Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-jr42d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T01:33:11.106Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 July 2017

Beate Hampe
Affiliation:
Universität Erfurt, Germany
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Metaphor
Embodied Cognition and Discourse
, pp. 335 - 365
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbot, Michael, and Forceville, Charles J. (2011). LOSS OF CONTROL IS LOSS OF HANDS in Azumanga Daioh volume 4. Language and Literature, 20(2), 91112.Google Scholar
Abreu, Ana Maria, Macaluso, Emiliano, Azevedo, Ruben, Cesari, Paola, Urgesi, Cosimo, and Aglioti, Salvatore Maria (2012). Action anticipation beyond the action observation network: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in expert basketball players. European Journal of Neuroscience, 35(10), 16461654.Google Scholar
Adam, Hajo, and Galinsky, Adam (2012). Enclothed cognition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(4), 918925.Google Scholar
Ahissar, Edud, Vaadia, Eilon, Ahissar, Merav, Bergman, Hagai, Arieli, Amos, and Abeles, Moshe (1992). Dependence of cortical plasticity on correlated activity of single neurons and on behavioral context. Science, 257, 14121415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alibali, Martha W., Boncoddo, Rebecca, and Hostetter, Autumn (2014). Gesture in reasoning: An embodied perspective. In Shapiro (ed.), 150–159.Google Scholar
Allbritton, David W., McKoon, Gail, and Gerrig, Richard (1995). Metaphor-based schemas and text representations: Making connections through conceptual metaphors. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 21(3), 612625.Google Scholar
Allen, Chris, Vassilev, Ivaylo, Kennedy, Anne, and Rogers, Anne (2016). Long-term condition self-management support in online communities: A meta-synthesis of qualitative papers. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 18(3), e61.Google Scholar
Alsmith, Adrian, and de Vignemont, Frédérique (2012). Embodying the mind and representing the body. Review of Philosophy and Psychology. Special Issue, 3(1), 113.Google Scholar
Alverson, Hoyt (1994). Semantics and Experience: Universal Metaphors of Time in English, Mandarin, Hindi, and Sesotho. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Anderson, John (2010). Cognitive Psychology and Its Implications. New York: Worth Publishers.Google Scholar
Anderson, Michael C., Bjork, Elizabeth L., and Bjork, Robert A. (2000). Retrieval-induced forgetting: Evidence for a recall-specific mechanism. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7(3), 522530.Google Scholar
Andrén, Mats (2010). Children’s Gestures from 18 to 30 Months. Lund: Centre for Languages and Literatures.Google Scholar
Arend, Isabel, Henik, Avishai, and Okon-Singer, Hadas (2015). Dissociating emotion and attention functions in the pulvinar nucleus of the thalamus. Neuropsychology, 29(2), 191196.Google Scholar
Armstrong, Natalie, Koteyko, Nelya, and Powell, John (2012). Oh dear, should I really be saying that on here? Issues of identity and authority in an online diabetes community. Health, 16(4), 347365.Google Scholar
Arnheim, Rudolf (1969). Visual Thinking. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Attardo, Salvatore (1994). Linguistic Theories of Humor. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Attardo, Salvatore (2000). Irony as relevant inappropriateness. Journal of Pragmatics, 32(6), 793826.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bak, Per (1997). How Nature Works: The Science of Self-Organized Criticality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bal, Mieke (2009). Narratology (3rd edn.). Toronto: University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
Barcelona, Antonio (ed.) (2000a). Metaphor and Metonymy at the Crossroads: A Cognitive Perspective. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Barcelona, Antonio (2000b). On the plausibility of claiming a metonymic motivation for conceptual metaphor. In Barcelona (ed.), 31–58.Google Scholar
Barcelona, Antonio (2009). Motivation of construction meaning and form: The roles of metonymy and inference. In Panther, Thornburg & Barcelona (eds.), 363–401.Google Scholar
Bargh, John A., Chen, Mark, and Burrows, Lara (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 230244.Google Scholar
Barsalou, Lawrence W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577609.Google Scholar
Barsalou, Lawrence W. (2008a). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617645.Google Scholar
Barsalou, Lawrence W. (2008b). Grounding symbolic operations in the brain’s modal systems. In Semin & Smith (eds.), 9–42.Google Scholar
Barsalou, Lawrence W., Simmons, Kyle, Barbey, Aron K., and Wilson, Christine D. (2003). Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(2), 8491.Google Scholar
Barsalou, Lawrence W., and Wiemer-Hastings, Katja (2005). Situating abstract concepts. In Pecher & Zwaan (eds.), 129–163.Google Scholar
Barton, David (2007). Literacy: An Introduction to the Ecology of Written Language (2nd edn.). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bavelas, Janet, Chovil, Nicole, Coates, Linda, and Roe, Lori (1995). Gestures specialized for dialogue. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(4), 394340.Google Scholar
Bednar, James A., and Wilson, Stuart P. (2015). Cortical maps. The Neuroscientist, 1–14, 10.1177/1073858415597645 [e-pub ahead of print]. Print edn. 2016, 22(6), 604–617.Google Scholar
Bergen, Benjamin K. (2012). Louder than Words: The New Science of How the Mind Makes Meaning. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bergen, Benjamin K. (2015). Embodiment. In Dabrowska, Ewa and Divjak, Dagmar (eds.), Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 1030). Berlin/Boston: de Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Bernárdez, Enrique (2013). Evidentiality and the epistemic use of the Icelandic verbs sjá and heyra: A cultural linguistic view. In Gaz, Adam, Danaher, David, and Łozowski, Przemysław (eds.), The Linguistic Worldview: Ethnolinguistics, Cognition, and Culture (pp. 415442). London: Versita.Google Scholar
Bierwiaczonek, Boguslaw (2013). Metonymy in Language, Thought and Brain. Sheffield: Equinox.Google Scholar
Bird, Chris M., Bisby, James A., and Burgess, Neil (2012). The hippocampus and spatial constraints on mental imagery. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(142), 112. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00142.Google Scholar
Bishop, Bill (2008). The Big Sort: Why Clustering of Like-Minded America is Tearing Us Apart. New York: Houghton-Mifflin.Google Scholar
Black, Max [1979] (1993). More about metaphor. In Ortony (ed.), 19–41.Google Scholar
Blanke, Olaf (2012). Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(8), 556571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boers, Frank (1999). When a bodily source domain becomes prominent. In Gibbs, Jr. & Steen (eds.), 47–56.Google Scholar
Boers, Frank, and Littlemore, Jeannette (2000). Cognitive style variables in participants’ explanations of conceptual metaphors. Metaphor and Symbol, 15(3), 177187.Google Scholar
Boers, Frank, and Littlemore, Jeannette (eds.) (2003). Cross-Cultural Differences in Conceptual Metaphor: Applied Linguistics Perspectives. Special issue of Metaphor and Symbol, 18(4).Google Scholar
Bonnier, Pierre (1905). L’Aschématie. Revue Neurologique (Paris), 13, 605609.Google Scholar
Boot, Inge, and Pecher, Diane (2010). Similarity is closeness: Metaphorical mapping in a conceptual task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(5), 942954.Google Scholar
Bordwell, David (1985). Narration in the Fiction Film. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Bordwell, David, and Thompson, Kirstin (2008). Film Art: An Introduction (8th edn.). Boston: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Borkent, Mike, Dancygier, Barbara, and Hinnell, Jennifer (2013). Language and the Creative Mind. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Boroditsky, Lera (2000). Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75(1), 128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boroditsky, Lera (2001). Does language shape thought? English and Mandarin speakers’ conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43(1), 122.Google Scholar
Boroditsky, Lera, and Ramscar, Michael (2002). The roles of body and mind in abstract thought. Psychological Science, 13(2), 185188.Google Scholar
Boulenger, Véronique, Hauk, Olaf, and Pulvermüller, Friedemann (2009). Grasping ideas with the motor system: Semantic somototopy in idiom comprehension. Cerebral Cortex, 19(8), 19051914.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowdle, Brian F., and Gentner, Dedre (2005). The career of metaphor. Psychological Review, 112(1), 193216.Google Scholar
Bowes, Andrea, and Katz, Albert N. (2015). Metaphor creates intimacy and enhances one’s ability to infer the internal states of others. Memory & Cognition, 43(6), 953963.Google Scholar
Breaux, Brooke O., and Feist, Michele I. (2008). The color of similarity. In Love, B. C., McRae, K., and Sloutsky, V. M. (eds.), Proceedings of the 30th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 253258). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Conference.Google Scholar
Calbris, Geneviève (2011). Elements of Meaning in Gesture. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Cameron, Lynne (1999). Identifying and describing metaphors in spoken discourse data. In Cameron, Lynne and Low, Graham (eds.), Researching and Applying Metaphor (pp. 105132). Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cameron, Lynne (2003). Deliberate and Conventional Metaphors in Educational Discourse. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Cameron, Lynne (2007a). Confrontation or complementarity? Metaphor in language use and cognitive metaphor theory. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 5(1), 107135.Google Scholar
Cameron, Lynne (2007b). Patterns of metaphor use in reconciliation talk. Discourse & Society, 18(2), 197222.Google Scholar
Cameron, Lynne (2008a). Metaphor and talk. In Gibbs, Jr. (ed.), 197–211.Google Scholar
Cameron, Lynne (2008b). Metaphor shifting in the dynamics of talk. In Zanotto, Cameron & Cavalcanti (eds.), 45–62.Google Scholar
Cameron, Lynne (2010a). The affective discourse dynamics of metaphor clustering. Ilha do Desterro: A Journal of English Language, Literatures in English and Cultural Studies, 41–62.Google Scholar
Cameron, Lynne (2010b). The discourse dynamics framework for metaphor. In Cameron & Maslen (eds.), 77–94.Google Scholar
Cameron, Lynne (2010c). Metaphors and discourse activity. In Cameron & Maslen (eds.), 77–94.Google Scholar
Cameron, Lynne (2010d). What is metaphor and why does it matter? In Cameron & Maslen (eds.), 3–25.Google Scholar
Cameron, Lynne (2011). Metaphor and Reconciliation: The Discourse Dynamics of Empathy in Post-Conflict Conversations. London/New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Cameron, Lynne (2016). Mixed metaphor from a discourse dynamics perspective: A non-issue? In Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr. (ed.), Mixing Metaphor (pp. 1730). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Cameron, Lynne, and Deignan, Alice (2003). Combining large and small corpora to investigate tuning devices around metaphor in spoken discourse. Metaphor and Symbol, 18(3), 149160.Google Scholar
Cameron, Lynne, and Deignan, Alice (2006). The emergence of metaphor in discourse Applied Linguistics, 27(4), 671690.Google Scholar
Cameron, Lynne, and Maslen, Robert (eds.) (2010). Metaphor Analysis: Research Practice in Applied Linguistics, Social Sciences and the Humanities. London: Equinox.Google Scholar
Cameron, Lynne, Maslen, Robert, and Low, Graham (2010). Finding systematicity in metaphor use. In Cameron & Maslen (eds.), 116–146.Google Scholar
Cameron, Lynne, Maslen, Robert, Todd, Zazie, Maule, John, Stratton, Peter, and Stanley, Neil (2009). The discourse dynamics approach to metaphor and metaphor-led discourse analysis. Metaphor and Symbol, 24(2), 6389.Google Scholar
Cang, Jianhua, and Feldheim, David A. (2013). Developmental mechanisms of topographic map formation and alignment. Annual Reviews in Neuroscience, 36, 5177.Google Scholar
Carroll, Noël (1996). A note on film metaphor. In Carroll, Noël, Theorizing the Moving Image (pp. 212223). Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Caruana, Fausto, and Cuccio, Valentina (2015). Types of abduction in tool behavior. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Science. doi:10.1007/s11097-015-9450-y.Google Scholar
Casasanto, Daniel (2008a). Similarity and proximity: When does close in space mean close in mind? Memory & Cognition, 36(6), 10471056.Google Scholar
Casasanto, Daniel (2008b). Who’s afraid of the Big Bad Whorf? Cross-linguistic differences in temporal language and thought. Language Learning, 58(177), 6379.Google Scholar
Casasanto, Daniel (2009a). Embodiment of abstract concepts: Good and bad in right- and left-handers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(3), 351367.Google Scholar
