Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-qsmjn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T13:17:19.579Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part VIII - Behavioral diversity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 September 2015

Michio Nakamura
Affiliation:
Kyoto University, Japan
Kazuhiko Hosaka
Affiliation:
Kamakura Women’s University, Japan
Noriko Itoh
Affiliation:
Kyoto University, Japan
Koichiro Zamma
Affiliation:
Great Ape Research Institute
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Mahale Chimpanzees
50 Years of Research
, pp. 519 - 598
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Boesch, C. (1995). Innovation in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). International Journal of Primatology, 16, 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonnie, K. E. and de Waal, F. B. M. (2006). Affiliation promotes the transmission of a social custom: handclasp grooming among captive chimpanzees. Primates, 47, 2734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corp, N., Hayaki, H., Matsusaka, T., et al. (2009). Prevalence of muzzle-rubbing and hand-rubbing behavior in wild chimpanzees in Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates, 50, 184–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fragaszy, D. (2003). Making space for traditions. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 6170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hosaka, K. (1995). [Water play.] Monkey, 39(2), 20–1. In Japanese.Google Scholar
Huffman, M. A. and Kalunde, M. S. (1993). Tool-assisted predation on a squirrel by a female chimpanzee in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. Primates, 34, 93–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imanishi, K. (1952). [Evolution of humanity.] In [Man], ed. Imanishi, K.. Tokyo: Mainichi-shinbun-sha, pp. 3694. In Japanese.Google Scholar
Ingold, T. (1996). Situating action VI: a comment on the distinction between the material and the social. Ecological Psychology, 8, 183–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingold, T. (1998). Comments. Current Anthropology, 39, 606–7.Google Scholar
Itani, J. and Nishimura, A. (1973). The study of infrahuman culture in Japan. In Precultural Primate Behavior, ed. Menzel, E. W.. Basel: Karger, pp. 2650.Google Scholar
Itoh, N. (2013). [Dual-rings of mode of coexistence and act choices: Chimpanzee group and emergence of institution.] In [Practices, Conventions and Institutions: the Evolution of Human Sociality], ed. Kawai, K.. Kyoto: Kyoto University Press, pp. 143–66. In Japanese.Google Scholar
Itoh, N. and Nishida, T. (2007). Chimpanzee grouping patterns and food availability in Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates, 48, 8796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krützen, M., van Schaik, C., and Whiten, A. (2007). The animal cultures debate: response to Laland and Janik. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 22, 6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laland, K. N. and Janik, V. M. (2006). The animal cultures debate. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 21, 542–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laland, K. N. and Janik, V. M. (2007). Response to Krützen et al.: further problems with the “method of exclusion”. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 22, 7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lévi-Strauss, C. (1967). Les Structures Élémentaires de la Parenté. The Hague: Mouton & Co and Maison des Sciences de l’Homme.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marchant, L. F. and McGrew, W. C. (1999). Innovative behavior at Mahale: new data on nasal probe and nipple press. Pan Africa News, 6, 1618.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsusaka, T. and Kutsukake, N. (2002). Use of leaf-sponge and leaf-spoon by juvenile chimpanzees at Mahale. Pan Africa News, 9, 69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsusaka, T., Nishie, H., Shimada, M., et al. (2006). Tool-use for drinking water by immature chimpanzees of Mahale: prevalence of an unessential behavior. Primates, 47, 113–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrew, W. C. (1992). Chimpanzee Material Culture: Implications for Human Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C. (2004). The Cultured Chimpanzee: Reflections on Cultural Primatology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C. and Tutin, C. E. G. (1978). Evidence for a social custom in wild chimpanzees? Man, 13, 234–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., Scott, S. E., and Tutin, C. E. G. (2001). Intergroup differences in a social custom of wild chimpanzees: The grooming hand-clasp of the Mahale Mountains. Current Anthropology, 42, 148–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M. (2002). Grooming-hand-clasp in Mahale M group chimpanzees: implication for culture in social behaviors. In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos, ed. Boesch, C., Hohmann, G., and Marchant, L. F.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 7183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M. (2003). [Questions about chimpanzee culture studies.] Ecosophia, 12, 5561. In Japanese.Google Scholar
Nakamura, M. (2010a). Ubiquity of culture and possible social inheritance of sociality among wild chimpanzees. In The Mind of the Chimpanzee, ed. Lonsdorf, E. V., Ross, S. R., and Matsuzawa, T.. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press, pp. 156–67.Google Scholar
Nakamura, M. (2010b). Poke-type social scratching persists at Mahale. Pan Africa News, 17, 1517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M. and Itoh, N. (2008). Hunting with tools by Mahale chimpanzees. Pan Africa News, 15, 36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M. and Nishida, T. (2006). Subtle behavioral variation in wild chimpanzees, with special reference to Imanishi’s concept of kaluchua. Primates, 47, 3542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M. and Uehara, S. (2004). Proximate factors of different types of grooming hand-clasp in Mahale chimpanzees: implications for chimpanzee social custom. Current Anthropology, 45, 108–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M., McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., and Nishida, T. (2000). Social scratch: another custom in wild chimpanzees? Primates, 41, 5971.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishida, T. (1968). The social group of wild chimpanzees in the Mahali Mountains. Primates, 9, 167224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1973). The ant-gathering behaviour by the use of tools among wild chimpanzees of the Mahali Mountains. Journal of Human Evolution, 2, 357–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1980a). Local differences in responses to water among wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatologica, 33, 189209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishida, T. (1980b). The leaf-clipping display: a newly-discovered expressive gesture in wild chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 9, 117–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1983). Alloparental behavior in wild chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. Folia Primatologica, 41, 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1987). Local traditions and cultural transmission. In Primate Societies, ed. Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W., and Struhsaker, T. T.. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press, pp. 462–74.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1994). Review of recent findings on Mahale chimpanzees: implications and future research directions. In Chimpanzee Cultures, ed. Wrangham, R. W., McGrew, W. C., de Waal, F. B. M., and Heltne, P. G.. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, pp. 373–96.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1997). Sexual behavior of adult male chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates, 38, 379–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (2003). Individuality and flexibility of cultural behavior patterns in chimpanzees. In Animal Social Complexity: Intelligence, Culture, and Individualized Societies, ed. de Waal, F. B. M. and Tyack, P.. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, pp. 392413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (2012). Chimpanzees of the Lakeshore: Natural History and Culture at Mahale. