Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
  • Print publication year: 2005
  • Online publication date: February 2010

12 - Assessing Evidential Support in Uncertain Environments

Summary

In this chapter, we explore a classic problem in psychology: How do individuals draw on their previous experience in an uncertain environment to make a prediction or diagnosis on the basis of a set of informational cues? Intuitive predictions based on multiple cues are often highly sensitive to environmental contingencies yet at the same time often exhibit pronounced and consistent biases.

Our research has focused on judgments of the probability of an outcome based on binary cues (e.g., present/absent), in which the diagnostic value of those cues has been learned from direct experience in an uncertain environment. For example, in medical diagnosis, we might predict which disease a patient has based on a symptom that a patient does or does not exhibit. We have developed a model that captures both the strengths and weaknesses of intuitive judgments of this kind (the Evidential Support Accumulation Model, ESAM; Koehler, White, & Grondin, 2003). The model assumes that the frequency with which each cue is observed to have co-occurred with the outcome variable of interest is stored. The perceived diagnostic value of each cue, based on these frequencies, is calculated in a normatively appropriate fashion and integrated with the prior probability of the outcome to arrive at a final probability judgment for a given outcome.

ESAM is designed to account for people's behavior in uncertain environments created in the lab that are, effectively by definition, sampled without bias.

Related content

Powered by UNSILO
References
Anderson, N. H. (1981). Foundations of information integration theory. New York: Academic Press
Brooks, L. R. (1978). Non-analytic concept formation and memory for instances. In Rosch, E. & Lloyd, B. B. (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 169–211). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Dawes, R. M. (1979). The robust beauty of improper linear models in decision making. American Psychologist, 34, 571–582
Dougherty, M. R. P., Gettys, C. E., & Ogden, E. E. (1999). MINERVA-DM: A memory processes model for judgments of likelihood. Psychological Review, 106 (1), 108–209
Edgell, S. E. (1978). Configural information processing in two-cue probability learning. Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes, 22, 404–416
Edgell, S. E. (1980). Higher order configural information processing in nonmetric multiple-cue probability learning. Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes, 25, 1–14
Fischhoff, B., Slovic, P., & Lichtenstein, S. (1978). Fault trees: Sensitivity of estimated failure probabilities to problem representation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 4, 330–334
Gigerenzer, G., & Goldstein, D. G. (1999). Betting on one good reason: The take the best heuristic. In Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. M., & the ABC Research Group (Eds.), Simple heuristics that make us smart: Evolution and cognition. London: Oxford University Press
Hintzman, D. L. (1988). Judgments of frequency and recognition memory in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review, 95, 528–551
Juslin, P., Nilsson, H., & Olsson, H. (2001). Where do probability judgments come from? Evidence for similarity-graded probability. In Moore, J. & Stenning, K. (Eds.), Proceedings of the twenty-third annual conference of the cognitive science society. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Juslin, P., & Persson, M. (2002). PROBabilities from Exemplars (PROBEX): A “lazy” algorithm for probabilistic inference from generic knowledge. Cognitive Science, 26, 563–607
Koehler, D. J. (2000). Probability judgment in three-category classification learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 28–52
Koehler, D. J., Brenner, L. A., & Tversky, A. (1997). The enhancement effect in probability judgment. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 10, 293–313
Koehler, D. J., White, C. M., & Grondin, R. (2003). An evidential support accumulation model of subjective probability. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 152–197
Luce, R. D. (1959). Individual choice behavior. New York: Wiley
Martignon, L., & Laskey, K. B. (1999). Bayesian benchmarks for fast and frugal heuristics. In Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. M., & the ABC Research Group (Eds.), Simple heuristics that make us smart: Evolution and cognition. London: Oxford University Press
Medin, D. L., & Schaffer, M. M. (1978). Context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review, 85, 207–238
Novemsky, N., & Kronzon, S. (1999) How are base-rates used, when they are used: A comparison of additive and Bayesian models of base-rate use. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 12, 55–69
Rottenstreich, Y., & Tversky, A. (1997). Unpacking, repacking, and anchoring: Advances in support theory. Psychological Review, 104, 406–415
Sedlmeier, P., & Betsch, T. (2002). ETC. Frequency processing and cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Tversky, A., & Koehler, D. J. (1994). Support theory: A nonextensional representation of subjective probability. Psychological Review, 101, 547–567
Wasserman, E. A., Dorner, W. W., & Kao, S. F. (1990). Contributions of specific cell information to judgments of inter-event contingency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16, 509–521
White, C. M. (2002). Modelling the influence of cue dependencies and missing information in multiple-cue probability learning. Master's thesis, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
White, C. M., & Koehler, D. J. (2003). Modeling the influence of cue dependencies in multiple-cue probability learning. Unpublished manuscript, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada