Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T06:02:12.699Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Inference generation in text comprehension: automatic and strategic processes in the construction of a mental representation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 May 2015

Edward J. O'Brien
Affiliation:
University of New Hampshire
Anne E. Cook
Affiliation:
University of Utah
Robert F. Lorch, Jr
Affiliation:
University of Kentucky
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackermann, P. (1988). Determinants of individual differences during skill acquisition: Cognitive abilities and informational processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 288318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, B. C., Bell, L. C., & Perfetti, C. A. (1995). A trading relationship between reading skill and domain knowledge in children's text comprehension. Discourse Processes, 20, 307–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Albrecht, J. E., & O'Brien, E. J. (1993). Updating a mental model: maintaining both local and global coherence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1061–70.Google Scholar
Barnes, M. A., Dennis, M., & Haefele-Kalvaitis, J. (1996). The effects of knowledge availability and knowledge accessibility on coherence and elaborative inferencing in children from six to fifteen years of age. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 61, 216–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bauer, P. J. (2013). Theory and processes in memory development: infancy and early childhood. In Holliday, R. E. & Marche, T. A. (eds.). Child Forensic Psychology (pp. 938). New York: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beeman, M. J., Bowden, E. M., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2000). Right and left hemisphere cooperation for drawing predictive and coherence inferences during normal story comprehension. Brain and Language, 71, 310336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Black, J. B., & Bower, G. H. (1980). Story understanding as problem-solving. Poetics, 9, 223–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, C. P., Fletcher, C. R., van den Broek, P., Reitz, L., & Shapiro, B. P. (1990). An online assessment of causal reasoning during comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 18, 6571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bråten, I., Britt, M. A., Strømsø, H. I., & Rouet, J.-F. (2011). The role of epistemic beliefs in the comprehension of multiple expository texts: toward an integrated model. Educational Psychologist, 46, 4870.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braze, D., Shankweiler, D., Ni, W., & Palumbo, L. C. (2002). Readers’ eye movements distinguish anomalies of form and content. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31, 2544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Britton, B. K., Stimson, M., Stennett, B., & Gülgöz, S. (1998). Learning from instructional text: test of an individual-differences model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 476–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children's reading comprehension ability: concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 3142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chi, M. T., & Koeske, R. D. (1983). Network representation of a child's dinosaur knowledge. Developmental Psychology, 19, 2939.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook, A. E., Limber, J. E., & O'Brien, E. J. (2001). Situation-based context and the availability of predictive inferences. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 220–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook, A. E., & Myers, J. L. (2004). Processing discourse roles in scripted narratives: the influences of context and world knowledge. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 268–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook, A. E., & O'Brien, E. J. (2014). Knowledge activation, integration, and validation during narrative text comprehension. Discourse Processes, 51, 2649.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Einhorn, H. J., & Hogarth, R. M. (1986). Judging probable cause. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Estevez, A., & Calvo, M. G. (2000). Working memory capacity and time course of predictive inferences. Memory, 8, 5161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferstl, E. C., Neumann, J., Bogler, C., & Von Cramon, D. Y. (2008). The extended language network: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on text comprehension. Human Brain Mapping, 29, 581–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, C. R., & Bloom, C. P. (1988). Causal reasoning in the comprehension of simple narrative texts. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 235–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fletcher, C. R., Chrysler, S. T., van den Broek, P., Deaton, J. A., & Bloom, C. P. (1995). The role of co-occurrence, coreference, and causality in the coherence of conjoined sentences. In Lorch, R. F. & O'Brien, E. J. (eds.), Sources of Coherence in Reading (pp. 203–18). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A., Varner, K. R., & Faust, M. E. (1990). Investigating differences in general comprehension skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 430–45.Google ScholarPubMed