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. and Hiraiwa, M. (1982). Natural history of a tool-using behavior by wild chimpanzees in feeding upon wood-boring ants. Journal of Human Evolution, 11, 7399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. and Uehara, S. (1980). Chimpanzees, tools, and termites: another example from Tanzania. Current Anthropology, 21, 671–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. and Uehara, S. (1983). Natural diet of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii): long-term record from the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. African Study Monographs, 3, 109–30.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. and Wallauer, W. (2003). Leaf-pile pulling: an unusual play pattern in wild chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology, 60, 167–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishida, T., Wrangham, R. W., Goodall, J., and Uehara, S. (1983). Local differences in plant-feeding habits of chimpanzees between the Mahale Mountains and Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Journal of Human Evolution, 12, 467–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T., Kano, T., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., and Nakamura, M. (1999). Ethogram and ethnography of Mahale chimpanzees. Anthropological Science, 107, 141–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T., Mitani, J. C., and Watts, D. P. (2004). Variable grooming behaviors in wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatologica, 75, 31–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishida, T., Matsusaka, T., and McGrew, W. C. (2009). Emergence, propagation or disappearance of novel behavioral patterns in the habituated chimpanzees of Mahale: A review. Primates, 50, 2336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishida, T., Zamma, K., Matsusaka, T., Inaba, A., and McGrew, W. C. (2010). Chimpanzee Behavior in the Wild. Tokyo: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishie, H. (2008). [Chimpanzee culture and sociality: reconsidering “the metaphor of transmission of knowledge”.] Primate Research, 24, 7390. In Japanese with English abstract.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishie, H. (2011). Natural history of Camponotus ant fishing by the M group chimpanzees at the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates, 52, 329–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishie, H. (2012). [Chimpanzee Culture and Sociality.] Doctoral Dissertation. Kyoto: Kyoto University. In Japanese.Google Scholar
Sakamaki, T. (1998). First record of algae-feeding by a female chimpanzee at Mahale. Pan Africa News, 5, 13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sakamaki, T., Nakamura, M., and Nishida, T. (2007). Evidence of cultural differences in diet between two neighboring unit groups of chimpanzees in Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Pan Africa News, 14, 35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shimada, M. (2002). Social scratch among chimpanzees in Gombe. Pan Africa News, 9, 21–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D. (1982). Le Savoir des Anthropologues. Paris: Hermann.Google Scholar
Takahata, Y. (1982). Termite fishing observed in M group chimpanzees. Mahale Mountains Chimpanzee Research Project Ecological Report, 18.Google Scholar
Takahata, Y., Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M., Takasaki, H., and Nyundo, R. (1986). Newly acquired feeding habits among the chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Human Evolution, 1, 277–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takasaki, H. (1983). Mahale chimpanzees taste mangoes: toward acquisition of a new food item? Primates, 24, 273–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uehara, S. (1982). Seasonal change in the techniques employed by wild chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania, to feed on termites (Pseudacanthotermes spiniger). Folia Primatologica, 37, 4476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Waal, F. B. M. (1999). Cultural primatology comes of age. Nature, 399, 635–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Waal, F. B. M. (2001). The Ape and the Sushi Master. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. and Seres, M. (1997). Propagation of handclasp grooming among captive chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology, 43, 339–46.3.0.CO;2-Y>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Washburn, S. and Benedict, B. (1979). Non-human primate culture. Man, 14, 163–4.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., et al. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., et al. (2001). Charting cultural variation in chimpanzees. Behaviour, 138, 1481–516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrangham, R. W., de Waal, F. B. M., and McGrew, W. C. (1994). The challenge of behavioral diversity. In Chimpanzee Cultures, ed. Wrangham, R. W., McGrew, W. C., de Waal, F. B. M., and Heltne, P. G.. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, pp.118.Google Scholar
Zamma, K. (2002). Leaf-grooming by a wild chimpanzee in Mahale. Primates, 43, 8790.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zamma, K. (2006). A louse egg left on a leaf. Pan Africa News, 13, 810.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Arcadi, A. C. (2000). Vocal responsiveness in male wild chimpanzees: implications for the evolution of language. Journal of Human Evolution, 39, 205–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bachorowski, J.-A., Smoski, M. J., and Owren, M. J. (2001). The acoustic features of human laughter. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 110, 1581–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C. and Boesch-Achermann, H. (2000). The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: Behavioural Ecology and Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1990). How Monkeys See the World: Inside the Mind of Another Species. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. P. (1993). Rank differences in the production of vocalizations by wild chimpanzees as a function of social context. American Journal of Primatology, 31, 159–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. P. and Wrangham, R. W. (1993). Acoustic analysis of wild chimpanzee pant hoots: do Kibale Forest chimpanzees have an acoustically distinct food arrival pant hoot? American Journal of Primatology, 31, 99–109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. P. and Wrangham, R. W. (1994). Chimpanzee arrival pant-hoots: do they signify food or status? International Journal of Primatology, 15, 185205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockford, C. and Boesch, C. (2003). Context-specific calls in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus: analysis of barks. Animal Behaviour, 66, 115–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockford, C., Herbinger, I., Vigilant, L., and Boesch, C. (2004). Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: a case for vocal learning? Ethology, 110, 221–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Mundry, R., and Zuberbühler, K. (2012). Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Current Biology, 22, 142–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davila-Ross, M., Owren, M., and Zimmermann, E. (2009). Reconstructing the evolution of laughter in great apes and humans. Current Biology, 19, 1106–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, J. (1992). The Third Chimpanzee: The Evolution and Future of the Human Animal. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Fedurek, P., Machanda, Z. P., Schel, A. M., and Slocombe, K. E. (2013a). Pant hoot chorusing and social bonds in male chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 86, 189–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fedurek, P., Schel, A. M., and Slocombe, K. E. (2013b). The acoustic structure of chimpanzee pant-hooting facilitates chorusing. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 67, 1781–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fossey, D. (1972). Vocalizations of the mountain gorilla (Gorilla gorilla beringei). Animal Behaviour, 20, 3653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fujimoto, M. and Hanamura, S. (2008). Responses of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) toward seismic aftershocks in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates, 49, 73–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Belknap.Google Scholar
Hanamura, S. (2010). [Long-distance calls by chimpanzees: how they connect actions and organize their social field beyond visual contact.] Primate Research, 26, 159–76. In Japanese with English summary.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harcourt, A. H., Stewart, K. J., and Hauser, M. D. (1993). Functions of wild gorilla “close” calls. I. Repertoire, context, and interspecific comparison. Behaviour, 124, 89–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hasegawa, T. and Kajikawa, S. (2002). [What do chimpanzees communicate by pant-hoot calls?] In [The Mahale Chimpanzees: 37 Years of <Panthropology>], ed. Nishida, T., Uehara, S., and Kawanaka, K.. Kyoto: Kyoto University Press, pp. 303–19. In Japanese.Google Scholar
Hayaki, H. (1990). Social context of pant-grunting in young chimpanzees. In The Chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains: Sexual and Life History Strategies, ed. Nishida, T.. Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press, pp. 189206.Google Scholar
Herbinger, I., Papworth, S., Boesch, C., and Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Vocal, gestural and locomotor responses of wild chimpanzees to familiar and unfamiliar intruders: a playback study. Animal Behaviour, 78, 1389–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1972). A comparative approach to the phylogeny of laughter and smiling. In Non-Verbal Communication, ed. Hinde, R. A.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 209–23.Google Scholar
Hopkins, W. D., Taglialatela, J. P., and Leavens, D. A. (2007). Chimpanzees differentially produce novel vocalizations to capture the attention of a human. Animal Behaviour, 73, 281–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hosaka, K., Matsumoto-Oda, A., Huffman, M. A., and Kawanaka, K. (2000). [Reactions to dead bodies of conspecifics by wild chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania.] Primate Research, 16, 115. In Japanese with English summary.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Itani, J. (1963). Vocal communication of the wild Japanese monkey. Primates, 4, 1166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kajikawa, S. and Hasegawa, T. (2000). Acoustic variation of pant hoot calls by male chimpanzees: a playback experiment. Journal of Ethology, 18, 133–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kojima, S. (2003). A Search for the Origins of Human Speech: Auditory and Vocal Functions of the Chimpanzee. Kyoto: Kyoto University Press.Google Scholar
Laporte, M. N. C. and Zuberbühler, K. (2010). Vocal greeting behaviour in wild chimpanzee females. Animal Behaviour, 80, 467–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1968). The behaviour of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve. Animal Behaviour Monographs, 1, 161312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macedonia, J. M. and Evans, C. S. (1993). Variation among mammalian alarm call systems and the problem of meaning in animal signals. Ethology, 93, 177–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P. (1976). Social organization, communication and graded signals: The chimpanzee and the gorilla. In Growing Points in Ethology, ed. Bateson, P. P. G. and Hinde, R. A.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 239–80.Google Scholar
Marler, P. and Hobbett, L. (1975). Individuality in a long-range vocalization of wild chimpanzees. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 38, 37109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P. and Tenaza, R. (1977). Signaling behavior of apes with special reference to vocalization. In How Animals Communicate, ed. Sebeok, T. A.. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press, pp. 965–1033.Google Scholar
Matsusaka, T. (2004). When does play panting occur during social play in wild chimpanzees? Primates, 45, 221–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C. (1994). Ethological studies of chimpanzee vocal behavior. In Chimpanzee Cultures, ed. Wrangham, R. W., McGrew, W. C., de Waal, F. B. M., and Heltne, P.. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, pp. 195210.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C. (1996). Comparative studies of African ape vocal behavior. In Great Ape Societies, ed. McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., and Nishida, T.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 241–54.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C. and Brandt, K. L. (1994). Social factors influence the acoustic variability in the long-distance calls of male chimpanzees. Ethology, 96, 233–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C. and Gros-Louis, J. (1995). Species and sex differences in the screams of chimpanzees and bonobos. International Journal of Primatology, 16, 393411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C. and Gros-Louis, J. (1998). Chorusing and call convergence in chimpanzees: tests of three hypotheses. Behaviour, 135, 1041–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C. and Nishida, T. (1993). Contexts and social correlates of long-distance calling by male chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 45, 735–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C., Hasegawa, T., Gros-Louis, J., Marler, P., and Byrne, R. W. (1992). Dialects in wild chimpanzees? American Journal of Primatology, 27, 233–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitani, J. C., Gros-Louis, J., and Macedonia, J. M. (1996). Selection for acoustic individuality within the vocal repertoire of wild chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology, 17, 569–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C., Hunley, K. L., and Murdoch, M. E. (1999). Geographic variation in the calls of wild chimpanzees: a reassessment. American Journal of Primatology, 47, 133–51.3.0.CO;2-I>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mori, A. (1983). Comparison of the communicative vocalizations and behaviors of group ranging in eastern gorillas, chimpanzees and pygmy chimpanzees. Primates, 24, 486500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1994). Review of recent findings on Mahale chimpanzees implications. In Chimpanzee Cultures, ed. Wrangham, R. W., McGrew, W. C., de Waal, F. B. M., and Heltne, P.. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, pp. 373–96.Google Scholar
Nishida, T., Kano, T., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., and Nakamura, M. (1999). Ethogram and ethnography of Mahale chimpanzees. Anthropological Science, 107, 141–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Notman, H. and Rendall, D. (2005). Contextual variation in chimpanzee pant hoots and its implications for referential communication. Animal Behaviour, 70, 177–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preuschoft, S and van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1997). The social function of “smile’’ and ‘‘laughter’’: variations across primate species and societies. In Nonverbal Communication: Where Nature Meets Culture, ed. Segerstråle, U. and Molnár, P.. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 171–89.Google Scholar
Provine, R. R. (1996). Laughter. American Scientist, 84, 3845.Google Scholar
Reynolds, V. and Reynolds, F. (1965). Chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest. In Primate Behavior: Field Studies of Monkeys and Apes, ed. DeVore, I.. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, pp. 368424.Google Scholar
Sakamaki, T. (2011). Submissive pant-grunt greeting of female chimpanzees in Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. African Study Monographs, 32, 2541.Google Scholar
Schel, A. M., Townsend, S. W., Machanda, Z. P., Zuberbühler, K., and Slocombe, K. E. (2013). Chimpanzee alarm call production meets key criteria for intentionality. PLoS ONE, 8, e76674.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seyfarth, R., Cheney, D., Harcourt, A., and Stewart, K. (1994). The acoustic features of gorilla double grunts and their relation to behavior. American Journal of Primatology, 33, 3150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slocombe, K. E. and Zuberbühler, K. (2007). Chimpanzees modify recruitment screams as a function of audience composition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 104, 17228–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, F. A., Piel, A. K., and O’Malley, R. C. (2012). Responses of chimpanzees to a recently dead community member at Gombe National Park, Tanzania. American Journal of Primatology, 74, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sugiyama, Y. and Humle, T. (2011). A wild chimpanzee uses a stick to disable a snare at Bossou, Guinea. Pan Africa News, 18, 34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teleki, G. (1973) Group response to the accidental death of a chimpanzee in Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Folia Primatologica, 20, 8194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsukahara, T. (1993). Lions eat chimpanzees: the first evidence of predation by lions on wild chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology, 29, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Waal, F. (1982). Chimpanzees Politics: Power and Sex among Apes. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Wilson, M. L., Hauser, M. D., and Wrangham, R. W. (2001). Does participation in intergroup conflict depend on numerical assessment, range location, or rank for wild chimpanzees? Animal Behaviour, 61, 1203–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. (1977). Feeding behaviour of chimpanzees in Gombe National Park, Tanzania. In Primate Ecology: Studies of Feeding and Ranging Behavior in Lemurs, Monkeys and Apes, ed. Clutton-Brock, T. H.. London: Academic Press, pp. 503–38.Google Scholar