Casasanto, Daniel (2009b). When is a linguistic metaphor a conceptual metaphor? In Evans, Vyvyan and Pourcel, Stéphanie (eds.), New Directions in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 127146). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Casasanto, Daniel (2010). Space for thinking. In Evans, Vyvyan and Chilton, Paul (eds.), Language, Cognition and Space: State of the Art and New Directions (pp. 453478). London: Equinox Publishing.Google Scholar
Casasanto, Daniel (2011). Different bodies, different minds: The body-specificity of language and thought. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(6), 378383.Google Scholar
Casasanto, Daniel (2013). Development of metaphorical thinking: The role of language. In Borkent, Dancygier & Hinnell (eds.), 3–18.Google Scholar
Casasanto, Daniel (2014a). Bodily Relativity. In Shapiro (ed.), 108–117.Google Scholar
Casasanto, Daniel (2014b). Experiential origins of mental metaphors: Language, culture, and the body. In Landau, Robinson & Meier (eds.), 249–268.Google Scholar
Casasanto, Daniel, and Boroditsky, Lera (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106(2), 579593.Google Scholar
Casasanto, Daniel, and Bottini, Roberto (2014a). Mirror reading can reverse the flow of time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(2), 473479.Google Scholar
Casasanto, Daniel, and Bottini, Roberto (2014b). Spatial language and abstract concepts. WIREs Cognitive Science, 5(2), 139149.Google Scholar
Casasanto, Daniel, and Chrysikou, Evangelia G. (2011). When left is “right”: Motor fluency shapes abstract concepts. Psychological Science, 22(4), 419422.Google Scholar
Casasanto, Daniel, and Gijssels, Tom (2015). What makes a metaphor an embodied metaphor? Linguistics Vanguard, 1(1), 327337.Google Scholar
Casasanto, Daniel, and Henetz, Tania (2012). Handedness shapes children’s abstract concepts. Cognitive Science, 36(2), 359372.Google Scholar
Casasanto, Daniel, and Jasmin, Kyle (2010). Good and bad in the hands of politicians: Spontaneous gestures during positive and negative speech. Public Library of Science ONE, 5(7), e11805.Google Scholar
Chafe, Wallace (1994). Discourse, Consciousness, and Time: The Flow and Displacement of Conscious Experience in Speaking and Writing. Chicago/London: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Chapple, Alison, and Ziebland, Sue (2004). The role of humor for men with testicular cancer. Qualitative Health Research, 14(8), 11231139.Google Scholar
Chemero, Anthony (2011). Radical Embodied Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chen, Jenn-Yeu (2007). Do Chinese and English speakers think about time differently? Failure of replicating Boroditsky (2001). Cognition 104(2), 427436.Google Scholar
Chiappe, Dan, Kennedy, John, and Smykowski, Tim (2003). Reversibility, aptness and the conventionality of metaphors and similes. Metaphor and Symbol, 18(2), 85106.Google Scholar
Ching, Marvin K. L. (1993). Games and play: Pervasive metaphors in American life. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 8(1), 4365.Google Scholar
Chklovskii, Dmitri B., and Koulakov, Alexei A. (2004). Maps in the brain: What can we learn from them? Annual Reviews in Neuroscience, 27, 369392.Google Scholar
Christakis, Nicholas A., and Fowler, James H. (2009). Connected: The Surprising Power of Our Social Networks and How They Shape Our Lives. New York: Little, Brown and Company.Google Scholar
Chui, Kawai (2011). Conceptual metaphors in gesture. Cognitive Linguistics, 22(3), 437458.Google Scholar
Cienki, Alan (1998). Metaphoric gestures and some of their relations to verbal metaphoric expressions. In Koenig, Jean-Pierre (ed.), Discourse and Cognition (pp. 189204). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Cienki, Alan (2005). Image schemas and gesture. In Hampe (ed.), 421–442.Google Scholar
Cienki, Alan (2010). Multimodal metaphor analysis. In Cameron & Maslen (eds.), 195–216.Google Scholar
Cienki, Alan (2013a). Cognitive Linguistics: Spoken language and gesture as expressions of conceptualization. In Müller, Cienki, Fricke, Ladewig, McNeill & Tessendorf (eds.), 182–201.Google Scholar
Cienki, Alan (2013b). Mimetic schemas and image schemas in cognitive linguistics and gesture studies. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 11(2), 417432.Google Scholar
Cienki, Alan, and Müller, Cornelia (eds.) (2008a). Metaphor and Gesture. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Cienki, Alan, and Müller, Cornelia (2008b). Metaphor, gesture, and thought. In Gibbs, Jr. (ed.), 483–501.Google Scholar
Citron, Francesca, and Goldberg, Adele E. (2014). Social context modulates the effect of physical warmth on perceived interpersonal kindness: A study of embodied metaphors. Language and Cognition, 6(1), 111.Google Scholar
Claridge, Claudia (2011). Hyperbole in English: A Corpus-Based Study of Exaggeration. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, Andy (2008). Supersizing the Mind: Embodiment, Action, and Cognitive Extension. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Andrew, and Chalmers, David (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58(1), 719.Google Scholar
Clark, Herbert H. (1973). Space, time, semantics and the child. In Moore, Timothy E. (ed.), Cognitive Development and the Acquisition of Language (pp. 2763). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Clark, Herbert H., and Gerrig, Richard J. (1984). On the pretense theory of irony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113(1), 121126.Google Scholar
Coates, Jennifer (2007). Talk in a play frame: More on laughter and intimacy. Journal of Pragmatics, 39(1), 2949.Google Scholar
Coëgnarts, Martin, and Kravanja, Peter (2012). Embodied visual meaning: Image schemas in film. Projections, 6(2), 84101.Google Scholar
Cole, Jonathan (1995). Pride and a Daily Marathon. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cole, Jonathan, and Paillard, Jacques (1995). Living without touch and information about body position and movement: Studies on deafferented subjects. In Bermudez, José, Marcel, Anthony, and Eilan, Naomi (eds.), The Body and the Self (pp. 245266). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Connor, Kathleen, and Kogan, Nathan (1980). Topic−vehicle relations in metaphor: The issue of asymmetry. In Honeck, R. P. and Hoffman, R. R. (eds.), Cognition and Figurative Language (pp. 283310). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cook, Susan W., Duffy, Ryan G., and Fenn, Kimberly M. (2013). Consolidation and transfer of learning after observing hand gesture. Child Development, 84(6), 18631871.Google Scholar
Corts, Daniel, and Meyers, Kristina (2002). Conceptual clusters in figurative language production. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31(4), 391408.Google Scholar
Corts, Daniel, and Pollio, Howard (1999). Spontaneous production of figurative language and gesture in college lectures. Metaphor and Symbol, 14(2), 81100.Google Scholar
Costantini, Marcello, Galati, Gaspare, Ferretti, Antonio, Caulo, Massimo, Tartaro, Armando, Romani, Gian Luca, et al. (2005). Neural systems underlying observation of humanly impossible movements: An FMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 15(11), 17611767.Google Scholar
Coulson, Seana (2001). Semantic Leaps: Frame-Shifting and Conceptual Blending in Meaning Construction. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cowley, Stephen J., and Vallée-Tourangeau, Frédéric (eds.) (2013). Cognition beyond the Brain: Computation, Interactivity and Human Artifice. London: Springer.Google Scholar
Croft, William (1993). The role of domains in the interpretation of metaphors and metonymies. Cognitive Linguistics, 4(4), 335370.Google Scholar
Croft, William, and Cruse, Alan (2004). Cognitive Linguistics. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cuccio, Valentina (2014). The notion of representation and the brain. Phenomenology and Mind, 7, 247258.Google Scholar
Cuccio, Valentina (2015). Embodied Simulation and metaphors: Towards a direct role of the body in language comprehension. Epistemologia, 37, 97112.Google Scholar
Cuffari, Elena, and Jensen, Thomas W. (2014). Living bodies: Co-enacting experience. In Müller, Cienki, Fricke, Ladewig, McNeill & Bressem (eds.), 2016–2025.Google Scholar
Cutting, James, Proffitt, Denis, and Kozlowski, Lynn (1978). A biochemical invariant for gait perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4(3), 357372.Google Scholar
Dale, Rick (2015). An Integrative Research Strategy for Exploring Synergies in Natural Language Performance. Ecological Psychology, 27(3), 190201.Google Scholar
Dale, Rick, Fusaroli, Ricardo, Duran, Nicholas, and Richardson, Daniel C. (2013). The self-organization of human interaction. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 59, 4395.Google Scholar
Dancygier, Barbara, and Sweetser, Eve E. (2014). Figurative Language. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dawson, Michael (2014). Embedded and situated cognition. In Shapiro (ed.), 59–68.Google Scholar
de Hevia, Maria D., Izard, Veronique, Coubart, Aurélie, Spelke, Elizabeth S., and Streri, Arlette (2014). Representations of space, time, and number in neonates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(13), 48094813.Google Scholar
de Vignemont, Frédérique (2010). The body-schema and the body-image: Pros and cons. Neuropsychologia, 48(3), 669680.Google Scholar
Deane, Paul D. (1992). Grammar in Mind and Brain: Explorations in Cognitive Syntax. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Deignan, Alice (2003). Metaphorical expressions and culture: An indirect link. Metaphor and Symbol, 18(4), 255271.Google Scholar
Deignan, Alice (2006a). The grammar of linguistic metaphors. In Stefanowitsch & Th. Gries (eds.), 106–122.Google Scholar
Deignan, Alice (2006b). Metaphor in Corpus Linguistics. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Deignan, Alice (2008a). Corpus linguistic data and conceptual metaphor theory. In Zanotto, Cameron & Cavalcanti (eds.), 149–162.Google Scholar
Deignan, Alice (2008b). Corpus linguistics and metaphor. In Gibbs, Jr. (ed.), 280–294.Google Scholar
Deignan, Alice, Littlemore, Jeannette, and Semino, Elena (2013). Figurative Language, Genre and Register. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Deignan, Alice, and Semino, Elena (2010). Corpus techniques for metaphor analysis. In Cameron & Maslen (eds.), 161–179.Google Scholar
Demjén, Zsófia (2016). Laughing at cancer: Humour, empowerment, solidarity and coping online. Journal of Pragmatics, 101, 1830.Google Scholar
Dennet, Daniel (1969). Content and Consciousness. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Denny-Brown, Derek, Meyer, John, and Horenstein, Simon (1952). The significance of perceptual rivalry resulting from from parietal lesion. Brain, 75(4), 433471.Google Scholar
Desai, Rutvik H., Binder, Jeffrey R., Conant, Lisa L., Mano, Quintino R., and Seidenberg, Mark S. (2011). The neural career of sensorimotor metaphors. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(9), 23762386.Google Scholar
di Pellegrino, Guiseppe, Fadiga, Luciano, Fogassi, Leonardo, Gallese, Vittorio, and Rizzolatti, Giacomo (1992). Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91(1), 176180.Google Scholar
Dijkerman, H. Chris, and de Haan, Edward H. F. (2007). Somatosensory processes subserving perception and action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(2), 189201.Google Scholar
Dirven, René, and Pörings, Ralf (eds.) (2002). Metaphor and Metonymy in Comparison and Contrast. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Dodge, Ellen, and Lakoff, George (2005). Image schemas: From linguistic analysis to neural grounding. In Hampe (ed.), 57–91.Google Scholar
Dolscheid, Sarah, Hunnius, Sabine, Casasanto, Daniel, and Majid, Asifa (2014). Prelinguistic infants are sensitive to space-pitch associations found across cultures. Psychological Science, 25(6), 12561261.Google Scholar
Dolscheid, Sarah, Shayan, Shakila, Majid, Asifa, and Casasanto, Daniel (2013). The thickness of musical pitch: Psychophysical evidence for linguistic relativity. Psychological Science, 24(5), 613621.Google Scholar
Dynel, Marta (2009). Creative metaphor is a birthday cake: Metaphor as the source of humour. metaphorik.de, 17, 2748.Google Scholar
Dynel, Marta (2011a). Joker in the pack: Towards determining the status of humorous framing in conversations. In Dynel, Marta (ed.), The Pragmatics of Humour across Discourse Domains (pp. 217242). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Dynel, Marta (2011b). Pragmatics and Linguistic research into Humour. In Dynel, Marta (ed.), The Pragmatics of Humour across Discourse Domains (pp. 112). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Eggertsson, Gunnar T., and Forceville, Charles J. (2009). Multimodal expressions of the HUMAN VICTIM IS ANIMAL metaphor in horror films. In Forceville & Urios-Aparisi (eds.), 429–449.Google Scholar
Eitan, Zohar, and Timmers, Renee (2010). Beethoven’s last piano sonata and those who follow crocodiles: Cross-domain mappings of auditory pitch in a musical context. Cognition, 114(3), 405422.Google Scholar
El Refaie, Elisabeth (2009a). Metaphor in political cartoons: Exploring audience responses. In Forceville & Urios-Aparisi (eds.), 173–196.Google Scholar
El Refaie, Elisabeth (2009b). Multiliteracies: How readers interpret political cartoons. Visual Communication, 8, 181205.Google Scholar
El Refaie, Elisabeth (2014). Appearances and dis/dys-appearances: A dynamic view of embodiment in Conceptual Metaphor Theory. Metaphor and the Social World, 4(1), 109125.Google Scholar
El Refaie, Elisabeth (2017). Multi-modal metaphors. In Semino & Demjén (eds.), 148–161.Google Scholar
Evans, Nicholas, and Wilkins, David (2000). In the mind’s ear: The semantic extensions of perception verbs in Australian languages. Language, 76(3), 546592.Google Scholar
Evans, Vyvyan, and Green, Melanie (2006). Cognitive Linguistics: An Introduction. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Fahlenbrach, Kathrin (ed.) (2015). Embodied Metaphors in Film, Television, and Video Games: Cognitive Approaches. London/New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Fauconnier, Gilles, and Turner, Mark (2002). The Way We Think: Conceptual Blending and the Mind’s Hidden Complexity. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Fauconnier, Gilles, and Turner, Mark (2008). Rethinking metaphor. In Gibbs, Jr. (ed.), 53–66.Google Scholar
Feldman, Jerome, and Narayanan, Srini (2004). Embodied meaning in a neural theory of language. Brain and Language, 89(2), 385392.Google Scholar
Fillmore, Charles J. (1977). Scenes-and-frames semantics. In Zampolli, Antonio (ed.), Linguistic Structures Processing 4 (pp. 5581). Amsterdam: North-Holland Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Fillmore, Charles (1982). Frame semantics. In Linguistic Society of Korea (ed.), Linguistics in the Morning Calm (pp. 111137). Seoul: Hanshin.Google Scholar
Fillmore, Charles (1985). Frames and the semantics of understanding. Quarderni di Semantica, 6(2), 222255.Google Scholar
Fillmore, Charles J., and Baker, Collin F. (2009). A frames approach to semantic analysis. In Heine, Bernd and Narrog, Heiko (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Analysis (pp. 313340). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fischer, Martin H., Dewulf, Nele, and Hill, Robin L. (2005). Designing bar graphs: Orientation matters. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19(7), 953962.Google Scholar
Fodor, Jerry A. (1975). The Language of Thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Forceville, Charles J. (1996). Pictorial Metaphor in Advertising. London/New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Forceville, Charles J. (2002). The identification of target and source in pictorial metaphors. Journal of Pragmatics, 34(1), 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forceville, Charles J. (2006a). Non-verbal and multimodal metaphor in a cognitivist framework: Agendas for research. In Kristiansen, Gitte, Achard, Michel, Dirven, René, and Ruiz de Mendoza Ibañez, Francisco (eds.), Cognitive Linguistics: Current Applications and Future Perspectives (pp. 379402). Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Forceville, Charles J. (2006b). The source-path-goal schema in the autobiographical journey documentary: McElwee, Van der Keuken, Cole. New Review of Film and Television Studies, 4(3), 241261.Google Scholar
Forceville, Charles J. (2008). Metaphor in pictures and multimodal representations. In Gibbs, Jr. (ed.), 462–482.Google Scholar
Forceville, Charles J. (2009a). Metonymy in visual and audiovisual discourse. In Ventola, Eija and Guijarro, Arsenio Jésus Moya (eds.), The World Told and the World Shown: Issues in Multisemiotics (pp. 5674). Basingstoke/New York: Palgrave MacMillan.Google Scholar
Forceville, Charles J. (2009b). The role of non-verbal sound and music in multimodal metaphor. In Forceville & Urios-Aparisi (eds.), 383–402.Google Scholar
Forceville, Charles J. (2011). The source-path-goal schema in Agnès Varda’s Les Glaneurs et la Glaneuse and Deux Ans Après. In Fludernik, Monika (ed.), Beyond Cognitive Metaphor Theory: Perspectives on Literary Metaphor (pp. 281297). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Forceville, Charles J. (2013). Metaphor and symbol: SEARCHING FOR ONE’S IDENTITY IS LOOKING FOR A HOME in animation film. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 11(2), 250268.Google Scholar
Forceville, Charles J. (2014). Relevance Theory as model for analysing multimodal communication. In Machin, David (ed.), Visual Communication (pp. 5170). Berlin/Boston: de Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Forceville, Charles J. (2016). The force and balance schemas in journey metaphor animations. In Fernandes, Carla (ed.), Multimodality and Performance (pp. 822). Newcastle-upon-Tyne: Cambridge Scholar Press.Google Scholar
Forceville, Charles J., and Jeulink, Marloes (2011). The flesh and blood of embodied understanding: The source-path-goal schema in animation film. Pragmatics & Cognition, 19(1), 3759.Google Scholar
Forceville, Charles J., and Renckens, Thijs (2013). The GOOD IS LIGHT and BAD IS DARK metaphors in feature films. Metaphor and the Social Word, 3(2), 160179.Google Scholar
Forceville, Charles J., and Urios-Aparisi, Eduardo (eds.) (2009). Multimodal Metaphor. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Frisson, Steven, and Pickering, Martin (1999). The processing of metonymy: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(6), 13661383.Google Scholar
Fuhrman, Orly, and Boroditsky, Lera (2010). Cross-cultural differences in mental representations of time: Evidence from an implicit non-linguistic task. Cognitive Science, 34(8), 14301451.Google Scholar
Fusaroli, Ricardo, Rączaszek-Leonardi, Joanna, and Tylén, Kristian (2014). Dialog as interpersonal synergy. New Ideas in Psychology, 32, 147157.Google Scholar
Gallagher, Shaun (1986). Body image and body schema: A conceptual clarification. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 7, 541554.Google Scholar
Gallagher, Shaun (2005). How the Body Shapes the Mind. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gallagher, Shaun, and Cole, Jonathan (1995). Body image and body schema in a deafferented subject. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 16(4), 369389.Google Scholar
Gallagher, Shaun, and Zahavi, Dan (2008). The Phenomenological Mind (2nd edn.). London/New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Gallese, Vittorio (2008). Mirror neurons and the social nature of language: The neural exploitation hypothesis. Social Neuroscience, 3(3–4), 317333.Google Scholar
Gallese, Vittorio (2009). Mirror neurons and the neural exploitation hypothesis: From embodied simulation to social cognition. In Pineda, Jaime A. (ed.), Mirror Neuron Systems (pp. 163190). New York: Humana Press.Google Scholar
Gallese, Vittorio, and Cuccio, Valentina (2016). The paradigmatic body: Embodied simulation, intersubjectivity, the bodily self and language. In Metzinger, Thomas and Windt, Jennifer (eds.), Open MIND. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gallese, Vittorio, and Lakoff, George (2005). The Brain’s concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22(3), 455479.Google Scholar
Gallese, Vittorio, and Sinigaglia, Corrado (2011). What is so special about embodied simulation? Trends in Cognitive Science, 15(11), 512519.Google Scholar
Geeraerts, Dirk, and Cuyckens, Hubert (eds.) (2007). The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Geng, Joy J., and Vossel, Simone (2013). Re-evaluating the role of TPJ in attentional control: Contextual updating? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(10–2), 26082620.Google Scholar
Gentner, Dedre, and Bowdle, Brian F. (2008). Metaphor as structure mapping. In Gibbs, Jr. (ed.), 3–13.Google Scholar
Gentner, Dedre, Imai, Mutsumi, and Boroditsky, Lera (2002). As time goes by: Evidence for two systems in processing space → time metaphors. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17(5), 537565.Google Scholar
Gernsbacher, Morton A., Keysar, Boaz, Robertson, Rachel R. W., and Werner, Necia K. (2001). The role of suppression in understanding metaphors. Journal of Memory and Language, 45(3), 433450.Google Scholar
Gerrig, Richard J. (1993). Experiencing Narrative Worlds: On the Psychological Activities of Reading. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr. (1991). Semantic analyzability in children’s understanding of idioms. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34(3), 613620.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (1992). What do idioms really mean? Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 485506.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (1994). The Poetics of Mind: Figurative Thought, Language, and Understanding. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (1999). Taking metaphor out of our heads and putting it into the cultural world. In Gibbs, Jr. & Steen (eds.), pp. 145–166.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2003). Embodied experience and linguistic meaning. Brain and Language, 84(1), 115.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2005a). Embodiment in Metaphorical Imagination. In Pecher & Zwaan (eds.), 65–92.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2005b). The psychological status of image schemas. In Hampe (ed.), 113–135.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2006a). Embodiment and Cognitive Science. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2006b). Metaphor interpretation as embodied simulation. Mind & Language, 21(3), 434458.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2007a). Idioms and formulaic language. In Geeraerts & Guyckens (eds.), 697–725.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2007b). Why cognitive linguists should care more about empirical methods. In Gonzalez-Marquez, Mittelberg, Coulson & Spivey (eds.), 2–18.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W.(ed.) (2008). The Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2011a). The allegorical impulse. Metaphor and Symbol, 26(2), 121130.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2011b). Are “deliberate” metaphors really deliberate? A question of human consciousness and action. Metaphor and the Social World, 1(1), 2652.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2011c). Evaluating Conceptual Metaphor Theory. Discourse Processes, 48(8), 529562.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2012). Metaphors, snowflakes, and termite nests: How nature creates such beautiful things. In MacArthur, Oncins-Martínez, Sanchez-García & Piquer-Píriz (eds.), 347–371.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2013a). Metaphoric cognition as social activity: Dissolving the divide between metaphor in thought and communication. Metaphor and the Social World, 3(1), 5476.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2013b). Walking the walk while thinking about the talk: Embodied interpretation of metaphorical narratives. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 42(4), 363378.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2014a). Conceptual metaphor in thought and social action. In Landau, Robinson & Meier (eds.), 17–40.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2014b). Embodied metaphor. In Littlemore & Taylor (eds.), 167–184.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2015a). The allegorical character of political metaphors in discourse. Metaphor and the Social World, 5(2), 264282.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2015b). Do pragmatic signals affect conventional metaphor understanding? A failed test of Deliberate Metaphor Theory. Journal of Pragmatics, 90, 7787.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2015c). Does deliberate metaphor theory have a future? Journal of Pragmatics, 90, 7376.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2015d). Metaphor. In Dabrowska, Ewa and Divjak, Dagmar (eds.), Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 167189). Berlin/Boston: de Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2017a). Metaphor Wars: Conceptual Metaphor in Human Life. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. (2017b). Metaphor, language and dynamical systems. In Semino & Demjén (eds.), 56–69.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr., Bogdanovich, Josephine M., Sykes, Jeffrey R., and Barr, Dale J. (1997). Metaphor in idiom comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 37(2), 141154.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr., and Bogdonovich, Jody (1999). Mental imagery in interpreting poetic metaphor. Metaphor and Symbol, 14(1), 3744.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr., and Cameron, Lynne (2008). The social-cognitive dynamics of metaphor performance. Journal of Cognitive Systems Research, 9(1–2), 6475.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr., and Colston, Herbert L. (2012). Interpreting Figurative Meaning. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr., Gould, Jessica, and Andric, Michael (2006). Imagining metaphorical actions: Embodied simulations make the impossible plausible. Imagination, Cognition, & Personality, 25(3), 221238.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr., Lima, Paula L. C., and Francuzo, Edson (2004). Metaphor is grounded in embodied experience. Journal of Pragmatics, 36(7), 11891210.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr., and Lonergan, Julia E. (2009). Studying metaphor in discourse: Some lessons, challenges and new data. In Musolff & Zinken (eds.), 251–261.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr., and Matlock, Teenie (2008). Metaphor, imagination, and simulation: Psycholinguistic evidence. In Gibbs, Jr. (ed.), 161–176.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr., and O’Brien, Jennifer (1990). Idioms and mental imagery: The metaphorical motivation for idiomatic meaning. Cognition, 36(1), 3568.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr., and Santa Cruz, Malaika J. (2012). Temporal unfolding of conceptual metaphor experience. Metaphor and Symbol, 27(4), 299311.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr., and Steen, Gerard J. (eds.) (1999). Metaphor in Cognitive Linguistics. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr., and Tendahl, Markus (2008). Complementary perspectives on metaphor: Cognitive linguistics and relevance theory. Journal of Pragmatics, 40(11), 18231864.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr., and Van Orden, Guy (2012). Pragmatic choice in conversation. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(1), 720.Google Scholar