Goldman, S. R. (2004). Cognitive aspects of constructing meaning through and across multiple texts. In Shuart-Faris, N. & Bloome, D. (eds.), Uses of Intertextuality in Classroom and Educational Research (pp. 317–52). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graesser, A. C., & Clark, L. F. (1985). Structures and Procedures of Implicit Knowledge. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Graesser, A. C., McNamara, D. S., Louwerse, M. M., & Cai, Z. (2004). Coh-Metrix: analysis of text on cohesion and language. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 193202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helder, A., van den Broek, P., Van Leijenhorst, L., & Beker, K. (2013). Sources of comprehension problems during reading. In Miller, B., Cutting, L., & McCardle, P. (eds.), Unraveling Reading Comprehension: Behavioral, Neurobiological, and Genetic Components (pp. 4353). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Huitema, J. S., Dopkins, S., Klin, C. M., & Myers, J. L. (1993). Connecting goals and actions during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1053–60.Google ScholarPubMed
Hyönä, J., Lorch, R. F. Jr., & Rinck, M. (2003). Eye movement measures to study global text processing. In Hyönä, J., Radach, R., & Deubel, H. (eds.), The Mind's Eye: Cognitive and Applied Aspects of Eye Movement Research (pp. 313–34). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Isberner, M. B., & Richter, T. (2014). Comprehension and validation: separable stages of information processing? A case for epistemic monitoring in language comprehension. In Rapp, D. N. & Braasch, J. L. G. (eds.), Processing Inaccurate Information: Theoretical and Applied Perspectives from Cognitive Science and the Educational Sciences (pp. 245–76). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Strauss and Giroux.Google Scholar
Keefe, D. E., & McDaniel, M. A. (1993). The time course and durability of predictive inferences. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 446–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendeou, P., Bohn-Gettler, C., White, M. J., & van den Broek, P. (2008). Children's inference generation across different media. Journal of Research in Reading, 31, 259–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendeou, P., & O'Brien, E. J. (2014). The knowledge revision components (KReC) framework: processes and mechanisms. In Rapp, D. N. & Braasch, J. L. G. (eds.), Processing Inaccurate Information: Theoretical and Applied Perspectives from Cognitive Science and the Educational Sciences (pp. 353–77). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kendeou, P., Savage, R., & van den Broek, P. (2009). Revisiting the simple view of reading. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 353–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendeou, P., Smith, E. R., & O'Brien, E. J. (2013). Updating during reading comprehension: Why causality matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 854–65.Google ScholarPubMed
Kendeou, P., & van den Broek, P. (2007). The effects of prior knowledge and text structure on comprehension processes during reading of scientific texts. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1567–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P., White, M. J., & Lynch, J. S. (2007). Comprehension in preschool and early elementary children: skill development and strategy interventions. In McNamara, D. S. (ed.), Reading Comprehension Strategies: Theories, Interventions, and Technologies (pp. 2745). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kieffer, M. J., Vukovic, R. K., & Berry, D., (2013). Roles of attention shifting and inhibitory control in fourth-grade reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 48, 333–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction integration model. Psychological Review, 95, 163–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kintsch, W., & Van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85, 363–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klin, C. M. (1995). Causal inferences in reading: from immediate activation to long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 1483–94.Google ScholarPubMed
Klin, C. M., Guzmán, A. E., & Levine, W. H. (1999). Prevalence and persistence of predictive inferences. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 593604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koornneef, A. W., & van Berkum, J. J. (2006). On the use of verb-based implicit causality in sentence comprehension: evidence from self-paced reading and eye tracking. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 445–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuperberg, G. R., Lakshmanan, B. M., Caplan, D. N., & Holcomb, P. J. (2006). Making sense of discourse: an fMRI study of causal inferencing across sentences. Neuroimage, 33, 343–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lassonde, K. A., & O'Brien, E. J. (2009). Contextual specificity in the activation of predictive inferences. Discourse Processes, 46, 426–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lea, R. B., Kayser, P. A., Mulligan, E. J., & Myers, J. L. (2002). Do readers make inferences about conversational topics? Memory & Cognition, 30, 945–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linderholm, T. (2002). Predictive inference generation as a function of working memory capacity and causal text constraints. Discourse Processes, 34, 259–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linderholm, T., & van den Broek, P. (2002). The effects of reading purpose and working memory capacity on the processing of expository text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 778–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linderholm, T., Virtue, S., Tzeng, Y., & van den Broek, P. (2004). Fluctuations in the availability of information during reading: capturing cognitive processes using the Landscape model. Discourse Processes, 37, 165–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, D. L., & Baynes, K. (2002). Discourse representation in the two cerebral hemispheres. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 228–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Long, D. L., & Lea, R. B. (2005). Have we been searching for meaning in all the wrong places? Defining the ‘search after meaning’ principle in comprehension. Discourse Processes, 39, 279–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorch, E. P., Diener, M. B., Sanchez, R. P., Milich, R., Welsh, R., & van den Broek, P. (1999). The effects of story structure on the recall of stories in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 273–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorch, E. P., Eastham, D., Milich, R., Lemberger, C. C., Polley Sanchez, R., Welsh, R., & van den Broek, P. (2004). Difficulties in comprehending causal relations among children with ADHD: the role of cognitive engagement. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 5663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorch, R., LeMarié, J., & Grant, R. (2011). Signaling hierarchical and sequential organization in expository text. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15, 267–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorch, E. P., Sanchez, R. P., van den Broek, P., Milich, R., Murphy, E. L., Lorch, R. F. Jr, & Welsh, R. (1999). The relation of story structure properties to recall of television stories in young children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and nonreferred peers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27, 293309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lynch, J. S., & van den Broek, P. (2007). Understanding the glue of narrative structure: Children's on- and off-line inferences about characters’ goals. Cognitive Development, 22, 323–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynch, J. S., van den Broek, P., Kremer, K. E., Kendeou, P., White, M. J., & Lorch, E. P. (2008). The development of narrative comprehension and its relation to other early reading skills. Reading Psychology, 29, 327–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magliano, J. P., Trabasso, T., & Graesser, A. C. (1999). Strategic processing during comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 615–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, R. A., & Just, M. A. (2006). Neuroimaging contributions to the understanding of discourse processes. In Traxler, M. & Gernsbacher, M. A. (eds.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics (pp. 765–99). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
McCrudden, M. T., Magliano, J. P., & Schraw, G. (2010). Exploring how relevance instructions affect personal reading intentions, reading goals and text processing: a mixed methods study. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35, 229–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1981). The comprehension processes and memory structures involved in instrumental inference. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 671–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1986). Inferences about predictable events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 8291.Google ScholarPubMed
McNamara, D. S., & Kendeou, P. (2011). Translating advances in reading comprehension research to educational practice. International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education, 4, 3346.Google Scholar
McNamara, D. S., & Magliano, J. (2009). Toward a comprehensive model of comprehension. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 51, 297384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, A. C., & Keenan, J. M. (2009). How word decoding skill impacts text memory: the centrality deficit and how domain knowledge can compensate. Annals of Dyslexia, 59, 99113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, J. D., Klin, C. M., & Myers, J. L. (1993). Forward inferences in narrative text. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 464–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myers, J. L., Cook, A. E., Kambe, G., Mason, R. A., & O'Brien, E. J. (2000). Semantic and episodic effects on bridging inferences. Discourse Processes, 29, 179–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myers, J. L., & O'Brien, E. J. (1998). Accessing the discourse representation during reading. Discourse Processes, 26, 131–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D., van den Broek, P., & Barrón Ruiz, A. B. (1999). The influence of reading purpose on inference generation and comprehension in reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 488–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oakhill, J., & Cain, K. (2007). Introduction to comprehension development. In Cain, K. & Oakhill, J. (eds.), Children's Comprehension Problems in Oral and Written Language: A Cognitive Perspective (pp. 340). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Oakhill, J., & Cain, K. (2012). The precursors of reading ability in young readers: evidence from a four-year longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 91121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Brien, E. J., & Myers, J. L. (1987). The role of causal connections in retrieval of text. Memory & Cognition, 15, 419–27.Google ScholarPubMed