References

Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Belknap.Google Scholar
Hayaki, H. (1985). Social play of juvenile and adolescent chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates, 26, 343–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M., Byrne, R. W., Takasaki, H., and Byrne, J. M. E. (1986). Aggression toward large carnivores by wild chimpanzees of Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Folia Primatologica, 47, 813.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirata, S., Yamakoshi, G., Fujita, S., Ohashi, G., and Matsuzawa, T. (2001). Capturing and toying with hyraxes (Dendrohyrax dorsalis) by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) at Bossou, Guinea. American Journal of Primatology, 53, 93–7.3.0.CO;2-X>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hosaka, K. (1995). [Water play.] Monkey 39(2), 20–1. In Japanese.Google Scholar
Hosaka, K., Matsumoto-Oda, A., Huffman, M. A., and Kawanaka, K. (2000). [Reactions to dead bodies of conspecifics by wild chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania.] Primate Research, 16, 115. In Japanese with English summary.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsusaka, T. (2004). When does play panting occur during social play in wild chimpanzees? Primates, 45, 221–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsusaka, T. (2007). Exploratory-threat behaviors in wild chimpanzees encountering a porcupine. Pan Africa News, 14, 2931.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsusaka, T. (2012). Playful drumming by immature wild chimpanzees at Mahale: do they enjoy making sounds? Pan Africa News, 19, 23–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsusaka, T., Nishie, H., Shimada, M., et al. (2006). Tool-use for drinking water by immature chimpanzees of Mahale: prevalence of an unessential behavior. Primates, 47, 113–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsuzawa, T. (1997). The death of an infant chimpanzee at Bossou, Guinea. Pan Africa News, 4, 46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C. and Marchant, L. F. (1998). Chimpanzee wears a knotted skin “necklace.” Pan Africa News, 5, 89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M. (1995). [“Teasing” play.] Monkey, 39, 1819. In Japanese.Google Scholar
Nakamura, M. (2009). Aesthete in the forest? A female chimpanzee at Mahale collected and carried guineafowl feathers. Pan Africa News, 16, 1719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M. (2012). Playing with his leg: a case of imaginary social play by an adolescent male chimpanzee at Bossou? Pan Africa News, 19, 13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M. (2013). A juvenile chimpanzee played with a live moth. Pan Africa News, 20, 22–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1983). Alloparental behavior in wild chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. Folia Primatologica, 41, 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (2002). A self-medicating attempt to remove the sand flea from a toe by a young chimpanzee. Pan Africa News, 9, 56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (2003). Harassment of mature female chimpanzees by young males in the Mahale Mountains. International Journal of Primatology, 24, 503–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (2012). Chimpanzees of the Lakeshore: Natural History and Culture at Mahale. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. and Inaba, A. (2009). Pirouettes: the rotational play of wild chimpanzees. Primates, 50, 333–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishida, T. and Nakamura, M. (1993). Chimpanzee tool use to clear a blocked nasal passage. Folia Primatologica, 61, 218–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. and Wallauer, W. (2003). Leaf-pile pulling: an unusual play pattern in wild chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology, 60, 167–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishida, T., Kano, T., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., and Nakamura, M. (1999). Ethogram and ethnography of Mahale chimpanzees. Anthropological Science, 107, 141–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T., Matsusaka, T., and McGrew, W. C. (2009). Emergence, propagation or disappearance of novel behavioral patterns in the habituated chimpanzees of Mahale: a review. Primates, 50, 2336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishida, T., Zamma, K., Matsusaka, T., Inaba, A., and McGrew, W. C. (2010). Chimpanzee Behavior in the Wild: An Audio-Visual Encyclopedia. Tokyo: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., et al. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zamma, K. (2002). A chimpanzee trifling with a squirrel: Pleasure derived from teasing? Pan Africa News, 9, 911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zamma, K. (2011). Responses of chimpanzees to a python. Pan Africa News, 18, 1315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zamma, K. and Fujita, S. (2004). Genito-genital rubbing among the chimpanzees of Mahale and Bossou. Pan Africa News, 11(2), 58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zamma, K., Nakashima, M. and Ramadhani, A. (2011). Mahale chimpanzees start to eat oil palm. Pan Africa News, 18, 68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Boesch, C. (1991). Handedness in wild chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology, 12, 541–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogart, S. L., Pruetz, J. D., Ormiston, L. K., et al. (2012). Termite fishing laterality in the Fongoli savanna chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus): further evidence of a left hand preference. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 149, 591–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, B. J. and Vigilant, L. (2002). The evolutionary genetics and molecular ecology of chimpanzees and bonobos. In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos, ed. Boesch, C., Hohmann, G., and Marchant, L.F.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 259–76.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. and Byrne, J. M. (1991). Hand preference in the skilled gathering tasks of mountain gorillas (Gorilla g. beringei). Cortex, 27, 521–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W. and Byrne, J. M. (1993). Complex leaf-gathering skills of mountain gorillas (Gorilla g. beringei): variability and standardization. American Journal of Primatology, 31, 241–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapelain, A. S., Hogervorst, E. Mbonzo, P., and Hopkins, W. D. (2011). Hand preference for bimanual coordination in 77 bonobos (Pan paniscus): replication and extension. International Journal of Primatology, 32, 491510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corp, N. and Byrne, R. W. (2004). Sex differences in chimpanzee handedness. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 123, 62–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobaiter, C. and Byrne, R. W. (2013). Laterality in the gestural communication of wild chimpanzees. In The Evolution of Human Handedness, ed. McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., and Schiefenhövel, W.. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1288, 9–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopkins, W.D. (1994) Hand preferences for bimanual feeding in 140 captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): rearing and ontogenetic determinants. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 109, 291–7.Google Scholar
Hopkins, W. D. (1999). On the other hand: statistical issues in the assessment and interpretation of hand preference data in non-human primates. International Journal of Primatology, 20, 851–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopkins, W. D., Russell, J. L., Cantalupo, C., Freeman, H., and Shapiro, S. J. (2005). Factors influencing the prevalence and handedness for throwing in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, 363–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humle, T. and Matsuzawa, T. (2009). Laterality in hand use across four tool-use behaviors among the wild chimpanzees of Bossou, Guinea, West Africa. American Journal of Primatology, 71, 40–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lonsdorf, E. V. (2005). Sex differences in the development of termite-fishing skills in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Animal Behaviour, 70, 673–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lonsdorf, E. and Hopkins, W. D. (2005). Wild chimpanzees show population-level handedness for tool use. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA, 102, 12634–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchant, L. F. and McGrew, W. C. (1994). An ethological perspective on human handedness. In The Ethological Roots of Culture, ed. Gardner, R. A., Chiarelli, A. B., Gardner, B. T., and Plooij, F. X.. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Kluwer, pp. 375–84.Google Scholar
Marchant, L. F. and McGrew, W. C. (1996). Laterality of limb function in wild chimpanzees of Gombe National Park: comprehensive study of spontaneous activities. Journal of Human Evolution, 30, 427–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marchant, L. F. and McGrew, W. C. (2007). Ant fishing by wild chimpanzees is not lateralised. Primates, 48, 22–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marchant, L. F. and McGrew, W. C. (2013). Handedness is more than laterality: lessons from chimpanzees. In The Evolution of Human Handedness, ed. McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., and Schiefenhövel, W.. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1288, 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marchant, L. F., McGrew, W. C., and Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1995). Is human handedness universal? Ethological analyses from three traditional cultures. Ethology, 101, 239–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marzke, M. W. and Wullstein, K. L. (1995). Chimpanzee and human grips: a new classification with a focus on evolutionary morphology. International Journal of Primatology, 17, 117–39.Google Scholar
Matsuzawa, T., Biro, D., Humle, T., et al. (2001). Emergence of culture in wild chimpanzees: education by master-apprenticeship. In Primate Origins of Human Cognition and Behavior, ed. Matsuzawa, T.. Tokyo: Springer, pp. 557–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C. and Marchant, L. F. (1992). Chimpanzees, tools, and termites: hand preference or handedness? Current Anthropology, 33, 114–19.Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C. and Marchant, L. F. (1996). On which side of the apes: ethological study of laterality of hand use. In Great Ape Societies, ed. McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., and Nishida, T.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 255–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C. and Marchant, L. F. (1997). On the other hand: current issues and meta-analysis of the behavioral laterality of hand function in non-human primates. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 40, 201–32.Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C. and Marchant, L. F. (2001). Ethological study of manual laterality in the chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. Behaviour, 138, 329–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C. and Tutin, C. E. G. (1978). Evidence for a social custom in wild chimpanzees? Man, 13, 234–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., Wrangham, R. W., and Klein, H. (1999). Manual laterality in anvil use: wild chimpanzees cracking Strychnos fruits, Laterality, 4, 7997.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., Payne, C. L. R., Webster, T. H., and Hunt, K. D. (2013). Well digging by Semliki chimpanzees: new data and possible significance of hydrology. Pan Africa News, 20, 58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, J. (1996). Savanna chimpanzees, referential models and the last common ancestor. In Great Ape Societies, ed. McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., and Nishida, T.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 275–92.Google Scholar
Morino, L. (2011). Left-hand preference for a complex manual task in a population of wild siamangs (Symphalangus syndactylus). International Journal of Primatology, 32, 793800.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M., McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., and Nishida, T. (2000). Social scratch: another custom in wild chimpanzees? Primates, 41, 237–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishida, T. (1973). The ant-gathering behaviour by the use of tools among wild chimpanzees of the Mahali Mountains. Journal of Human Evolution, 2, 357–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1983). Alloparental behavior in wild chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. Folia Primatologica, 41, 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1993). Left nipple suckling preference in wild chimpanzees. Ethology and Sociobiology, 14, 4552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1994). [Thirty-six Stories of Wild Chimpanzees.] Tokyo: Kinokuniya. In Japanese.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (2012). Chimpanzees of the Lakeshore: Natural History and Culture at Mahale. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. and Hiraiwa, M. (1982). Natural history of a tool-using behavior by wild chimpanzees in feeding upon wood-boring ants. Journal of Human Evolution, 11, 7399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T., McGrew, W. C., and Marchant, L.F. (2012). Wild chimpanzees at Mahale are not manually lateralized for throwing. Pan Africa News, 19, 21–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norikoshi, K. (1994). [Hand preference of wild chimpanzees observed in their tool behavior.] Primate Research, 10, 315–19. In Japanese with English summary.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norikoshi, K. (1998). Hand preference of wild chimpanzees observed in their tool behaviour. In: Resource Use Patterns and Social Structure among Chimpanzees, ed. Nishida, T.. Kyoto: Nisshindo, pp. 97–104.Google Scholar
Palmer, A. R. (2002). Chimpanzee right-handedness reconsidered: evaluating the evidence with funnel plots. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 118, 191–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sugiyama, Y., Fushima, T., Sakaru, O., and Matsuzawa, T. (1993). Hand preference and tool use in wild chimpanzees. Primates, 34, 151–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., et al. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Assersohn, C., Whiten, A., Kiwede, Z. T., Tinka, J., and Karamagi, J. (2004). Use of leaves to inspect ectoparasites in wild chimpanzees: a third cultural variant? Primates, 45, 255–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bermejo, M. and Illera, G. (1999). Tool-set for termite-fishing and honey extraction by wild chimpanzees in the Lossi Forest, Congo. Primates, 40, 619–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bermejo, M., Illera, G., and Sabater Pi, J. (1989). New observations on the tool-behavior of chimpanzees from Mt. Assirik (Senegal, West Africa). Primates, 30, 6573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C. (1995). Innovation in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). International Journal of Primatology, 16, 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C. and Boesch, H. (1990). Tool use and tool making in wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatologica, 54, 8699.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bogart, S. L. and Pruetz, J. D. (2008). Ecological context of savanna chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) termite fishing at Fongoli, Senegal. American Journal of Primatology, 70, 605–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collins, D. A. and McGrew, W. C. (1987). Termite fauna related to differences in tool-use between groups of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Primates, 28, 457–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corp, N., Hayaki, H., Matsusaka, T., et al. (2009). Prevalence of muzzle-rubbing and hand-rubbing behavior in wild chimpanzees in Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates, 50, 184–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deblauwe, I., Guislain, P., Dupain, J., and van Elasacker, L. (2006). Use of tool-set by Pan troglodytes troglodytes to obtain termites (Macrotermes) in the periphery of the Dja Biosphere Reserve, southeast Cameroon. American Journal of Primatology, 68, 1191–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fay, J. M. and Carroll, R. W. (1994). Chimpanzee tool use for honey and termite extraction in Central Africa. American Journal of Primatology, 34, 309–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fowler, A. and Sommer, V. (2007). Subsistence technology of Nigerian chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology, 28, 9971023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodall, J. (1963). Feeding behaviour of wild chimpanzees: a preliminary report. Symposia of Zoological Society of London, 10, 3947.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1971). In the Shadow of Man. London: William Collins.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Belknap.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1990). Through a Window: Thirty Years with the Chimpanzees of Gombe. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson Ltd.Google Scholar