Gibson, James J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Giora, Rachel (1997). Understanding figurative and literal language: The graded salience hypothesis. Cognitive Linguistics, 8(3), 183206.Google Scholar
Giora, Rachel (2003). On Our Mind: Salience, Context, and Figurative Language. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Giora, Rachel (2008). Is metaphor unique? In Gibbs, Jr. (ed.), 143–160.Google Scholar
Glenberg, Arthur M. (1999). Why mental models need to be embodied. In Rickheit, Gert and Habel, Christopher (eds.), Mental Models in Discourse Processing (pp. 7790). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Glenberg, Arthur M., and Robertson, David (2000). Symbol grounding and meaning: A comparison of high dimensional and embodied theories of meaning. Journal of Memory and Language, 43(3), 379401.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, Sam (2001). Understanding Figurative Language: From Metaphors to Idioms. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, Sam (2008). How metaphors create categories – quickly. In Gibbs (ed.), 67–83.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, Sam, Brown, Mary, and McGlone, Matthew S. (1993). Conceptual metaphors are not automatically accessed during idiom comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 21(5), 711719.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, Sam, Gildea, Patricia, and Bookin, Howard B. (1982). On understanding nonliteral speech: Can people ignore metaphors? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21(1), 8598.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, Sam, and Keysar, Boaz (1990). Understanding metaphorical comparisons: Beyond similarity. Psychological Review, 97, 318.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, Sam, Keysar, Boaz, and McGlone, Matthew S. (1992). Metaphor understanding and accessing conceptual schema: Reply to Gibbs (1992). Psychological Review, 99(3), 578581.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, Sam, McGlone, Matthew S., and Manfredi, Deanna (1997). Property attribution in metaphor comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 36(1), 5067.Google Scholar
Goatly, Andrew (1997). The Language of Metaphors. London/New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Goldberg, Adele E. (1995). Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure. Chicago/London: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Gong, Shu-Ping, and Ahrens, Kathleen (2007). Processing conceptual metaphors in ongoing discourse. Metaphor and Symbol, 22(4), 313330.Google Scholar
Gonzalez-Marquez, Monica, Mittelberg, Irene, Coulson, Seana, and Spivey, Michael J. (eds.) (2007). Methods in Cognitive Linguistics. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Goodman, Nelson [1955] (1983). Fact, Fiction, and Forecast. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goodwin, Charles (1981). Conversational Organization: Interaction between Speakers and Hearers. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Goodwin, Charles (2011a). Building action in public environments with diverse semiotic resources. Versus: quaderni di studi semiotici (112–113), 169–182. Special issue: The External Mind: Perspectives on Semiosis, Distribution and Situation in Cognition, ed. Riccardo Fusaroli, Tommaso Granelli & Claudio Paolucci.Google Scholar
Goodwin, Charles (2011b). Contextures of action. In Streeck, Jürgen, Goodwin, Charles, and LeBaron, Curtis (eds.), Embodied Interaction: Language and Body in the Material World (pp. 182193). Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goossens, Louis (1990). Metaphtonymy: The interaction of metaphor and metonymy in expressions for linguistic actions. Cognitive Linguistics, 1(3), 323340.Google Scholar
Grady, Joseph E. (1997a). Foundations of meaning: Primary metaphors and primary scenes. Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Linguistics, University of California at Berkeley.Google Scholar
Grady, Joseph E. (1997b). THEORIES ARE BUILDINGS re-visited. Cognitive Linguistics, 8(4), 267290.Google Scholar
Grady, Joseph E. (1998). The “Conduit Metaphor” revisited: A reassessment of metaphors for communication. In Koenig, Jean-Pierre (ed.), Discourse and Cognition: Bridging the Gap (pp. 205218). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Grady, Joseph E. (1999). A typology of motivation for conceptual metaphor: Correlation vs. resemblance. In Gibbs, Jr. & Steen (eds.), 79–100.Google Scholar
Grady, Joseph E. (2005a). Image schemas and perception: Refining a definition. In Hampe (ed.), 35–56.Google Scholar
Grady, Joseph E. (2005b). Primary metaphors as inputs to conceptual integration. Journal of Pragmatics, 37(10), 15951614.Google Scholar
Grady, Joseph E. (2008). “Superschemas” and the grammar of metaphorical mappings. In Tyler, Andrea, Kim, Yiyoung, and Takada, Mari (eds.), Language in the Context of Use: Discourse and Cognitive Approaches to Language (pp. 339360). Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Grady, Joseph E., and Johnson, Christopher (2002). Converging evidence for the notions of “subscene” and “primary scene.” In Dirven & Pörings (eds.), 533–554.Google Scholar
Grady, Joseph E., Oakley, Todd, and Coulson, Seana (1999). Blending and Metaphor. In Gibbs, Jr. & Steen (eds.), 101–124.Google Scholar
Grady, Joseph E., Taub, Sarah, and Morgan, Pamela S. (1996). Primitive and compound metaphors. In Goldberg, Adele E. (ed.), Conceptual Structure, Discourse and Language (pp. 17187). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Grandhi, Sukeshini A., Joue, Gina, and Mittelberg, Irene (2011). Understanding naturalness and intuitiveness in gesture production: Insights for touchless gestural interfaces Proceedings of the ACM’s 2011 Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI), Vancouver, BC (pp. 821824). New York: ACM Press.Google Scholar
Grandhi, Sukeshini A., Joue, Gina, and Mittelberg, Irene (2012). To move or to remove? A human-centric approach to understanding of gesture interpretation Proceedings of the 10th ACM Conference on Designing Interactive Systems (DIS) (pp. 582591). Newcastle/New York: ACM Press.Google Scholar
Graziano, Michael S., and Aflalo, Tyson N. (2007). Mapping behavioral repertoire onto the cortex. Neuron, 56(2), 239251.Google Scholar
Green, Melanie C. (2004). Transportation into narrative worlds: The role of prior knowledge and perceived realism. Discourse Processes, 38(2), 247266.Google Scholar
Guerra, Ernesto, and Knoeferle, Pia (2012). Abstract language comprehension is incrementally modulated by non-referential spatial information: Evidence from eye-tracking. In Carlson, Laura, Hölscher, Christoph, and Shipley, Thomas F. (eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 16201625). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Halliday, Michael A. K. (1985). An Introduction to Functional Grammar. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Hampe, Beate (ed.) (2005a). From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Hampe, Beate (2005b). Image schemas in cognitive linguistics: Introduction. In Hampe (ed.), 1–12.Google Scholar
Hampe, Beate (2005c). On the role of iconic motivation in conceptual metaphor: Has metaphor theory come full circle? In Maeder, Constantino, Fischer, Olga, and Herlofsky, William J. (eds.), Outside-In – Inside-Out (pp. 3966). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hampe, Beate (2005d). When down is not bad and up not good enough: A usage-based assessment of the plus-minus parameter in image-schema theory. Cognitive Linguistics, 16(1), 81112.Google Scholar
Hampes, William P. (1992). Relation between intimacy and humour. Psychological Reports, 71(1), 127130.Google Scholar
Hanks, Patrick (2006). Metaphoricity is gradable. In Stefanowitsch & Th. Gries (eds.), 17–35.Google Scholar
Hanne, Michael (2015). An introduction to the Warring with Words project. In Hanne, Michael, Crano, William D., and Mio, Jeffery Scott (eds.), Warring with Words: Narrative and Metaphor in Politics (pp. 150). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Hartley, Tom, Lever, Colin, Burgess, Neil, and O’Keefe, John (2014). Space in the brain: How the hippocampal formation supports spatial cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1635), 20120510.Google Scholar
Hartmann, Matthias, Gashaj, Verena, Stahnke, Antje, and Mast, Fred W. (2014). There is more than “more is up”: Hand and foot responses reverse the vertical association of number magnitudes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(4), 1401.Google Scholar
Haser, Verena (2005). Metaphor, Metonymy, and Experientialist Philosophy: Challenging Cognitive Semantics. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Hauk, Olaf, Johnsrude, Ingrid, and Pulvermüller, Friedemann (2004). Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron, 41(2), 301307.Google Scholar
Heath, Robin, and Blonder, Lee X. (2003). Conversational humour among stroke survivors. Humor – International Journal of Humor Research, 16(1), 91106.Google Scholar
Hebb, Donald O. (1949). The Organization of Behavior: A Neuropsychological Theory. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Held, Richard, and Hein, Alan (1963). Movement-produced stimulation in the development of visually guided behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56(5), 872876.Google Scholar
Holmes, Kevin J., and Lourenco, Stella F. (2011). Horizontal trumps vertical in the spatial organization of numerical magnitude. In Carlson, Laura, Hölscher, Christoph, and Shipley, Thomas F. (eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 22762281). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Hopper, Paul J., and Thompson, Sandra A. (1980). Transitivity in grammar and discourse. Language, 56(2), 251299.Google Scholar
Horst, Dorothea, Boll, Franziska, Schmitt, Christina, and Müller, Cornelia (2014). Gesture as interactive expressive movement: Inter-affectivity in face-to-face communication. In Müller, Cienki, Fricke, Ladewig, McNeill & Bressem (eds.), 2112–2124.Google Scholar
Horton, William S. (2007). Metaphor and readers’ attributions of intimacy. Memory & Cognition, 35(1), 8794.Google Scholar
Horton, William S. (2013). Character intimacy influences the processing of metaphoric utterances during narrative comprehension. Metaphor and Symbol, 28(3), 148166.Google Scholar
Hostetter, Autumn, and Alibali, Martha (2008). Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(3), 495514.Google Scholar
Hunston, Susan (2013). Corpus Approaches to Evaluation: Phraseology and Evaluative Language. London/New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hutchins, Edwin (1995). Cognition in the Wild. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hutchins, Edwin (2005). Material anchors for conceptual blends. Journal of Pragmatics, 37(10), 15551577.Google Scholar
Hutchins, Edwin (2010). Cognitive ecology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2(4), 705715.Google Scholar
Ibarretxe-Antuñano, Iraide (2013). The relationship between conceptual metaphor and culture. Intercultural Pragmatics, 10(2), 315339.Google Scholar
IJzerman, Hans, and Koole, Sander (2011). From perceptual rags to metaphoric riches: Bodily, social, and cultural constraints on socio-cognitive metaphors. Psychological Bulletin, 137(2), 355361.Google Scholar
IJzerman, Hans, and Semin, Gün R. (2009). The thermometer of social relations mapping social proximity on temperature. Psychological Science, 20(10), 12141220.Google Scholar
Ito, Yashuhiro, and Hatta, Takeshi (2004). Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Memory & Cognition, 32(4), 662673.Google Scholar
Jakobson, Roman (1956). Two aspects of language and two types of aphasic disturbances. In Monville-Burston, Monique and Waugh, Linda (eds.), Roman Jakobson: On Language (pp. 115133). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Jakobson, Roman (1960). Linguistics and poetics. In Pomorska, Krystyna and Rudy, Stephen (eds.), Roman Jakobson: Language in Literature (pp. 6294). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Jarvilehto, Timo (2000). Feeling as knowing – Part I: Emotion as reorganization of the organism-environment system. Consciousness & Emotion, 1(2), 245257.Google Scholar