O'Brien, E. J., Plewes, P. S., & Albrecht, J. E. (1990). Antecedent retrieval processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 241–49.Google ScholarPubMed
O'Brien, E. J., Raney, G. E., Albrecht, J. E., & Rayner, K. (1997). Processes involved in the resolution of explicit anaphors. Discourse Processes, 23, 124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Brien, E. J., Rizzella, M. L., Albrecht, J. E., & Halleran, J. G. (1998). Updating a situation model: a memory-based text processing view. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 1200–10.Google Scholar
O'Brien, E. J., Shank, D. M., Myers, J. L., & Rayner, K. (1988). Elaborative inferences during reading: Do they occur online? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 410–20.Google ScholarPubMed
Ouellette, G.P. (2006). What's meaning got to do with it: the role of vocabulary in word reading and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 554–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, P. D., & Hamm, D. N. (2005). The assessment of reading comprehension: a review of practices-past, present, and future. In Paris, S. G. & Stahl, S. A. (eds.), Children's Reading Comprehension and Assessment (pp. 1370). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pearson, P. D., Hansen, J., & Gordon, C. (1979). The effect of background knowledge on young children's comprehension of explicit and implicit information. Journal of Literacy Research, 11, 201–9.Google Scholar
Pearson, P. D., Moje, E., & Greenleaf, C. (2010). Literacy and science: each in the service of the other. Science, 328, 459–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peracchi, K. A., & O'Brien, E. J. (2004). Character profiles and the activation of predictive inferences. Memory & Cognition, 32, 1044–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perfetti, C. (2007). Reading ability: lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 357–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C., & Frishkoff, G. A. (2008).The neural bases of text and discourse processing. In Stemmer, B. & Whitaker, H. A. (eds.), Handbook of the Neuroscience of Language (pp. 165–74). Cambridge, MA: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In Verhoeven, L., Elbro, C., & Reitsma, P. (eds.), Precursors of Functional Literacy (pp. 189213). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., Landi, N., & Oakhill, J. (2005). The acquisition of reading comprehension skill. In Snowling, M. J. & Hulme, C. (eds.), The Science of Reading: A Handbook (pp. 227–47). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., Rouet, J.-F., & Britt, M. A. (1999). Towards a theory of documents representation. In van Oostendorp, H. & Goldman, S. R. (eds.), The Construction of Mental Representations during Reading (pp. 99122). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C., & Stafura, J. (2013). Word knowledge in a theory of reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18, 116.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C., Yang, C. L., & Schmalhofer, F. (2008). Comprehension skill and word-to-text integration processes. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 303–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prat, C. S., Mason, R. A., & Just, M. A. (2011). Individual differences in the neural basis of causal inferencing. Brain and Language, 116, 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pressley, M., & McCormick, C. B. (1995). Advanced Educational Psychology for Educators, Researchers, and Policymakers. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Rawson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (2002). How does background information improve memory for text content? Memory & Cognition, 30, 768–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richter, T. (2011). Cognitive flexibility and epistemic validation in learning from multiple texts. In Elen, J., Stahl, E., Bromme, R., & Clarebout, G. (eds.), Links between Beliefs and Cognitive Flexibility (pp. 125–40). Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Richter, T., Schroeder, S., & Wöhrmann, B. (2009). You don't have to believe everything you read: background knowledge permits fast and efficient validation of information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 538–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanchez, C. A., & Wiley, J. (2006). An examination of the seductive details effect in terms of working memory capacity. Memory & Cognition, 34, 344–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanders, T. J. M., & Noordman, L. G. M. (2000). The role of coherence relations and their linguistic markers in text processing. Discourse Processes, 29, 3760.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanders, T., & Spooren, W. (2007). Discourse and text structure. In Geeraerts, D. & Cuykens, H. (eds.), Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 916–43). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sanford, A. J., Sanford, A. J., Molle, J., & Emmott, C. (2006). Shallow processing and attention capture in written and spoken discourse. Discourse Processes, 42, 109–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schank, R., & Abelson, R. (1977). Scripts, Plans, Goals and Understanding: An Inquiry into Human Knowledge Structures. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Singer, M. (1994). Discourse inference processes. In Gernsbacher, M. A. (ed.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics (pp. 479515). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Singer, M. (2006). Verification of text ideas during reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 574–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, M. (2013). Validation in reading comprehension. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 361–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, M., & Halldorson, M. (1996). Constructing and validating motive bridging inferences. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, M., Halldorson, M., Lear, J. C., & Andrusiak, P. (1992). Validation of causal bridging inferences in discourse understanding. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 507–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strasser, K., & del Río, F. (2014). The role of comprehension monitoring, theory of mind, and vocabulary depth in predicting story comprehension and recall of kindergarten children. Reading Research Quarterly, 49, 169–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sundermeier, B. A., van den Broek, P., & Zwaan, R. A. (2005). Causal coherence and the availability of locations and objects during narrative comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 33, 462–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sundermeier, B. A., Virtue, S. M., Marsolek, C. J., & van den Broek, P. (2005). Evidence for dissociable neural mechanisms underlying inference generation in familiar and less-familiar scenarios. Brain and Language, 95, 402–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tilstra, J., McMaster, K., van den Broek, P., Kendeou, P., & Rapp, D. N. (2009). Simple but complex: Components of the simple view of reading across grade levels. Journal of Research in Reading, 32, 383401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tippett, C. D. (2010). Refutation text in science education: a review of two decades of research. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 8, 951–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trabasso, T., Secco, T., & van den Broek, P. W. (1984). Causal cohesion and story coherence. In Mandl, H., Stein, N. L., & Trabasso, T. (eds.), Learning and Comprehension of Text (pp. 83111). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Trabasso, T., & Sperry, L. (1985). Causal relatedness and importance of story events. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 595611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trabasso, T., & van den Broek, P. W. (1985). Causal thinking and the representation of narrative events. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 612–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trabasso, T., van den Broek, P., & Suh, S. Y. (1989). Logical necessity and transitivity of causal relations in stories. Discourse Processes, 12, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tzeng, Y., van den Broek, P., Kendeou, P., & Lee, C. (2005). The computational implementation of the Landscape model: modeling inferential processes and memory representations of text comprehension. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 277–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Berkum, J. J., Brown, C. M., Zwitserlood, P., Kooijman, V., & Hagoort, P. (2005). Anticipating upcoming words in discourse: evidence from ERPs and reading times. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 443–67.Google ScholarPubMed
Van den Broek, P. W. (1988). The effects of causal relations and hierarchical position on the importance of story statements. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Broek, P. W. (1989). Causal reasoning and inference making in judging the importance of story statements. Child Development, 60, 286–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van den Broek, P. W. (1990). Causal inferences in the comprehension of narrative texts. In Graesser, A. C. & Bower, G. H. (eds.), Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Inferences and Text Comprehension (pp. 175–96). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P. W. (1994). Comprehension and memory of narrative texts: inferences and coherence. In Gernsbacher, M. A. (ed.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics (pp. 539–88). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P. W. (2005). Integrating memory-based and constructionist processes in accounts of reading comprehension. Discourse Processes, 39, 299316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Bohn-Gettler, C., Kendeou, P., Carlson, S., & White, M. J. (2011). When a reader meets a text: the role of standards of coherence in reading comprehension. In McCrudden, M. T., Magliano, J. P., & Schraw, G. (eds.), Text Relevance and Learning from Text (pp. 123–40). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P., & Espin, C. A. (2012). Connecting cognitive theory and assessment: Measuring individual differences in reading comprehension. School Psychology Review, 41, 315–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Broek, P., & Gustafson, M. (1999). Comprehension and memory for texts: three generations of research. In Goldman, S. R., Graesser, A. C. & van den Broek, P. (eds.), Narrative Comprehension, Causality, and Coherence: Essays in Honor of Tom Trabasso (pp. 1534). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P. W., Helder, A., & van Leijenhorst, L. (2013). Sensitivity to structural centrality: developmental and individual differences in reading comprehension skills. In Britt, M. A., Goldman, S. R., & Rouet, J-F (eds.), Reading: From Words to Multiple Texts (pp. 132–46). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P., & Kendeou, P. (2008). Cognitive processes in comprehension of science texts: the role of co-activation in confronting misconceptions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 335–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Kendeou, P., Lousberg, S., & Visser, G. (2011). Preparing for reading comprehension: fostering text comprehension skills in preschool and early elementary school children. International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education, 4, 259–68.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P., & Lorch, R. F. Jr. (1993). Network representations of causal relations in memory for narrative texts: evidence from primed recognition. Discourse Processes, 16, 7598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Lorch, R. F. Jr., Linderholm, T., & Gustafson, M. (2001). The effects of readers’ goals on inference generation and memory for texts. Memory & Cognition, 29, 1081–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van den Broek, P., Lorch, E. P., & Thurlow, R. (1996). Children's and adults’ memory for television stories: the role of causal factors, story-grammar categories, and hierarchical level. Child Development, 67, 3010–28.Google ScholarPubMed
Van den Broek, P., Lynch, J. S., Naslund, J., Ievers-Landis, C. E., & Verduin, K. (2003). The development of comprehension of main ideas in narratives: evidence from the selection of titles. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 707–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Rapp, D., & Kendeou, P. (2005). Integrating memory-based and constructionist processes in accounts of reading comprehension. Discourse Processes, 39, 299316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Broek, P. W., Risden, K., Fletcher, C. R., & Thurlow, R. (1996) A ‘landscape’ view of reading: fluctuating patterns of activation and the construction of a stable memory representation. In Britton, B. K. & Graesser, A. C. (eds.), Models of Understanding Text (pp. 165–87). Mahwah , NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Risden, K., & Husebye-Hartmann, E. (1995). The role of reader's standards of coherence in the generation of inferences during reading. In Lorch, E. P. & O'Brien, E. J. (eds.), Sources of Coherence in Reading (pp. 353–74). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Rohleder, L., & Narváez, D. (1996). Causal inferences in the comprehension of literary texts. In Kreuz, R. J. & MacNealy, M. S. (eds.), Empirical Approaches to Literature and Aesthetics (pp. 179200). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P. W., & Trabasso, T. (1986). Causal networks versus goal hierarchies in summarizing text. Discourse Processes, 9, 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Broek, P., White, M. J., Kendeou, P., & Carlson, S. (2009). Reading between the lines: developmental and individual differences in cognitive processes in reading comprehension. In Wagner, R., Schatschneider, C., & Phythian-Sence, C. (eds.), Beyond Decoding: The Behavioral and Biological Foundations of Reading Comprehension (pp. 107–23). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Young, M., Tzeng, Y., & Linderholm, T. (1999). The Landscape model of reading: inferences and the online construction of a memory representation. In van Oostendorp, H. & Goldman, S. R. (eds.), The Construction of Mental Representations during Reading (pp. 7198). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Virtue, S., van den Broek, P., & Linderholm, T. (2006). Hemispheric processing of inferences: the effects of textual constraint and working memory capacity. Memory & Cognition, 34, 1341–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development, 69, 848–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitney, P., Ritchie, B. G., & Clark, M. B. (1991). Working-memory capacity and the use of elaborative inferences in text comprehension. Discourse Processes, 14, 133–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitney, P., Ritchie, B. G., & Crane, R. S. (1992). The effect of foregrounding on readers’ use of predictive inferences. Memory & Cognition, 20, 424–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiley, J., & Myers, J. L. (2003): Availability and accessibility of information and causal inferences from scientific text. Discourse Processes, 36, 109–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. P. (1993). Comprehension of students with and without learning disabilities: Identification of narrative themes and idiosyncratic text representations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 631–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolman, C., van den Broek, P. W., & Lorch, R. F. (1997). Effects of causal structure on immediate and delayed story recall by children with mild mental retardation, children with learning disabilities, and children without disabilities. Journal of Special Education, 30, 439–55.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R. A., Magliano, J. P., & Graesser, A. C. (1995). Dimensions of situation model construction in narrative comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 386–97.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×