Huffman, M. A. and Kalunde, M. S. (1993). Tool-assisted predation on a squirrel by a female chimpanzee in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. Primates, 34, 93–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humle, T. (1999). New record of fishing for termites (Macrotermes) by the chimpanzees of Bossou (Pan troglodytes verus), Guinea. Pan Africa News, 6, 35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, C. and Sabater Pi, J. (1969). Sticks used by chimpanzees in Rio Muni, West Africa. Nature, 223, 100–1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1968). The behaviour of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve. Animal Behaviour Monographs, 1, 161311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lonsdorf, E. V. (2005). Sex differences in the development of termite-fishing skills in the wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii, of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Animal Behaviour, 70, 673–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marchant, L. F. and McGrew, W. C. (1999). Innovative behavior at Mahale: new data on nasal probe and nipple press. Pan Africa News, 6, 1618.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsusaka, T., Nishie, H., Shimada, M., et al. (2006). Tool-use for drinking water by immature chimpanzees of Mahale: prevalence of an unessential behavior. Primates, 47, 113–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McBeath, N. M. and McGrew, W. C. (1982). Tools used by wild chimpanzees to obtain termites at Mt Assirik, Senegal: the influence of habitat. Journal of Human Evolution, 11, 6572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C. (1974). Tool use by wild chimpanzees in feeding upon driver ants. Journal of Human Evolution, 3, 501–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C. (1983). Animal foods in the diets of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): why cross-cultural variation? Journal of Ethology, 1, 4661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C. (1992). Chimpanzee Material Culture: Implications for Human Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C. and Collins, D. A. (1985). Tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) to obtain termites (Macrotermes herus) in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. American Journal of Primatology, 9, 4762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrew, W. C. and Marchant, L. F. (1998). Chimpanzee wears a knotted skin “necklace.” Pan Africa News, 5, 89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C. and Rogers, M. E. (1983). Chimpanzees, tools, and termites: new record from Gabon. American Journal of Primatology, 5, 171–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrew, W. C., Tutin, C. E. G., and Baldwin, P. J. (1979). Chimpanzees, tools, and termites: cross-cultural comparisons of Senegal, Tanzania, and Rio Muni. Man, 14, 185214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C., Pruetz, J. D., and Fulton, S. J. (2005). Chimpanzees use tools to harvest social insects at Fongoli, Senegal. Folia Primatologica, 76, 222–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muroyama, Y. (1991). Chimpanzees’ choices of prey between two sympatric species of Macrotermes in the Campo Animal Reserve, Cameroon. Human Evolution, 6, 143–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M. (2009). Aesthete in the forest? A female chimpanzee at Mahale collected and carried guineafowl feathers. Pan Africa News, 16, 1719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M. and Itoh, N. (2008). Hunting with tools by Mahale chimpanzees. Pan Africa News, 15, 36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M. and Nishida, T. (2012). Long-term field studies of chimpanzees at Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. In Long-Term Field Studies of Primates, ed. Kappeler, P. M. and Watts, D. P.. Heidelberg, Germany: Springer-Verlag, pp. 339–56.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1973). The ant-gathering behaviour by the use of tools among wild chimpanzees of the Mahali Mountains. Journal of Human Evolution, 2, 357–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1980a). Local differences in responses to water among wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatologica, 33, 189209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishida, T. (1980b). The leaf-clipping display: a newly-discovered expressive gesture in wild chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 9, 117–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1981). [The World of Wild Chimpanzees.] Tokyo: Chūkō-shinsho. In Japanese.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1987). Local traditions and cultural transmission. In Primate Societies, ed. Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W., and Struhsaker, T. T.. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, pp. 462–74.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1994). Review of recent findings on Mahale chimpanzees: implications and future research directions. In Chimpanzee Cultures, ed. Wrangham, R. W., McGrew, W. C., de Waal, F. B. M., and Heltne, P. G.. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, pp. 373–96.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1997). Sexual behavior of adult male chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates, 38, 379–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (2002). A self-medicating attempt to remove the sand flea from a toe by a young chimpanzee. Pan Africa News, 9, 56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (2003a). Harassment of mature female chimpanzees by young males in the Mahale Mountains. International Journal of Primatology, 24, 503–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (2003b). Individuality and flexibility of cultural behavior patterns in chimpanzees. In Animal Social Complexity and Intelligence, ed. de Waal, F. B. M. and Tyack, P.. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, pp. 392413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. and Hiraiwa, M. (1982). Natural history of a tool-using behavior by wild chimpanzees in feeding upon wood-boring ants. Journal of Human Evolution, 11, 7399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T., and Nakamura, M. (1993). Chimpanzee tool use to clear a blocked nasal passage. Folia Primatologica, 61, 218–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. and Uehara, S. (1980). Chimpanzees, tools, and termites: another examples from Tanzania. Current Anthropology, 21, 671–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. and Wallauer, W. (2003). Leaf-pile pulling: an unusual play pattern in wild chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology, 60, 167–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishida, T., Kano, T., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., and Nakamura, M. (1999). Ethogram and ethnography of Mahale chimpanzees. Anthropological Science, 107, 141–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T., Matsusaka, T., and McGrew, W. C. (2009). Emergence, propagation or disappearance of novel behavioral patterns in the habituated chimpanzees of Mahale: A review. Primates, 50, 2336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishie, H. (2011). Natural history of Camponotus ant fishing by the M group chimpanzees at the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates, 52, 329–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishie, H., Inoue, E., Matsusaka, T., Nakamura, M., and Zamma, K. (2014). Leaf-clipping is not a token of “courtship display”: from the descriptions of leaf-clipping behavior by the M group chimpanzees at Mahale, Tanzania. Abstracts of the 25th Congress of the International Primatological Society, 176.Google Scholar
