Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-20T03:43:08.721Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Confluence of Individual and Caregiver Influences on Socioemotional Development in Typical and Atypical Populations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 May 2010

Carol M. Worthman
Affiliation:
Emory University, Atlanta
Paul M. Plotsky
Affiliation:
Emory University, Atlanta
Daniel S. Schechter
Affiliation:
Hôpitaux Universitaires de Genève
Constance A. Cummings
Affiliation:
Foundation for Psychocultural Research, California
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

Children exhibit a range of individual differences in reactions to the world at birth. Some are easily soothed, develop regular sleep-wake cycles, react positively with approach behaviors toward novel stimuli, and easily adapt to environmental changes. Other infants display high rates of negative emotions, are difficult to soothe, become easily overwhelmed by novel stimuli, react negatively to environmental changes, and have irregular sleep-wake patterns. These early individual differences make up what we call temperament. The interaction of temperament and environmental influences presumably shapes socioemotional development.

In this chapter, we illustrate how individual and caregiver differences influence socioemotional development in typical and atypical populations. The chapter comprises two major sections. The first section sets forth an overview of three models of development that can inform socioemotional development: temperament, attachment, and interactionist approaches. For each model, we give a brief historical overview and discuss definitional, conceptual, and cross-cultural issues. Section two presents how these models may shed light on socioemotional development in typical and atypical populations, using childhood shyness and childhood maltreatment and autism, respectively, as examples. We conclude with suggestions for future work examining individual and caregiver influences on socioemotional development.

MODELS OF DEVELOPMENT

Temperament Theories

Historical Precedent and Definitional Issues. The first western scientific theory of temperament was proposed in the 2nd century A.D. by Galen, a Greek physician. Galen's theory suggested that individual temperament differences could be explained by the balance between four bodily humors or fluids: flegma, chole, melanchole, and sanguis (Galen, 170 A.D.).

Type
Chapter
Information
Formative Experiences
The Interaction of Caregiving, Culture, and Developmental Psychobiology
, pp. 106 - 135
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adrien, J. L., Perrot, A., Sauvage, D., Leddet, I., Larmande, C., Hameury, L., et al. (1992). Early symptoms in autism from family home movies. Evaluation and comparison between 1st and 2nd year of life using I.B.S.E. scale. Acta Paedopsychiatrica, 55, 71–75.Google ScholarPubMed
Ahadi, S., Rothbart, M. K., & Ye, R. (1993). Children's temperament in the United States and China: Similarities and differences. European Journal of Personality, 7, 359–378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Bell, S. M. (1969). Some contemporary patterns in the feeding situation. In Ambrose, A. (Ed.), Stimulation in early infancy (pp. 133–170). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Bowlby, J. (1991). An ethological approach to personality development. American Psychologist, 46, 333–341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Wittig, B. A. (1969). Attachment and the exploratory behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation. In Foss, B. M. (Ed.), Determinants of infant behavior (Vol. 4, pp. 113–136). London: Methuen.Google Scholar
,American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Bailey, D. B. J., Hatton, D. D., Mesibov, G., Ament, N., & Skinner, M. (2000). Early development, temperament, and functional impairment in autism and fragile X syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 49–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barber, B. K., Olsen, J. E., & Shagle, S. C. (1994). Associations between parental psychological and behavioral control and youth internalized and externalized behaviors. Child Development, 65, 1120–1136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, J. E. (1986). The measurement of temperament. In Plomin, R. & Dunn, J. (Eds.), The study of temperament: Changes, continuities and challenges (pp. 1–11). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bates, J., Maslin, C., & Frankel, K. (1985). Attachment security, mother-child interaction, and temperament as predictors of behavior problem ratings at age three years. In Bretherton, I. & Waters, E. (Eds.), Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50, 167–193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bauman, M. L., & Kemper, T. L. (2003). The neuropathology of the autism spectrum disorders: What have we learned?Novartis Foundation Symposium, 251, 112–22; discussion 122–8, 281–297.Google ScholarPubMed
Beaton, E. A., Schmidt, L. A., Ashbaugh, A. R., Santesso, D. L., Antony, M. M., & McCabe, R. E. (2008). Resting and reactive frontal brain electrical activity (EEG) among a non-clinical sample of socially anxious adults: Does concurrent depressive mood matter?Neuropsychiatric Disease & Treatment, 4(1), 187–192.Google ScholarPubMed