Jefferson, Gail (2004). Glossary of Transcript Symbols with an Introduction. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Jefferson, Gail, Sacks, Harvey, and Schegloff, Emanuel (1987). Notes on laughter in the pursuit of intimacy. In Button, Graham and Lee, John R. E. (eds.), Talk and Social Organisation (pp. 152205). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Jensen, Thomas W. (2014). New perspectives on language, cognition, and values. Journal of Multicultural Discourses, 9(1), 7178.Google Scholar
Jensen, Thomas W., and Cuffari, Elena (2014). Doubleness in experience: Toward a distributed enactive approach to metaphoricity. Metaphor and Symbol, 29(4), 278297.Google Scholar
Johnson, Mark (1987). The Body in the Mind: The Bodily Basis of Meaning, Imagination, and Reason. Chicago/London: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, Mark J (2005). The philosophical significance of image schemas. In Hampe (ed.), 15–33.Google Scholar
Johnson, Mark J (2007). The Meaning of the Body: Aesthetics of Human Understanding. Chicago/London: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Jostmann, Nils B., Lakens, Daniel, and Schubert, Thomas W. (2009). Weight as an embodiment of importance. Psychological Science, 20(9), 11691174.Google Scholar
Kanold, Patrick O., Nelken, Israel, and Polley, Daniel B. (2014). Local versus global scales of organization in auditory cortex. Trends in Neuroscience, 37(9), 502510.Google Scholar
Kappelhoff, Hermann, and Greifenstein, Sarah (2015). Audiovisual metaphors: Embodied meaning and fictionalization. In Fahlenbach, Kathrin (ed.), Embodied Metaphors in Film, Television, and Video Games: Cognitive Approaches (pp. 183201). London/New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Kappelhoff, Hermann, and Müller, Cornelia (2011). Embodied meaning construction: Multimodal metaphor and expressive movement in speech, gesture, and in feature film. Metaphor and the Social Word, 1(2), 121153.Google Scholar
Kaschak, Michael P., Jones, John L., Carranza, Julie, and Fox, Melissa R. (2014). Embodiment and language comprehension. In Shapiro (ed.), 118–126.Google Scholar
Katz, Albert N., Cacciari, Cristina, Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr., and Turner, Mark (eds.) (1998). Figurative Language and Thought. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Katz, Albert N., and Taylor, Tamsen E. (2008). The journeys of life: Examining a conceptual metaphor with semantic and episodic memory recall. Metaphor and Symbol, 2(3), 148173.Google Scholar
Keil, Frank C. (1979). Semantic and Conceptual Development: An Ontological Perspective. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kendon, Adam (2004). Gesture: Visual Action as Utterance. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Keysar, Boaz, Shen, Yeshayahu, Glucksberg, Sam, and Horton, William S. (2000). Conventional language: How metaphorical is it? Journal of Memory and Language, 43(4), 576593.Google Scholar
Kintsch, Walter (1998). Comprehension: A Paradigm for Cognition. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kintsch, Walter (2008). How the mind computes the meaning of metaphor: A simulation based on LSA. In Gibbs, Jr. (ed.), 129–142.Google Scholar
Kirkham, Natasha Z., Slemmer, Jonathan A., and Johnson, Scott P. (2002). Visual statistical learning in infancy: Evidence for a domain general learning mechanism. Cognition, 83(2), B35B42.Google Scholar
Kirsh, David (1995). The intelligent use of space. Artificial Intelligence, 73(1), 3168.Google Scholar
Kohler, Evelyne, Keysars, Christian, Umilta, M. Alessandra, Forgassi, Leonardo, Gallese, Vittorio, Rizzolatti, Giacomo (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action representation in mirror neurons. Science, 297, 846848.Google Scholar
Koller, Veronika (2008). Brothers in arms: Contradictory metaphors in contemporary marketing discourse. In Zanotto, Cameron & Cavalcanti (eds.), 103–127.Google Scholar
Kolter, Astrid, Ladewig, Silvia H., Summa, Michaela, Koch, Sabine, Müller, Cornelia, and Fuchs, Thomas (2012). Body memory and the emergence of metaphor in movement and speech: An interdisciplinary case study. In Koch, Sabine, Fuchs, Thomas, Summa, Michaela, and Müller, Cornelia (eds.), Body Memory, Metaphor and Movement (pp. 201226). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Kotthoff, Helga (2006). Pragmatics of performance and the analysis of conversational humor. Humor – International Journal of Humor Research, 19(3), 271304.Google Scholar
Kounios, John, and Beeman, Mark (2014). The cognitive neuroscience of insight. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 7193.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Zoltán (2002). Metaphor: A Practical Introduction (2nd edn.). Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Zoltán (2005). Metaphor in Culture: Universality and Variation. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Zoltán (2009). Metaphor, culture, and discourse: The pressure of coherence. In Musolff & Zinken (eds.), 11–24.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Zoltán (2010a). Metaphor and culture. Acta Universitatis Sapientiae, Philologica, 2(2), 197220.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Zoltán (2010b). Metaphor: A Practical Introduction (2nd edn.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Zoltán (2013). The metaphor–metonymy relationship: Correlation metaphors are based on metonymy. Metaphor and Symbol, 28(2), 7588.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Zoltán (2015). Where Metaphors Come From: Reconsidering Context in Metaphor. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Zoltán, and Radden, Günter (1998). Metonymy: Developing a cognitive linguistic view. Cognitive Linguistics, 9(1), 3777.Google Scholar
Kromhout, Roelf, and Forceville, Charles (2013). LIFE IS A JOURNEY: The source-path-goal schema in the videogames “Half-Life 2,” “Heavy Rain,” and “Grim Fandango.” Metaphor and the Social World, 3(1), 100116.Google Scholar
Ladewig, Silvia H. (2011). Putting a recurrent gesture on a cognitive basis. CogniTextes, 6, http://cognitextes.revues.org/406.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George (1987). Woman, Fire, and Dangerous Things. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George (1990). The Invariance Hypothesis: Is abstract reason based on image-schemas? Cognitive Linguistics, 1(1), 3974.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George (1993). The contemporary theory of metaphor. In Ortony (ed.), 202–251.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George (2008a). The neural theory of metaphor. In Gibbs, Jr. (ed.), 17–38.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George (2008b). The Political Mind. New York: Penguin.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George (2012). Explaining embodied cognition results. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(1), 113.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George (2014). Mapping the brain’s metaphor circuitry: Metaphorical thought in everyday reason. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 114.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George, and Johnson, Mark (1980). Metaphors We Live By. Chicago/London: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George, and Johnson, Mark (1999). Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind and Its Challenge to Western Thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George, and Johnson, Mark (2003). Afterword. In Metaphors We live By (pp. 243276). Chicago/London: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George, and Núñez, Rafael (2000). Where Mathematics Comes From: How the Embodied Mind Brings Mathematics Into Being. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George, and Turner, Mark (1989). More than Cool Reason: A Field Guide to Poetic Metaphor. Chicago/London: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Landau, Barbara, and Lakusta, Laura (2009). Spatial representation across species: Geometry, language, and maps. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 19(1), 1219.Google Scholar
Landau, Mark J., Meier, Brian P., and Keefer, Lukas A. (2010). A metaphor-enriched social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 10451067.Google Scholar
Landau, Mark J., Robinson, Michael D., and Meier, Brian P. (2014a). Introduction. In Landau, Robinson & Meier (eds.), 3–16.Google Scholar
Landau, Mark J., Robinson, Michael D., and Meier, Brian P. (eds.) (2014b). The Power of Metaphor: Examining Its Influence on Social Life. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Landauer, Thomas K., and Dumais, Susan T. (1997). A solution to Plato’s problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104(2), 211240.Google Scholar
Langacker, Ronald W. (1987). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar. Vol. I: Theoretical Prerequisites. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, Ronald W. (1991). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar. Vol. II: Descriptive Application. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, Ronald W. (2000). A dynamic usage-based model. In Barlow, Michael and Kemmer, Suzanne (eds.), Usage-Based Models of Language (pp. 163). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Langacker, Ronald W. (2008). Cognitive Grammar: A Basic Introduction. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Larsen-Freeman, Diane, and Cameron, Lynne (2008). Complex Systems and Applied Linguistics. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lee, Spike W. S., and Schwarz, Norbert (2012). Bidirectionality, mediation, and moderation of metaphorical effects: The embodiment of social suspicion and fishy smells. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(5), 737749.Google Scholar
Levin, Magnus (2008). “Hitting the back of the net just before the final whistle”: High-frequency phrase in football reporting. In Lavric, Eva and Pisek, Gerhard (eds.), The Linguistics of Football (pp. 143155). Tübingen: Gunter Narr.Google Scholar
Linell, Per (2005). The Written Language Bias in Linguistics: Its Nature, Origins, and Transformations. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Linell, Per (2009). Rethinking Language, Mind, and World Dialogically: Interactional and Contextual Theories of Human Sense-Making. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publications.Google Scholar
Littlemore, Jeannette (2015). Metonymy: Hidden Shortcuts in Language, Thought and Communication. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Littlemore, Jeannette, and Taylor, John R. (eds.) (2014), The Bloomsbury Companion to Cognitive Lingustics. London: Blooomsbury.Google Scholar
Loetscher, Tobias, Bockisch, Christopher, Nicholls, Michael E. R., and Brugger, Peter (2010). Eye position predicts what number you have in mind. Current Biology, 20(6), R264–265.Google Scholar
Lourenco, Stella F., and Longo, Matthew R. (2011). Origins and the development of generalized magnitude representation. In Dehaene, Stanislas and Brannon, Elizabeth M. (eds.), Space, Time, and Number in the Brain: Searching for the Foundations of Mathematical Thought (pp. 225244). London: Academic Press, Elsevier.Google Scholar
Low, Graham, Littlemore, Jeanette, and Koester, Almut (2008). The use of metaphor in three university lectures. Applied Linguistics, 29(3), 428455.Google Scholar
Lucas, Margery (2007). Semantic priming without association: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7(4), 618630.Google Scholar
Lupyan, Gary (2012). What do words do? Toward a theory of language-augmented thought. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 57, 255297.Google Scholar
Lupyan, Gary, Rakison, David H., and McClelland, James L. (2007). Language is not just for talking: Redundant labels facilitate learning of novel categories. Psychological Science, 18(2), 10771083.Google Scholar
MacArthur, Fiona, and Oncins-Martínez, José Luis (2012). Introduction: Metaphor in use. In MacArthur, Oncins-Martínez, Sanchez-García & Piquer-Píriz (eds.), 1–17.Google Scholar
MacArthur, Fiona, Oncins-Martínez, José Luis, Sanchez-García, Manuel, and Piquer-Píriz, Ana María (eds.) (2012). Metaphor in Use: Context, Culture, and Communication (Vol. 38). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Macnamara, Diana S., and Magliano, Joe (2009). Toward a comprehensive model of comprehension. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 51, 297384.Google Scholar
Mahlberg, Michaela (2013). Corpus Stylistics and Dickens’s Fiction. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Mandler, Jean, and Pagán Cánovas, Cristóbal (2014). On defining image schemas. Language and Cognition, 0, 123.Google Scholar
Marghetis, Tyler (2015). Every number in its place: The spatial foundations of calculation and conceptualization. Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, San Diego.Google Scholar
Marghetis, Tyler, and Youngstrom, Kendall (2014). Pierced by the number-line: Integers are associated with back-to-front sagittal space. In Bello, Paul, Guarini, Marcello, McShane, Marjorie, and Scassellati, Brian (eds.), Proceedings of the 36th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 946951). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Martin, Rod A. (2007). The Psychology of Humor: An Integrative Approach. Burlington, MA/London: Elsevier Academic Press.Google Scholar