O’Malley, R. C. and Nishie, H. (2013). Patterns of ant fishing for carpenter ants (Camponotus spp.) by Gombe, and chimpanzees, Mahale. Program of the 82nd Annual Meeting of the American Association of Physical Anthropologists, pp. 209.Google Scholar
O’Malley, R. C., Wallauer, W., Murray, C. M., and Goodall, J. (2012). The appearance and spread of ant fishing among the Kasekela chimpanzees of Gombe: a possible case of intercommunity cultural transmission. Current Anthropology, 53, 650–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pruetz, J. D. and Bertolani, P. (2007). Savanna chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus, hunt with tools. Current Biology, 17, 412–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabater Pi, J. (1974). An elementary industry of the chimpanzees in the Okorobikó Mountains, Rio Muni (Republic of Equatorial Guinea), West Africa. Primates, 15, 351–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanz, C., Morgan, D., and Gulick, S. (2004). New insights into chimpanzees, tools, and termites from the Congo Basin. The American Naturalist, 164, 567–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shumaker, R. W., Walkup, K. R., and Beck, B. B. (2011). Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals. Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugiyama, Y. (1985). The brush-stick of chimpanzees found in south-west Cameroon and their cultural characteristics. Primates, 26, 361–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugiyama, Y. and Koman, J. (1979). Tool-using and -making behavior in wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea. Primates, 20, 513–24.Google Scholar
Sugiyama, Y., Koman, J., and Sow, M. B. (1988). Ant-catching wands of wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea. Folia Primatologica, 51, 5660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suzuki, A. (1966). On the insect-eating habits among wild chimpanzees living in the savanna woodland of western Tanzania. Primates, 7, 481–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suzuki, S., Kuroda, S., and Nishihara, T. (1995). Tool-set for termite-fishing by chimpanzees in the Ndoki Forest, Congo. Behaviour, 132, 219–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takahata, Y. (1982). Termite fishing observed in M group chimpanzees. Mahale Mountains Chimpanzee Research Project Ecological Report, 18.Google Scholar
Tutin, C. E. G. and Fernandez, M. (1992). Insect-eating by sympatric lowland gorillas (Gorilla g. gorilla) and chimpanzees (Pan t. troglodytes) in the Lopé Reserve, Gabon. American Journal of Primatology, 28, 2940.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tutin, C. E. G., Ham, R., and Wrogemann, D. (1995). Tool-use by chimpanzees (Pan t. troglodytes) in the Lopé Reserve, Gabon. Primates, 36, 181–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uehara, S. (1982). Seasonal change in the techniques employed by wild chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania, to feed on termites (Pseudacanthotermes spiniger). Folia Primatologica, 37, 4476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uehara, S. (1999). Why don’t the chimpanzees of M group at Mahale fish for termites? Pan Africa News, 6, 22–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., et al. (2001). Charting cultural variation in chimpanzees. Behaviour, 138, 1481–516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yamamoto, S., Yamakoshi, G., Humle, T., and Matsuzawa, T. (2008). Invention and modification of a new tool use behavior: ant fishing in trees by a wild chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) at Bossou, Guinea. American Journal of Primatology, 70, 699702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zamma, K. (2002). Leaf grooming by a wild chimpanzee in Mahale. Primates, 43, 8790.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zamma, K. (2006). A louse egg left on a leaf. Pan Africa News, 13, 810.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Baldwin, P. J., Sabater Pí, J., McGrew, W. C., and Tutin, C. E. G. (1981). Comparisons of nests made by different populations of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Primates, 22, 474–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Basabose, A. K. and Yamagiwa, J. (2002). Factors affecting nesting site choice in chimpanzees at Tshibati, Kahuzi-Biega National Park: influence of sympatric gorillas. International Journal of Primatology, 23, 263–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brownlow, A. R., Plumptre, A. J., Reynolds, V., and Ward, R. (2001). Sources of variation in the nesting behavior of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Budongo Forest, Uganda. American Journal of Primatology, 55, 4955.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Casteren, A., Sellers, W. I., Thorpe, S. K. S., et al. (2012). Nest-building orangutans demonstrate engineering know-how to produce safe, comfortable beds. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA,109, 6873–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freemon, F. R., McNew, J. J., and Adey, W. R. (1969). Sleep of unrestrained chimpanzee: cortical and subcortical recordings. Experimental Neurology, 25, 129–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freemon, F. R., McNew, J. J., and Adey, W. R. (1970). Sleep of unrestrained chimpanzee: differences between first and last rapid eye movement periods. Folia Primatologica, 13, 144–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freemon, F. R., McNew, J. J., and Adey, W. R. (1971). Chimpanzee sleep stages. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 31, 485–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fruth, B. and Hohmann, G. (1996). Nest building behavior in the great apes: the great leap forward? In Great Ape Societies, ed. McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L., and Nishida, T.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 225–40.Google Scholar
Furuichi, T. and Hashimoto, C. (2004). Botanical and topographical factors influencing nesting-site selection by chimpanzees in Kalinzu Forest, Uganda. International Journal of Primatology, 25, 755–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghiglieri, M. P. (1984). The Chimpanzees of Kibale Forest: A Field Study of Ecology and Social Structure. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1962). Nest building behavior in the free ranging chimpanzee. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 102, 455–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Belknap.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1989). The Glossary of Chimpanzee Behaviors. Tucson, AZ: Jane Goodall Institute.Google Scholar
Hall, K. R. L. (1965). Behaviour and ecology of the wild patas monkey, Erythrocebus patas, in Uganda. Journal of Zoology, 148, 1587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansell, M. H. (2000). Bird Nests and Construction Behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hausfater, G. and Meade, B. J. (1982). Alternation of sleeping groves by yellow baboons (Papio cynocephalus) as a strategy for parasite avoidance. Primates, 23, 287–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayaki, H. (1988). Association partners of young chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates, 29, 147–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hernandez-Aguilar, R. A. (2009). Chimpanzee nest distribution and site reuse in a dry habitat: implications for early hominin ranging. Journal of Human Evolution, 57, 350–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M. (1989). Sex differences in the behavioral development of chimpanzees at Mahale. In Understanding Chimpanzees, ed. Heltne, P. G. and Marquardt, L. A.. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, pp. 104–15.Google Scholar
Hobson, J. A. (2005). Sleep is of the brain, by the brain and for the brain. Nature, 437, 1254–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ihobe, H. (2005). Life span of chimpanzee beds at the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Pan Africa News, 12, 1012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iwata, Y. and Ando, C. (2007). Bed and bed-site reuse by western lowland gorillas (Gorilla g. gorilla) in Moukalaba-Doudou National Park, Gabon. Primates, 48, 7780.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Izawa, K. and Itani, J. (1966). Chimpanzees in Kasakati Basin, Tanganyika. 1. Ecological study in the rainy season 1963–1964. Kyoto University African Studies, 1, 73156.Google Scholar