Bell, S. M., & Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1972). Infant crying and maternal responsiveness. Child Development, 43, 1171–1190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belmonte, M. K., Cook, E. H. J., Anderson, G. M., Rubenstein, J. L., Greenough, W. T., Beckel-Mitchener, A., et al. (2004). Autism as a disorder of neural information processing: Directions for research and targets for therapy. Molecular Psychiatry, 9, 646–663.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., & Isabella, R. (1988). Maternal, infant and social-contextual determinants of attachment security: A process analysis. In Belsky, J. & Nezworski, T. (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 41–94). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bolger, K. E., Patterson, C. J., & Kupersmidt, J. B. (1998). Peer relations and self-esteem among children who have been maltreated. Child Development, 69, 1171–1197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowlby, J. (1958). The nature of the child's tie to his mother. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 39, 350–373.Google ScholarPubMed
Bowbly, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bryson, S. E., Zwaigenbaum, L., McDermott, C., Rombough, V., & Brian, J. (2007). The autism observation scale for infants: Scale development and reliability data. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 12–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buitelaar, J. K. (1995). Attachment and social withdrawal in autism: Hypotheses and findings. Behavior, 132, 319–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing personality traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1986). The EAS approach to temperament. In Plomin, R., & Dunn, J. (Eds.), The study of temperament: Changes, continuities and challenges (pp. 68–80). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Caldji, C., Tannenbaum, B., Sharma, S., Francis, D., Plotsky, P. M., & Meaney, M. J. (1998). Maternal care during infancy regulates the development of neural systems mediating the expression of fearfulness in the rat. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95, 5335–5340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calkins, S. D., Fox, N. A., & Marshall, T. R. (1996). Behavioral and physiological andtecedents of inhibited and uninhibited behavior. Child Development, 67, 523–540.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Capps, L., Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1994). Attachment security in children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 249–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, V., Cicchetti, D., Barnett, D., & Braunwald, K. (1989). Disorganized, disoriented attachment relationships in maltreated infants. Development Psychology, 25, 525–531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caspi, A., & Silva, P. A. (1995). Temperamental qualities at age three predict personality traits in young adulthood: Longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. Child Development, 66, 486–498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., et al. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297, 851–854.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Craig, I. W., Harrington, H. L., et al. (2003). Influence of life stress on depression: Moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science, 301, 386–389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cassidy, J., & Berlin, L. J. (1994). The insecure/ambivalent pattern of attachment: Theory and research. Child Development, 65, 971–991.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chess, S., & Thomas, A. (1984). Origins and evolution of behavior disorders. New York: Brunner/Mazel, Inc.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (1995). A developmental psychopathology perspective on child abuse and neglect. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 541–565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohn, J. F., Campbell, S. B., Matias, R., & Hopkins, J. (1990). Face-to-face interactions of postpartum depressed and nondepressed mother-infant pairs at 2 months. Developmental Psychology, 26, 13–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colin, V. L. (1996). Human attachment. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Coplan, J. D., Andrews, M. W., Rosenblum, L. A., Owens, M. J., Friedman, S., Gorman, J. M., et al. (1996). Persistent elevations of cerebrospinal fluid concentrations of corticotrophin-releasing factor in adult nonhuman primates exposed to early-life stressors: Implications for the pathophysiology of mood and anxiety disorders. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93, 1619–1623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Courchesne, E., Carper, R., & Akshoomoff, N. (2003). Evidence of brain overgrowth in the first year of life in autism. Journal of the American Medical Association 290, 337–344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Courchesne, E., Karns, C. M., Davis, H. R., Ziccardi, R., Carper, R. A., Tigue, Z. D., et al. (2001) Unusual brain growth patterns in early life in patients with autistic disorder: An MRI study. Neurology 57, 245–254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dahlgren, S. O., & Gillberg, C. (1989). Symptoms in the first two years of life. A preliminary population study of infantile autism. European Archives of Psychiatry and Neurological Sciences 238, 169–174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giacomo, A., & Fombonne, E. (1998). Parental recognition of developmental abnormalities in autism. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 7, 131–136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dissanayake, C., & Crossley, S. A. (1996). Proximity and sociable behaviors in autism: Evidence for attachment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 149–156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dubner, A. E., & Motta, R. W. (1999). Sexually and physically abused foster care children and posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 67, 367–373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egeland, B., & Sroufe, L. A. (1981). Developmental sequelae of maltreatment in infancy. In Cicchetti, D. & Rizle, R. (Eds.), Developmental approaches to child maltreatment: New directions for child development (pp. 77–91). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Eley, T. C., Sugden, K., Corsico, A., Gregory, A. M., Sham, P., McGuffin, P., et al. (2004). Gene-environment interaction analysis of serotonin system markers with adolescent depression. Molecular Psychiatry, 9, 908–915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fleming, A. S., O'Day, D. H., & Kraemer, G. W. (1999). Neurobiology of mother-infant interactions: Experience and central nervous system plasticity across development and generations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 23, 673–685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fombonne, E. (2005). Epidemiology of autistic disorder and other pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 66(Suppl. 10), 3–8.Google ScholarPubMed
Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Rubin, K. H., Calkins, S. D., & Schmidt, L. A. (2001). Continuity and discontinuity of behavioral inhibition and exuberance: psychophysiological and behavioral influences across the first four years of life. Child Development, 72, 1–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, N. A., Nichols, K. E., Henderson, H. A., Rubin, K. H., Schmidt, L. A., Hamer, D., et al. (2005). Evidence for a gene-environment interaction in predicting behavioral inhibition in middle childhood. Psychological Science, 16, 921–926.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, N. A., Rubin, K. H., Calkins, S. D., Marshall, T. R., Coplan, R. J., Porges, S. W., et al. (1995). Frontal activation asymmetry and social competence at four years of age. Child Development, 66, 1770–1784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Francis, D., Diorio, J., Liu, D., & Meaney, M. J. (1999). Nongenomic transmission across generations of maternal behavior and stress responses in the rat. Science, 5, 1155–1158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia-Coll, C., Kagan, J., & Reznick, S. (1984). Behavioral inhibition in young children. Child Development, 55, 1005–1019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., Zwaigenbaum, L., Smith, I. M., Brian, J., Roberts, W., & Szatmari, P. (2009). Temperament and its relationship to autistic symptoms in a high-risk infant sib cohort. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 59–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghera, M. M., Hane, A. A., Malesa, E. E., & Fox, N. A. (2006). The role of infant soothability in the relation between infant negativity and maternal sensitivity. Infant Behavior and Development, 29, 289–293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, S. (1991). Recent development in attachment theory and research. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 36, 393–400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldsmith, H. H., Buss, A. H., Plomin, R., Rothbart, M. K., Thomas, A., Chess, S., et al. (1987). Roundtable: What is temperament? Four approaches. Child Development, 58, 505–529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldsmith, H. H., & Campos, J. J. (1982). Toward a theory of infant temperament. In Emde, R. N. & Harmon, R. J. (Eds.), The development of attachment and affiliative systems. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Goodman-Campbell, S. B. (1979). Mother-infant interaction as a function of maternal ratings of temperament. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 10, 67–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunnar, M., & Quevedo, K. (2007). The neurobiology of stress and development. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 145–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harlow, H. F., & Zimmerman, R. R. (1959). Affectional responses in the infant monkey. Science, 130, 421–432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hetherington, M. E., Parke, R. D., & Schmuckler, M. (2003). Child psychology: A contemporary viewpoint (1st Canadian ed.). Toronto: McGraw-Hill Ryerson.Google Scholar
Hoshino, Y., Kaneko, M., Yashima, Y., Kumashiro, H., Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (1987). Clinical features of autistic children with setback course in their infancy. Japanese Journal of Psychiatry and Neurology 41, 237–245.Google ScholarPubMed