Martínez, María-Ángelez, Kraljevic Mujic, Blanca, and Hidalgo-Downing, Laura (2013). Multimodal narrativity in TV ads. In Pennock-Speck, Barry and del Saz-Rubio, María M. (eds.), The Multimodal Analysis of Television Commercials (pp. 91111). València: PUV Universitat de València.Google Scholar
Mashal, Nira, Shen, Yeshayahu, Jospe, Karine, and Gil, David (2014). Language effects on the conceptualization of hybrids. Language and Cognition, 6(2), 217241.Google Scholar
Maslen, Robert (2017). Systematic metaphors. In Semino & Demjén (eds.), 88–101.Google Scholar
Matlock, Teenie (2004a). The conceptual motivation of fictive motion. In Radden, Günter and Dirven, René (eds.), Motivation in Grammar. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Matlock, Teenie (2004b). Fictive motion as cognitive simulation. Memory & Cognition, 32(8), 13891400.Google Scholar
Matlock, Teenie (2010). Abstract motion is no longer abstract. Language and Cognition, 2(2), 243260.Google Scholar
Matlock, Teenie, Holmes, Kevin J., Srinivasan, Mahesh, and Ramscar, Michael (2011). Even abstract motion influences the understanding of time. Metaphor and Symbol, 26(4), 260271.Google Scholar
Matlock, Teenie, Ramscar, Michael, and Boroditsky, Lera (2005). The experiential link between spatial and temporal language. Cognitive Science, 29(4), 655664.Google Scholar
Matthews, Justin L., and Matlock, Teenie (2011). Understanding the link between spatial distance and social distance. Social Psychology, 42(3), 185192.Google Scholar
McCreaddie, May, and Wiggins, Sally (2008). The purpose and function of humour in health, health care and nursing: A narrative review. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 61(6), 584595.Google Scholar
McGlone, Matthew S. (1996). Conceptual metaphors and figurative language interpretation: Food for thought? Journal of Memory and Language, 35(4), 544565.Google Scholar
McGlone, Matthew S. (2007). What is the explanatory value of a conceptual metaphor? Language & Communication, 27(2), 109126.Google Scholar
McGlone, Matthew S., and Harding, Jennifer (1998). Back (or forward?) to the future: The role of perspective in temporal language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 24(5), 12111223.Google Scholar
McNaughton, Bruce L., Barnes, Carol A., Gerrard, Jason L., Gothard, Katalin, Jung, Min W., Knierim, James J., et al. (1996). Deciphering the hippocampal polyglot: The hippocampus as a path integration system. Journal of Experimental Biology, 199(1), 173185.Google Scholar
McNeill, David (1992). Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal about Thought. Chicago/London: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, David (2005). Gesture and Thought. Chicago/London: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, Andrew N., and Moore, M. Keith (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198(4312), 7578.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, Andrew N., and Moore, M. Keith (1989). Imitation in newborn infants: Exploring the range of gestures imitated and the underlying mechanisms. Developmental Psychology, 25(6), 954962.Google Scholar
Merlau-Ponty, Maurice (1945). Phénoménologie de la Perception. Paris: Gallimard.Google Scholar
Merritt, Dustin J., Casasanto, Daniel, and Brannon, Elizabeth M. (2010). Do monkeys think in metaphors? Representations of space and time in monkeys and humans. Cognition, 117(2), 191202.Google Scholar
Miall, Chris, and Wolpert, Daniel (1996). Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Networks, 9, 12651279.Google Scholar
Michotte, Albert (1963). The Perception of Causality. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Miller, John H., and Page, Scott E. (2007). Complex Adaptive Systems: An Introduction to Computational Models of Social Life. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Mitchell, Derek G., and Greening, Steven G. (2012). Conscious perception of emotional stimuli: Brain mechanisms. Neuroscientist, 18(4), 386398.Google Scholar
Mittelberg, Irene (2006). Metaphor and Metonymy in Language and Gesture: Discourse Evidence for Multimodal Models of Grammar. Ann Arbor, MI: UMI.Google Scholar
Mittelberg, Irene (2008). Peircean semiotics meets conceptual metaphor: Iconic modes in gestural representations of grammar. In Cienki, Alan and Müller, Cornelia (eds.), Metaphor and Gesture (pp. 115154). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Mittelberg, Irene (2010). Geometric and image-schematic patterns in gesture space. In Evans, Vyvyan and Chilton, Paul (eds.), Language, Cognition and Space: The State of the Art and New Directions (pp. 351385). London: Equinox.Google Scholar
Mittelberg, Irene (2013a). Balancing acts: Image schemas and force dynamics as experiential essence in pictures by Paul Klee and their gestural enactments. In Borkent, Dancygier & Hinnell (eds.), 325–346.Google Scholar
Mittelberg, Irene (2013b). The exbodied mind: Cognitive-semiotic principles as motivating forces in gesture. In Müller, Cienki, Fricke, Ladewig, McNeill & Tessendorf (eds.), 750–779.Google Scholar
Mittelberg, Irene (2014). Gestures and iconicity. In Müller, Cienki, Fricke, Ladewig, McNeill & Bressem (eds.), 1712–1732.Google Scholar
Mittelberg, Irene (2017). Embodied frames, metonymy and pragmatic inferencing in gesture. Gesture, 16(2).Google Scholar
Mittelberg, Irene, and Waugh, Linda (2009). Metonymy first, metaphor second: A cognitive semiotic approach to multimodal figures of thought in co-speech gesture. In Forceville & Urios-Aparisi (eds.), 330–356.Google Scholar
Mittelberg, Irene, and Waugh, Linda (2014). Gestures and metonymy. In Müller, Cienki, Fricke, Ladewig, McNeill & Bressem (eds.), 1747–1766.Google Scholar
Müller, Cornelia (1998). Redebegleitende Gesten: Kulturgeschichte – Theorie – Sprachvergleich. Berlin: Spitz Verlag.Google Scholar
Müller, Cornelia (2003). Gestik als Lebenszeichen ‘toter Metaphern’: Tote, schlafende und wache Metaphern. Zeitschrift für Semiotik, 1(2), 6172.Google Scholar
Müller, Cornelia (2004). The Palm-Up-Open-Hand: A case of a gesture family? In Müller, Cornelia and Posner, Roland (eds.), The Semantics and Pragmatics of Everyday Gestures (pp. 233256). Berlin: Weidler.Google Scholar
Müller, Cornelia (2008a). Metaphors Dead and Alive, Sleeping and Waking: A Dynamic View. Chicago/London: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Müller, Cornelia (2008b). What gestures reveal about the nature of metaphor. In Cienki, Alan and Müller, Cornelia (eds.), Metaphor and Gesture (pp. 219245). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Müller, Cornelia (2010). Wie Gesten bedeuten: Eine kognitiv-linguistische und sequenzanalytische Perspektive. Sprache und Literatur, 41(1), 3768.Google Scholar
Müller, Cornelia (2011). Are “deliberate” metaphors really deliberate? A question of human consciousness and action. Metaphor in the Social World, 1(1), 6166.Google Scholar
Müller, Cornelia (2014). Gestures as “deliberate expressive movement.” In Seyfeddinipur, Mandana and Gullberg, Marianne (eds.), From Gesture in Conversation to Visible Action as Utterance (pp. 127152). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Müller, Cornelia, and Cienki, Alan (2009). Metaphor, gestures, and beyond: Forms of multimodal metaphor in the use of spoken language. In Forceville & Urios-Aparisi (eds.), 293–321.Google Scholar
Müller, Cornelia, and Ladewig, Silvia H. (2013). Metaphors for sensorimotor experiences: Gestures as embodied and dynamic conceptualizations of balance in dance lessons. In Borkent, Dancygier & Hinnell (eds.), 295–324.Google Scholar
Müller, Cornelia, and Schmitt, Christina (2015). Audio-visual metaphors of the financial crisis: Meaning making and the flow of experience. Revista Brasileira de Linguistica Aplicada (RBLA), Belo Horizonte, 15(2), 311341.Google Scholar
Müller, Cornelia, and Tag, Susanne (2010). The dynamics of metaphor: Foregrounding and activating metaphoricity in conversational interaction. Cognitive Semiotics, 10(6), 85120.Google Scholar
Müller, Cornelia, Cienki, Alan, Fricke, Ellen, Ladewig, Silvia H., McNeill, David, and Bressem, Jana (eds.) (2014). Handbook: Body – Language – Communication: An International Handbook on Multimodality in Human Interaction (Vol. 2). Berlin/Boston: de Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Müller, Cornelia, Cienki, Alan, Fricke, Ellen, Ladewig, Silvia H., McNeill, David, and Tessendorf, Sedinha (eds.) (2013). Body – Language – Communication: An International Handbook on Multimodality in Human Interaction (Vol. 1). Berlin/Boston: de Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Müller, Dana, and Schwarz, Wolf (2007). Is there an internal association of numbers to hands? The task set influences the nature of the SNARC effect. Memory & Cognition, 35(5), 11511161.Google Scholar
Murphy, Gregory L. (1996). On metaphoric representation. Cognition, 60(2), 173204.Google Scholar
Musolff, Andreas (2000). Political imagery of Europe: A house without exit doors? Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 21(3), 216229.Google Scholar
Musolff, Andreas (2004). Metaphor and Political Discourse: Analogical Reasoning in Debates about Europe. Basingstoke/New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Musolff, Andreas (2006). Metaphor scenarios in public discourse. Metaphor and Symbol, 21(1), 2338.Google Scholar
Musolff, Andreas (2007). Popular science concepts and their use in creative metaphors in media discourse. metaphorik.de, 13, 6786.Google Scholar
Musolff, Andreas, and Zinken, Jörg (eds.) (2009). Metaphor and Discourse. Basingstoke/New York: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Nayak, Nandini, and Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr. (1990). Conceptual knowledge in the interpretation of idioms. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 119(3), 315330.Google Scholar
Nieder, Andreas (2005). Counting on neurons: The neurobiology of numerical competence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(3), 177190.Google Scholar
Noë, Alva (2004). Action in Perception. Cambridge, MA/London: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Noë, Alva (2009). Out of Our Heads: Why You Are Not Your Brain, and Other Lessons from the Biology of Consciousness. New York: Hill and Wang.Google Scholar
Norrick, Neal R. (2003). Issues in conversational joking. Journal of Pragmatics, 35(9), 13331359.Google Scholar
Norrick, Neal R. (2010). Conversational Narrative: Storytelling in Everyday Talk. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Núñez, Rafael, Motz, Benjamin, and Teuscher, Ursina (2006). Time after time: The psychological reality of the ego- and time-reference-point distinction in metaphorical contruals of time. Metaphor and Symbol, 21(3), 133146.Google Scholar
Ortiz, Maria J. (2011). Primary metaphors and monomodal visual metaphors. Journal of Pragmatics, 43, 15681580.Google Scholar
Ortony, Andrew (1979). Beyond literal similarity. Psychological Review, 86(3), 161180.Google Scholar
Ortony, Andrew (ed.) [1979] (1993). Metaphor and Thought (2nd edn.). Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ortony, Andrew, Vondruska, Richard J., Foss, Mark A., and Jones, Lawrence (1985). Salience, similes, and the asymmetry of similarity. Journal of memory and language, 24(5), 569594.Google Scholar
Ouellet, Mark, Santiago, Julio, Israeli, Ziv, and Gabay, Shai (2010). Is the future the right time? Experimental Psychology, 57, 308314.Google Scholar
Panther, Klaus-Uwe, and Thornburg, Linda (2003). Metonymy and Pragmatic Inferencing. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Panther, Klaus-Uwe, and Thornburg, Linda (2012). Antonymy in language structure and use. In Brdar, Mario, Raffaelli, Ida, and Fuchs, Milena Žic (eds.), Cognitive Linguistics between Universality and Variation (pp. 161188). Newcastle-upon-Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
Panther, Klaus-Uwe, Thornburg, Linda, and Barcelona, Antonio (eds.) (2009). Metonymy and Metaphor in Grammar. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Parise, Cesare V., Knorre, Katharina, and Ernst, Marc O. (2014). Natural auditory scene statistics shapes human spatial hearing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(16), 61046108.Google Scholar
Parrill, Fey, and Sweetser, Eve E. (2004). What we mean by meaning: Conceptual integration in gesture analysis and transcription. Gesture, 4(2), 197219.Google Scholar
Pashler, Harold, Coburn, Noriko, and Harris, Christine R. (2012). Priming of social distance? Failure to replicate effects on social and food judgments. Public Library of Science ONE, 7(8), e42510.Google Scholar
Pauen, Michael (2012). The second person perspective. Inquiry, 55(1), 3349.Google Scholar
Paxton, Alexandra, and Dale, Rick (2013). Arguments disrupt interpersonal synchrony. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(11), 20922102.Google Scholar
Pecher, Diane, and Zwaan, Rolf A. (eds.) (2005). Grounding Cognition: The Role of Perception and Action in Memory, Language, and Thinking. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Peña, Sandra (2008). Dependency systems for image-schematic patterns in a usage-based approach to language. Journal of Pragmatics, 40(6), 10411066.Google Scholar