Kano, T. (1972). Distribution and adaptation of the chimpanzee on the eastern shore of Lake Tanganyika. Kyoto University African Studies, 7, 37129.Google Scholar
Kano, T. (1983). An ecological study of the pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Yalosidi, Republic of Zaire. International Journal of Primatology, 4, 131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kouakou, C. Y., Boesch, C., and Kuehl, H. (2009). Estimating chimpanzee population size with nest counts: validating methods in Taï National Park. American Journal of Primatology, 71, 447–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1968). The behaviour of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve. Animal Behaviour Monographs, 1, 165311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lumsden, W. H. R. (1951). The night-resting habits of monkeys in a small area on the edge of the Semliki Forest, Uganda: a study in relation to the epidemiology of Sylvan yellow fever. The Journal of Animal Ecology, 20, 1130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacKinnon, J. (1974). The behaviour and ecology of wild orang-utans (Pongo pygmaeus). Animal Behaviour, 22, 374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marchesi, P., Marchesi, N., Fruth, B., and Boesch, C. (1995). Census and distribution of chimpanzees in Côte d’Ivoire. Primates, 36, 591607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, A. and Matthews, A. (2004). Survey of gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes troglodytes) in Southwestern Cameroon. Primates, 45, 1524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C. (1992). Chimpanzee Material Culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muroyama, Y. (1991). Chimpanzees’ choices of prey between two sympatric species of Macrotermes in the Campo Animal Reserve, Cameroon. Human Evolution, 6, 143–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1981). [The World of Wild Chimpanzees.] Tokyo: Chūkō-shinsho. In Japanese.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1994). Review of recent findings in Mahale chimpanzees: implications and future research directions. In Chimpanzee Cultures, ed. Wrangham, R. W., McGrew, W. C., de Waal, F. B. M., and Heltne, P. G.. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (2012). Chimpanzees of the Lakeshore: Natural History and Culture at Mahale. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nishida, T., Zamma, K., Matsusaka, T., Inaba, A., and McGrew, W. C. (2010). Chimpanzee Behavior in the Wild. Tokyo: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nissen, H. W. (1931). A field study of the chimpanzee: observations of chimpanzee behavior and environment in western French Guinea. Comparative Psychology Monographs, 8, 1122.Google Scholar
Ogawa, H. and Takahashi, H. (2003). Triadic positions of Tibetan macaques huddling at a sleeping site. International Journal of Primatology, 24, 591606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ogawa, H., Idani, G., Moore, J., Pintea, L., and Hernandez-Aguilar, A. (2007). Sleeping parties and nest distribution of chimpanzees in the savanna woodland, Ugalla, Tanzania. International Journal of Primatology, 28, 1397–412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ogawa, H., Yoshikawa, M., and Idani, G. (2013). The population and habitat preferences of chimpanzees in non-protected areas of Tanzania. Pan Africa News, 20, 15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plumptre, A. J. and Reynolds, V. (1996). Censusing chimpanzees in the Budongo Forest, Uganda. International Journal of Primatology, 17, 8599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plumptre, A. J. and Reynolds, V. (1997). Nesting behavior of chimpanzees: implications for census. International Journal of Primatology, 18, 475–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pruetz, J. D. and Bertolani, P. (2009). Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) behavioral responses to stresses associated with living in a savanna-mosaic environment: implications for hominin adaptations to open habitats. PaleoAnthropology, 2009, 252–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pruetz, J. D., Fulton, S. J., Marchant, L. F., et al. (2008). Arboreal nesting as anti-predator adaptation by savanna chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) in southeastern Senegal. American Journal of Primatology, 70, 393401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pusey, A. E. (1990) Behavioural changes at adolescent in chimpanzees. Behaviour, 115, 203–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynolds, V. (2005). The Chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest: Ecology, Behaviour, and Conservation. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Samson, D. R. and Hunt, K. D. (2012). A thermodynamic comparison of arboreal and terrestrial sleeping sites for dry-habitat chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) at the Toro-Semliki Wildlife Reserve, Uganda. American Journal of Primatology, 74, 811–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaller, G. (1963). The Mountain Gorilla: Ecology and Behavior. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Sousa, J., Barata, A. V., Sousa, C., Casanova, C. C. N., and Vicente, L. (2011). Chimpanzee oil-palm use in southern Cantanhez National Park, Guinea-Bissau. American Journal of Primatology, 73, 485–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanford, C. B. and O’Malley, R. C. (2008). Sleeping tree choice by Bwindi chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology, 70, 642–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, F. A. (2011). Why sleep in a nest? Empirical testing of the function of simple shelters made by wild chimpanzees. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 146, 313–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, F. A. and Pruetz, J. D. (2013). Do chimpanzee nests serve an anti-predatory function? American Journal of Primatology, 75, 593604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, F. A., Pruetz, J. D., and Hansell, M. H. (2007). Do chimpanzees build comfortable nests? American Journal of Primatology, 69, 930–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, F. A., Piel, A. K., and McGrew, W. C. (2011). Living archaeology: artifacts of specific nest site fidelity in wild chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 61, 388–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tagg, N., Willie, J., Petre, C.-A., and Haggis, O. (2013). Ground night nesting in chimpanzees: new insights from central chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes troglodytes) in South-East Cameroon. Folia Primatologica, 84, 362–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Takasaki, H., Nishida, T., Uehara, S., et al. (1990). Summary of meteorological data at Mahale research camps, 1973–1988. In The Chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains: Sexual and Life History Strategies, ed. Nishida, T.. Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press, pp. 291300.Google Scholar
Tsuchiya, S. (1978). [Nocturnal behavior of Japanese monkeys.] [Monkey], 22(6), 613. In Japanese.Google Scholar
Tutin, C. E. G. and Fernandez, M. (1984). Nationwide census of gorilla (Gorilla g. gorilla) and chimpanzee (Pan t. troglodytes) populations in Gabon. American Journal of Primatology, 6, 313–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tutin, C. E. G., McGrew, W. C., and Baldwin, P. J. (1981). Responses of wild chimpanzees to potential predators. In Primate Behavior and Sociobiology, ed. Chiarelli, A. B. and Corruccini, R. S.. Berlin: Springer-Verlag, pp. 136–41.Google Scholar
Videan, E. N. (2006). Bed-building in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): the importance of early rearing. American Journal of Primatology, 68, 745–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yamakoshi, G. and Sugiyama, Y. (1995). Pestle-pounding behavior of wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea: a newly observed tool-using behavior. Primates, 36, 489500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zamma, K. (2014).What makes wild chimpanzees wake up at night? Primates, 55, 51–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zamma, K. and Makelele, M. (2012). Comparison of the longevity of chimpanzee beds between two areas in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Pan Africa News, 19, 25–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zamma, K., Nakashima, M., and Ramadhani, A. (2011). Mahale chimpanzees starts to eat oil palm. Pan Africa News, 18, 68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×