP Research Group. 1986. Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E. & Todd, P. 1993. Learning and connectionist representations. In Attention and Performance XIV (ed. Meyer, D. & Kornblum, S.), pp. 3–31. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Shang, Y. & Wah, B. W. 1996. Global optimization for neural network training. IEEE Computer 29, 45–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepherd, G. M. & Koch, C. 1998. Introduction to synaptic circuits. In The Synaptic Organization of the Brain (ed. G. M. Shepherd), pp. 1–36. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Smolensky, P. 1988. On the proper treatment of connectionism. Behav Brain Sci 11, 1–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stone, M. 1974. Cross-validatory choice and assesment of statistical predictions. J R Statist Soc B36, 111–133.Google Scholar
Stroop, J. R. 1935. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 18, 643–662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutton, R. S. 1988. Learning to predict by the method of temporal differences. Machine Learn 3, 9–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutton, R. S. & Barto, A. G. 1998. Reinforcement: An Introduction. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Swindale, N. V. 1980. A model for the formation of ocular dominance stripes. Proc R Soc B 208, 243–264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Touretzky, D. S. 1995. Connectionist and symbolic representations. In The Handbook of Brain Theory and Neural Networks, 1st Edn. (ed. Arbib, M.), pp. 243–247. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tveter, D. R. 1996. Backpropagator's review. www.dontveter.com/bpr/activate.html
Vallortigara, G. & Bressan, P. 1991. Occlusion and the perception of coherent motion. Vision Res 31, 1967–1978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
der Malsburg, C. 1973. Self-organization of orientation sensitive cells in the striate cortex. Kybernetik 14, 85–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallach, H. 1976. On perceived identity 1: The direction of motion of straight lines. In On Perception (ed. Wallach, H.). Quadrangle.Google Scholar
Werbos, P. 1974. Beyond Regression: New Tools for Prediction and Analysis in the Behavioural Sciences. Harvard University.Google Scholar
Widrow, B. & Hoff Jr, M. E. 1960. Adaptive switching circuits. In IRE WESCON Convention Record, pp. 96–104.
Widrow, B. & Stearns, S. D. 1985. Adaptive Signal Processing. Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Wieland, A. & Leighton, R. 1987. Geometric analysis of neural network capabilities. In 1st IEEE International Conference on Neural Networks, Vol. III, pp. 385–392. San Diego, California.Google Scholar
Williams, R. J. 1987. Reinforcement Learning Connectionist Systems. Northeastern University, Boston.Google Scholar
Willshaw, D. J. & der Malsburg, C. 1976. How patterned neural connections can be set up by self-organization. Proc R Soc B 194, 431–445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zipser, D. 1992. Identification models of the nervous system. Neuroscience 47, 853–862.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zipser, D. & Andersen, R. A. 1988. A back-propagation programmed network that simulates response properties of a subset of posterior parietal neurons. Nature 331, 679–684.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×