Peña, Sandra, and Ruiz de Mendoza Ibañez, Francisco (2009). Metonymic and metaphoric bases of two image-schema transformations. In Panther, Thornburg & Barcelona (eds.), 339–361.Google Scholar
Petty, Richard E., and Cacioppo, John T. (1981). Attitudes and Persuasion: Classic and Contemporary Approaches. Dubuque, IA: W. C. Brown Co.Google Scholar
Pfaff, Kerry, Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr., and Johnson, Michael (1997). Metaphor in using and understanding euphemism and dysphemism. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 5983.Google Scholar
Piaget, Jean (1967). Biologie et connaissance: Essai sur les relations entre les régulations organiques et les processus cognitifs. Paris: Gallimard.Google Scholar
Ping, Raedy, Dhillon, Sonica, and Beilock, Sian Leah (2009). Reach for what you like: The body’s role in shaping preferences. Emotion Review, 1(2), 140150.Google Scholar
Piquer-Píriz, Ana María (2005). Young EFL learners’ understanding of some semantic extensions of the lexemes “hand,” “mouth” and “head.” Ph.D. dissertation, University of Extremadura, Spain.Google Scholar
Pragglejaz-Group (2007). MIP: A method for identifying metaphorically used words in discourse. Metaphor and Symbol, 22(1), 139.Google Scholar
Pratt, Carol C. (1930). The spatial character of high and low tones. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 13(3), 278285.Google Scholar
Prestin, Abby, and Chou, Wen-ying Sylvia (2014). Web 2.0 and the changing health communication environment. In Hamilton, Heidi E. and Chou, Wen-ying Silvia (eds.), The Routledge Handbook of Language and Health Communication (pp. 184197). Abingdon/New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Quinn, Naomi (1992). The cultural basis of metaphor. In Fernandes, J. (ed.), Beyond Metaphor: The Theory of Tropes in Anthropology (pp. 5693). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Radden, Günter (2000). How metonymic are metaphors? In Barcelona (ed.), 93–108.Google Scholar
Radden, Günter, and Kövecses, Zoltán (1999). Towards a theory of metonymy. In Panther, Klaus-Uwe and Radden, Gunter (eds.), Metonymy in Language and Thought (pp. 1759). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Rakova, Marina (2003). The Extent of the Literal: Metaphor, Polysemy, and Theories of Concepts. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Read, Stephen, Cesa, Ian, Jones, David, and Collins, Nancy (1990). When is the federal budget like a baby? Metaphor in political rhetoric. Metaphor and Symbol, 5(3), 125149.Google Scholar
Rennig, Johannes, Bilali’c, Merim, Huberle, Elisabeth, Karnath, Hans-Otto, and Himmelbach, Marc (2013). The temporo-parietal junction contributes to global gestalt perception: Evidence from studies in chess experts. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7(513), 111.Google Scholar
Riceur, Paul (1986). Die lebendige Metapher. Munich: Wilhelm Fink.Google Scholar
Richardson, Michael J., and Chemero, Anthony (2014). Complex dynamical systems and embodiment. In Shapiro (ed.), 39–50.Google Scholar
Ritchie, L. David (2003). Categories and similarities: A note on circularity. Metaphor and Symbol, 18(1), 4953.Google Scholar
Ritchie, L. David (2006). Context and Connection in Metaphor. Basingstoke/New York: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Ritchie, L. David (2008a). Gateshead revisited: Perceptual simulators and fields of meaning in the analysis of metaphors. Metaphor and Symbol, 23(1), 2449.Google Scholar
Ritchie, L. David (2008b). X is a journey: Embodied simulation in metaphor interpretation. Metaphor and Symbol, 23(3), 174199.Google Scholar
Ritchie, L. David (2009). Relevance and simulation in metaphor. Metaphor and Symbol, 24(4), 249262.Google Scholar
Ritchie, L. David (2010a). Between mind and language: “A journey worth taking.” In Cameron & Maslen (eds.), 57–94.Google Scholar
Ritchie, L. David (2010b). “Everybody goes down”: Metaphors, stories, and simulations in conversations. Metaphor and Symbol, 25(3), 123143.Google Scholar
Ritchie, L. David (2011). Justice is blind”: A model for analyzing metaphor transformations and narratives in actual discourse. Metaphor and the Social World, 1(1), 7089.Google Scholar
Ritchie, L. David (2013). Metaphor. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ritchie, L. David (2017). Metaphorical Stories in Discourse. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, Giacomo, Sinigaglia, Corrado, and Anderson, Frances (2008). Mirrors in the Brain: How Our Minds Share Actions and Emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Robin, Shani, and Mayer, Richard (2000). The metaphor framing effect: Metaphorical reasoning about text-based dilemmas. Discourse Processes, 30(1), 5786.Google Scholar
Robinson, Peter, and Ellis, Nick C. (eds.) (2008). Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics and Second Language Acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Roffler, Suzanne K., and Butler, Robert A. (1968). Localization of tonal stimuli in the vertical plane. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 43(6), 12601265.Google Scholar
Rohrer, Tim (2007). Embodiment and experientialism. In Cuyckens & Geeraerts (eds.), 25–47.Google Scholar
Rosca, Andrea (2012). Bases for the Development of Ontological Semantics within the Conceptual Domains of Change and Possession: Implementations and Implications for the Lexico-Syntactic-Cognition Interface and the Development of Intelligent Agents. La Rioja, Spain: University of La Rioja.Google Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza Ibañez, Francisco J. (2011). Metonymy and cognitive operations. In Benczes, Reka, Barcelona, Antonio, and Ruiz de Mendoza Ibañez, Francisco (eds.), Defining Metonymy in Cognitive Linguistics: Towards a Consensus View (pp. 103123). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza Ibañez, Francisco J. (2014). On the nature and scope of metonymy in linguistic description and explanation: Towards settling some controversies. In Littlemore & Taylor (eds.), 143–166.Google Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza Ibañez, Francisco J., and Diez Velasco, Olga (2002). Patterns of conceptual interaction. In Dirven & Pörings (eds.), 489–532.Google Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza Ibañez, Francisco, and Galera, Alicia (2014). Cognitive Modeling: A Linguistic Perspective. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez, Francisco J., and Pérez Hernández, Lorena (2011). The contemporary theory of metaphor: Myths, developments and challenges. Metaphor and Symbol, 26(3), 161185.Google Scholar
Rundblad, Gabriela, and Annaz, Dagmara (2010). Metaphor and metonymy comprehension: Receptive vocabulary and conceptual knowledge. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28(3), 547563.Google Scholar
Saffran, Jenny R., Aslin, Richard N., and Newport, Elissa L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 19261928.Google Scholar
Saffran, Jenny R., Johnson, E. K., Aslin, Richard N., and Newport, Elissa L. (1999). Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults. Cognition, 70, 2752.Google Scholar
Santiago, Julio, Lupiáñez, Juan, Pérez, Elvira, and Funes, María Jesús (2007). Time (also) flies from left to right. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(3), 512516.Google Scholar
Santiago, Julio, Ouellet, Mark, Román, Antonio, and Valenzuela, Javier (2012). Attentional factors in conceptual congruency. Cognitive Science, 36(6), 10511077.Google Scholar
Schank, Roger C., and Berman, Tamara R. (2002). The pervasive role of stories in knowledge and action. In Green, Melanie C., Strange, Jeffrey J., and Brock, Timothy C. (eds.), Narrative Impact: Social and Cognitive Foundations (pp. 287314). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schelling, Thomas (1971). Dynamic models of segregation. Journal of Mathematical Sociology, 1(2), 143186.Google Scholar
Schnall, Simone (2014). Are there basic metaphors? In Landau, Robinson & Meier (eds.), 225–248.Google Scholar
Schneider, Iris K., Rutjens, Bastiaan T., Jostmann, Nils B., and Lakens, Daniel (2011). Weighty matters: Importance literally feels heavy. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2(5), 474478.Google Scholar
Schön, Donald A. ([1979] 1993). Generative metaphor: A perspective on problem solving in social policy. In Ortony (ed.), 137–163.Google Scholar
Schubert, Thomas W., Waldzus, Sven, and Seibt, Beate (2008). The embodiment of power and communalism in space and bodily contact. In Semin & Smith (eds.), 160–183.Google Scholar
Semin, Gün R., and Cacioppo, John T. (2008). Grounding social cognition: Synchronization, coordination, and co-regulation. In Semin & Smith (eds.), 119–147.Google Scholar
Semin, Gün R., and Smith, Eliot R. (eds.) (2008). Embodied Grounding: Social, Cognitive, Affective, and Neuroscientific Approaches. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Semino, Elena (2006). A corpus-based study of metaphors for speech activity in British English. In Stefanowitsch & Th. Gries (eds.), 36–62.Google Scholar
Semino, Elena (2008). Metaphor in Discourse. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Semino, Elena (2010). Descriptions of pain, metaphor, and embodied simulation. Metaphor and Symbol, 25(4), 205226.Google Scholar
Semino, Elena, Deignan, Alice, and Littlemore, Jeannette (2013). Metaphor, genre, and recontextualization. Metaphor and Symbol, 28(1), 4159.Google Scholar
Semino, Elena, and Demjén, Zsófia (eds.) (2017). The Routledge Handbook of Metaphor and Language. Abingdon: Routledge.Google Scholar
Shapiro, Lawrence (ed.) (2014). The Routledge Handbook of Embodied Cognition. Abingdon/New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Shayan, Shakila, Ozturk, Ozge, and Sicoli, Mark A. (2011). The thickness of pitch: Crossmodal metaphors in Farsi, Turkish, and Zapotec. The Senses and Society, 6(1), 96105.Google Scholar
Sheets-Johnstone, Maxine (1999). The Primacy of Movement. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Shen, Yeshayahu (2008). Metaphor and poetic figures. In Gibbs, Jr. (ed.), 295–307.Google Scholar
Shen, Yeshayahu, and Gil, David, (2017). How language influences the way we categorize hybrids. In Cohen, Henri and Lefebvre, Claire (eds.), Handbook of Categorization in Cognitive Science (2nd edn.) (pp. 580601). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Shipp, Stewart (2004). The brain circuitry of attention. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8(5), 223230.Google Scholar
Silver, Michael A., and Kastner, Sabine (2009). Topographic maps in human frontal and parietal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Science, 13(11), 488495.Google Scholar
Siquera, Maity, and Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr. (2007). Children’s acquisition of primary metaphors: A cross-linguistic study. Organon, 21(43), 161179.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. (1985). Crosslinguistic evidence for the language-making capacity. In Slobin, (ed.), The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition. Vol. II: Theoretical Issues (pp. 11571256). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. (1996). From “thought and language” to “thinking for speaking.” In Gumperz, John J. and Levinson, Stephen C. (eds.), Rethinking Linguistic Relativity (pp. 7096). Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Snaevarr, Stefan (2010). Metaphors, Narratives, Emotions: Their Interplay and Impact. Amsterdam: Rodopi.Google Scholar
Soliman, Tamer, and Glenberg, Arthur M. (2014). The embodiment of culture. In Shapiro (ed.), 207–219.Google Scholar
Sparkes, Andrew C., and Smith, Brett (2005). When narratives matter: Men, sport and spinal injury. Journal of Medical Ethics: Medical Humanities, 31(2), 8188.Google Scholar
Sperber, Dan, and Wilson, Deirdre (1995). Relevance Theory (2nd edn.). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Spivey, Michael J. (2007). The Continuity of Mind. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Srinivasan, Mahesh, and Carey, Susan (2010). The long and the short of it: On the nature and origin of functional overlap between representations of space and time. Cognition, 116(2), 217241.Google Scholar
Stanfield, Robert A., and Zwaan, Rolf A. (2001). The effect of implied orientation derived from verbal context on picture recognition. Psychological Science, 12(2), 153156.Google Scholar
Steen, Gerard J. (2008). The paradox of metaphor: Why we need a three-dimensional model of metaphor. Metaphor and Symbol, 23(4), 213241.Google Scholar
Steen, Gerard J. (2011a). The contemporary theory of metaphor – now new and improved! Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 9(1), 2664.Google Scholar
Steen, Gerard J. (2011b). From three dimensions to five steps: The value of deliberate metaphor. metaphorik.de 21, 83110.Google Scholar
Steen, Gerard J. (2011c). Issues in collecting converging evidence: Is metaphor always a matter of thought? In Schönefeld, Doris (ed.), Converging Evidence: Methodological and Theoretical Issues for Linguistic Research (pp. 3355). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Steen, Gerard J. (2011d). What does “really deliberate” really mean? More thoughts on metaphor and consciousness. Metaphor and the Social World, 1(1), 5356.Google Scholar
Steen, Gerard J. (2013). Deliberate metaphor affords conscious metaphorical thought. Cognitive Semiotics, 5(1–2), 179197.Google Scholar
Steen, Gerard J. (2015). Developing, testing and interpreting Deliberate Metaphor Theory. Journal of Pragmatics, 90, 6772.Google Scholar
Stefanowitsch, Anatol (2004). HAPPINESS in English and German: A metaphorical-pattern analysis. In Archard, Michel and Kemmer, Suzanne (eds.), Language, Culture, and Mind (pp. 137149). Stanford; CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Stefanowitsch, Anatol (2006). Words and their metaphors: A Corpus-based approach. In Stefanowitsch & Th. Gries (eds.), 63–106.Google Scholar
Stefanowitsch, Anatol, and Gries, Stefan Th. (eds.) (2006). Corpus-Based Approaches to Metaphor and Metonymy. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Steffensen, Sune V. (2013). Human interactivity: Problem-solving, solution-probing and verbal patterns in the wild. In Cowley, Stephen J. and Vallée-Tourangeau, Frédéric (eds.), Cognition beyond the Brain: Computation, Interactivity and Human Artifice (pp. 195221). London: Springer.Google Scholar
Steffensen, Sune V. (2015). Distributed language and dialogism: Notes on non-locality, sense-making and interactivity. Language Sciences, 50, 105119.Google Scholar
Stephens, Greg, Gilbert, Lauren, and Hasson, Uri (2010). Speaker-listener neural coupling underlies successful communication. PNAS: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States, 107, 144425144430.Google Scholar
Steward, John R., Gapenne, Oliver, and Di Paolo, Ezequiel A. (2010). Enaction: Toward a New Paradigm for Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Stibbe, Astrid (1996). Metaphor and Alternative Conceptions of Illness. Lancaster: Lancaster University.Google Scholar
Straube, Benjamin, Green, Antonia, Bromberger, Bianca, and Kircher, Tilo (2011). The differentiation of iconic and metaphoric gestures: Common and unique integration processes. Human Brain Mapping, 32(4), 520533.Google Scholar
Streeck, Jürgen (1993). Gesture as communication I: Its coordination with gaze and speech. Communication Monographs, 60(4), 275299.Google Scholar
Streeck, Jürgen (2008). Laborious intersubjectivity: Attentional struggle and embodied communication in an auto-shop. In Wachsmuth, Ipke, Lenzen, Manuela, and Knoblich, Günther (eds.), Embodied Communication in Humans and Machines (pp. 202228). Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Streeck, Jürgen (2009). Gesturecraft: The Manu-Facture of Meaning. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Streeck, Jürgen, Goodwin, Charles, and LeBaron, Curtis (2011). Embodied Interaction: Language and Body in the Material World. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stubbs, Michael (2003). Two quantitative methods of studying phraseology in English. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 7(2), 215244.Google Scholar
Sullivan, Karen S. (2007). Grammar in Metaphor: A Construction Grammar Account of Metaphoric Language. Berkeley, CA: University of California at Berkeley.Google Scholar
Sullivan, Karen S. (2013). Frames and Constructions in Metaphoric Language. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Swales, John (1990). Genre Analysis. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sweetser, Eve E. (1990). From Etymology to Pragmatics: Metaphorical and Cultural Aspects of Semantic Structure. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sweetser, Eve (1998). Regular Metaphoricity in Gesture: Bodily-Based Models of Speech Interaction. In Actes du 16e Congrès International des Linguistes (CD-ROM). Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Sweetser, Eve (2007). Looking at space to study mental spaces: Co-speech gesture as a crucial data source in cognitive linguistics. In Gonzalez-Marquez, Mittelberg, Coulson & Spivey (eds.), 201–224.Google Scholar
Talmy, Leonard (1988). Force dynamics in language and cognition. Cognitive Science, 12, 49100.Google Scholar
Talmy, Leonard (2000). Toward a Cognitive Semantics. Vol. I: Concept Structuring System. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Taub, Sarah (2001). Language from the Body: Iconicity and Metaphor in American Sign Language. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Theiner, Georg (2014). Varieties of group cognition. In Shapiro (ed.), 347–358.Google Scholar
Thibault, Paul (2011). First-order languaging dynamics and second-order language: The distributed language view. Ecological Psychology, 23(3), 210245.Google Scholar
Tiemersma, Douwe (1989). Body Schema and Body Image: An Interdisciplinary and Philosophical Study. Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Turner, Mark, and Fauconnier, Gilles (2009). Conceptual integration and formal expression. Metaphor and Symbol, 10(3), 183204.Google Scholar
Tversky, Barbara (2011). Visualizing thought. Topics in Cognitive Science, 3(3), 499535.Google Scholar
Tversky, Barbara, Kugelmass, Sol, and Winter, Atalia (1991). Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions. Cognitive Psychology, 23(4), 515557.Google Scholar
Urena, José Manuel, and Faber, Pamela (2010). Reviewing imagery in resemblance and non-resemblance metaphors. Cognitive Linguistics, 21(1), 123149.Google Scholar
Van Dijk, Teun A., and Kintsch, Walter (1983). Strategies of Discourse Comprehension. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Walker, Esther, and Cooperrider, Kensy (2015). The continuity of metaphor: Evidence from temporal gestures. Cognitive Science, 40(2), 481495.Google Scholar
Walker, Peter, Bremner, J. Gavin, Mason, Uschi, Spring, Jo, Mattock, Karen, Slater, Alan, et al. (2010). Preverbal infants’ sensitivity to synaesthetic cross-modality correspondences. Psychological Science, 21(1), 2125.Google Scholar
Wang, Xiaoqin, Merzenich, Michael M., Sameshima, Koichi, and Jenkins, William M. (1995). Remodelling of hand representation in adult cortex determined by timing of tactile stimulation. Nature, 378, 7175.Google Scholar
Waterworth, John A., Lund, Andreas, and Modjeska, David (2003). Experiential Design of Shared Information Spaces. In Munro, Alan, Höök, Kristina, and Benyon, David (eds.), Designing Information Spaces: The Social Navigation Approach (pp. 125150). London: Springer.Google Scholar
Watson, Wilfred (1986). Classical Hebrew Poetry: A Guide to Its Techniques (2nd edn.). Sheffield: JSOT Press.Google Scholar
Wertheimer, Max (1938). Laws of organization in perceptual forms In Ellis, W. D. (ed.), A Source Book of Gestalt Psychology (pp. 7188). New York: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
Whorf, Benjamin L. (1956). Language, Thought, and Reality: Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Ed. Carroll, John B.. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Williams, Lawrence E., and Bargh, John A. (2008a). Experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth. Science, 322, 606607.Google Scholar
Williams, Lawrence E., and Bargh, John A. (2008b). Keeping one’s distance: The influence of spatial distance cues on affect and evaluation. Psychological Science, 19(3), 302308.Google Scholar
Williams, Lawrence E., Huang, Julie Y., and Bargh, John A. (2009). The scaffolded mind: Higher mental processes are grounded in early experience of the physical world. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39(7), 12571267.Google Scholar
Wilson, Deirdre, and Carston, Robyn (2006). Metaphor, relevance and the “emergent property” issue. Mind & Language, 21(3), 404433.Google Scholar
Wilson, Deirdre, and Sperber, Dan (2012). Explaining irony. In Wilson, Deirdre and Sperber, Dan (eds.), Meaning and Relevance (pp. 123145). Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, Nicole L., and Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr. (2007). Real and imagined body movement primes metaphor comprehension. Cognitive Science, 31(4), 721731.Google Scholar
Winawer, Jonathan, Witthoft, Nathan, Frank, Michael C., Wu, Lisa, Wade, Alex R., Boroditsky, Lera (2007). Russian blues reveal effects of language on color discrimination. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(19), 77807785.Google Scholar
Winner, Ellen (1988). The Point of Words: Children’s Understanding of Metaphor and Irony. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Winter, Bodo (2014). Horror movies and the cognitive ecology of primary metaphors. Metaphor and Symbol, 29(3), 151170.Google Scholar
Winter, Bodo, and Bergen, Benjamin (2012). Language comprehenders represent object distance both visually and auditorily. Language and Cognition, 4(1), 116.Google Scholar
Winter, Bodo, and Matlock, Teenie (2013). More is up … and right: Random number generation along two axes. In Knauff, M., Pauen, M., Sebanz, N., and Wachsmuth, I. (eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 37893974). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Winter, Bodo, Matlock, Teenie, Shaki, Samuel, and Fischer, Martin H. (2015). Mental number space in three dimensions. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 57, 209219.Google Scholar
Winter, Bodo, Perlman, Markus, and Matlock, Teenie (2013). Using space and gesture to talk about numbers: Evidence from the TV News Archive. Gesture, 13, 377408.Google Scholar
Wiseman, Rob (2014). Social distance in hunter-gather settlement sites: A conceptual metaphor in material culture. Metaphor and Symbol, 29(2), 129143.Google Scholar
Wolff, Philip, and Gentner, Dedre (2011). Structure-mapping in metaphor comprehension. Cognitive Science, 35(8), 14561488.Google Scholar
Wolff, Philip, and Holmes, Kevin J. (2011). Linguistic relativity. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2(3), 253265.Google Scholar
Yu, Ning (2003). The bodily dimension of meaning in Chinese: What do we do and mean with “hands.” In Casad, Eugene H. and Palmer, Gerry B. (eds.), Cognitive Linguistics and Non-Indo-European Languages (pp. 337362). Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Yu, Ning (2008). Metaphor from body and culture. In Gibbs, Jr. (ed.), 247–261.Google Scholar
Yu, Ning (2009). Nonverbal and multimodal manifestations of metaphors and metonymies: A case study. In Forceville & Urios-Aparisi (eds.), 119–146.Google Scholar
Yus, Francisco (2011). Cyberpragmatics: Internet-Mediated Communication in Context. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Zaborszky, Laszlo (2002). The modular organization of brain systems. Basal forebrain: The last frontier. Progress in Brain Research, 136, 359372.Google Scholar
Zanotto, Mara Sophia, Cameron, Lynne, and Cavalcanti, Marilda C. (eds.) (2008). Confronting Metaphor in Use: An Applied-Linguistic Approach. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Zhang, Jiajie, and Patel, Vimla (2006). Distributed cognition, representation, and affordance. Pragmatics & Cognition, 14(2), 333341.Google Scholar
Zhong, Chen-Bo, and Leonardelli, Geoffrey J. (2008). Cold and lonely: Does social exclusion literally feel cold? Psychological Science, 19(9), 838842.Google Scholar
Zhong, Chen-Bo, and Liljenquist, Katie (2006). Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313, 14511452.Google Scholar
Ziem, Alexander (2014). Frames of Understanding in Text and Discourse. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Zinken, Jörg (2007). Discourse metaphors: The link between figurative language and habitual analogies. Cognitive Linguistics, 18(3), 445466.Google Scholar
Zinken, Jörg, Hellsten, Iina, and Nerlich, Brigitte (2008). Discourse metaphors. In Frank, Roslyn M., Dirven, René, Ziemke, Tom, and Bernárdez, Enrique (eds.), Body, Language, and Mind. Vol. II: Sociocultural Situatedness (pp. 363385). Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Zinken, Jörg, and Musolff, Andreas (2009). A discourse-centred perspective on metaphorical meaning and understanding. In Musolff & Zinken (eds.), 1–8.Google Scholar
Zlatev, Jordan (2005). What’s in a schema? Bodily mimesis and the grounding of language. In Hampe (ed.), 313–342.Google Scholar
Zwaan, Rolf A. (2014). Embodiment and language comprehension: Reframing the discussion. Trends in Cognitive Science, 18(5), 229234.Google Scholar
Zwaan, Rolf A. (2016). Situation models, mental simulations, and abstract concepts in discourse comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 10281034.Google Scholar
Zwaan, Rolf A., and Madden, Carol J. (2005). Embodied sentence comprehension. In Pecher & Zwaan (eds.), 224–245.Google Scholar
Zwaan, Rolf A., Taylor, Lawrence J., and de Boer, Mirte (2010). Motor resonance as a function of narrative time: Further tests of the linguistic focus hypothesis. Brain & Language, 112(3), 143149.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Edited by Beate Hampe, Universität Erfurt, Germany
  • Book: Metaphor
  • Online publication: 05 July 2017
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108182324.019
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Edited by Beate Hampe, Universität Erfurt, Germany
  • Book: Metaphor
  • Online publication: 05 July 2017
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108182324.019
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Edited by Beate Hampe, Universität Erfurt, Germany
  • Book: Metaphor
  • Online publication: 05 July 2017
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108182324.019
Available formats
×