Isabella, R. A., & Belsky, J. (1991). Interactional synchrony and the origins of infant-mother attachments: A replication study. Child Development, 62, 373–384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M. H., & Munakata, Y. (2005). Processes of change in brain and cognitive development. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 152–158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J. (1994). Galen's prophecy: Temperament in human nature. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Kagan, J., Arcus, D., Snidman, N., Feng, W.Y., Hendler, J., & Greene, S. (1994). Reactivity in infants: A cross-national comparison. Developmental Psychology, 30, 342–345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J., Kearsley, R. B., & Zelazo, P. R. (1978). Infancy: Its place in human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kagan, J., Reznick, S. J., Clarke, C., Snidman, N., & Garcia-Coll, C. (1984). Behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar. Child Development, 55, 2212–2225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Snidman, N. (1987). The physiology and psychology of behavioral inhibition in children. Child Development, 58, 1459–1473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J., Reznick, S. J., & Snidman, N. (1988). Biological bases of childhood shyness. Science, 240, 167–171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J., Reznick, S. J., Snidman, N., Gibbons, J., Johnson, M. O. (1988). Childhood derivaties of inhibition and lack of inhibition to the unfamiliar. Child Development, 59, 1580–1589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J., & Snidman, N. (1991). Infant predictors of inhibited and uninhibited profiles. Psychological Science, 2, 40–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kasari, C., & Sigman, M. (1997). Linking parental perceptions to interactions in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 39–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kasari, C., Sigman, M., Mundy, P., & Yirmiya, N. (1988). Caregiver interactions with autistic children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 45–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, J. (1991). Depressive disorders in maltreated children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 257–265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, S. E., & Kagan, J. (2005). Infant predictors of kindergarten behavior: The contribution of inhibited and uninhibited temperament styles. Behavioral Disorders, 30, 331–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, J., Yang, B. Z., Douglas-Palumberi, H., Grasso, D., Lipschitz, D., Houshyar, S., et al. (2006). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor-5-HTTLPR gene interactions and environmental modifiers of depression in children. Biological Psychiatry, 59, 673–680.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, J., Yang, B. Z., Douglas-Palumberi, H., Houshyar, S., Lipschitz, D., Krystal, J. H., et al. (2004). Social supports and serotonin transporter gene moderate depression in maltreated children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 1010, 17316–17321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Kuhn, J. W., Vittum, J., Prescott, C. A., & Riley, B. (2005). The interaction of stressful life events and a serotonin transporter polymorphism in the prediction of episodes of major depression: A replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 529–535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kochanska, G. (1991). Patterns of inhibition to the unfamiliar in children of normal and affectively ill mothers. Child Development, 62, 250–263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lemanek, K. L., Stone, W. L., & Fishel, P. T. (1993). Parent-child interactions in handicapped preschoolers: The relation between parent behaviors and compliance. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 22, 68–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, D., Diorio, J., Tannenbaum, B., Caldji, C., Francis, D., Freedman, A., et al. (1997). Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. Science, 277(5332), 1659–1662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorenz, K. Z. (1935). Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels [The companion in the bird's world]. Journal fuer Ornithologie, 83, 137–213. (Abbreviated English translation published 1937 Auk, 54, 245–273).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth strange situation. In Greenberg, M., Cicchetti, D., & Cummings, E. M. (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 121–160). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Matheny, A. (1989). Children's behavioral inhibition over age and across situations: Genetic similarity for a trait during change. Journal of Personality, 57, 215–235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matheny, A. P., Wilson, R. S., & Nuss, S. M. (1984). Toddler temperament: Stability across setting and over ages. Child Development, 55, 1200–1211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDevitt, S. C., & Carey, W. B. (1978). The measurement of temperament in 3–7 year old children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 19, 245–253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meaney, M. J. (2001). Maternal care, gene expression, and the transmission of individual differences in stress reactivity across generations. Annual Review in Neuroscience, 24, 1161–1192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mills, R. S. L., & Rubin, K. H. (1998). Are behavioral and psychological control both differentially associated with childhood aggression and social withdrawal?Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 30, 132–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moss, E., Rousseau, D., Parent, S., St-Laurent, D., & Saintonge, J. (1998). Correlates of attachment at school age: Maternal reported stress, mother-child interaction, and behavior problems. Child Development, 69, 1390–1405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nachmias, M., Gunnar, M., Mangelsdorf, S., Parritz, R. H., & Buss, K. (1996). Behavioral inhibition and stress reactivity: The moderating role of attachment. Child Development, 67, 508–522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 24, 247–257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, S. Y., Belsky, J., Putnam, S., & Crnic, K. (1997). Infant emotionality, parenting, and 3-year inhibition: Exploring stability and lawful discontinuity in a male sample. Developmental Psychology, 33, 218–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pedlow, R., Sanson, A., Prior, M., & Oberklaid, F. (1993). Stability of maternally reported temperament from infancy to 8 years. Developmental Psychology, 29, 998–1007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pynoos, R. S., & Nader, K. (1993). Issues in the treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder in children and adolescents. In Wilson, J. & Raphael, B. (Eds.), International Handbook of Traumatic Stress Syndromes (pp. 535–549). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reznick, S. J., Kagan, J., Snidman, N., Gersten, M., Baak, K., & Rosenberg, A. (1986). Inhibited and uninhibited children: A follow-up study. Child Development, 57, 660–680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, J. L., Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Corley, R. (1992). The heritability of inhibited and uninhibited behavior: A twin study. Developmental Psychology, 28, 1030–1037.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodier, P. M. (2002). Converging evidence for brain stem injury in autism. Development and Psychopatholog,y 14, 537–557.Google ScholarPubMed
Rogers, S. J., Ozonoff, S., & Maslin-Cole, C. (1993). Developmental aspects of attachment behavior in young children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 1274–1282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenblum, L. A., & Andrews, M. W. (1994). Influences of environmental demand on maternal behavior and infant development. Acta Paediatrica Supplement, 397, 57–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child Development, 52, 569–578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothbart, M. K. (1986). Longitudinal observation of infant temperament. Developmental Psychology, 22, 356–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In Damon, W., Lerner, R.. & Eisenberg, N. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 99–166). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rubin, K. H., Burgess, K. B., & Hastings, P. D. (2002). Stability and social-behavioral consequences of toddlers' inhibited temperament and parenting behaviors. Child Development, 73, 483–495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, K. H., Hastings, P. D., Stewart, S. L., Henderson, H. A., & Chen, X. (1997). The consistency and concomitants of inhibition: Some of the children, all of the time. Child Development, 68, 467–483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, K. H., Nelson, L. J., Hastings, P., & Asendorpf, J. (1999). Transaction between parents' perceptions of their children's shyness and their parenting styles. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 23, 937–957.CrossRef
Rutgers, A. H., Ijzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Swinkels, S. H., Daalen, E., Dietz, C., et al. (2007). Autism, attachment and parenting: A comparison of children with autism spectrum disorder, mental retardation, language disorder, and non-clinical children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 859–870.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanchez, M. M., Ladd, C. O., & Plotsky, P. M. (2001). Early adverse experience as a developmental risk factor for later psychopathology: Evidence from rodent and primate models. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 419–450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sapolsky, R. M. (2004). Mothering style and methylation. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 791–792.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, L. A. & Fox, N.A. (1994). Patterns of cortical electrophysiology and autonomic activity in adults' shyness and sociability. Biological Psychology, 38, 183–198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., & Hamer, D. H. (2007). Evidence for a gene-gene interaction in predicting children's behavior problems: Association of serotonin transporter short and dopamine receptor D4 long genotypes with internalizing and externalizing behaviors in typically developing 7-year-olds. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 1105–1116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., Perez-Edgar, K., & Hamer, D. H. (2009). Linking gene, brain, and behavior: DRD4, frontal asymmetry, and temperament. Psychological Science, 20, 831–837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., Rubin, K. H., Sternberg, E. M., Gold, P. W., Smith, C. C., & et al. (1997). Behavioral and neuroendocrine responses in shy children. Developmental Psychobiology, 30, 127–140.3.0.CO;2-S>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., Schulkin, J., & Gold, P. W. (1999). Behavioral and psychophysiological correlates of self-presentation in temperamentally shy children. Developmental Psychobiology, 35, 119–135.3.0.CO;2-G>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, C. B., Henderson, H. A., Inge, A. P., Zahka, N. E., Coman, D. C., Kojkowski, N. M., et al. (2009). Temperament as a predictor of symptomotology and adaptive functioning in adolescents with high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 842–855.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, C. E., Snidman, N., & Kagan, J. (1999). Adolescent social anxiety as an outcome of inhibited temperament in childhood. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 1008–1015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sigman, M., Mundy, P., Sherman, T., & Ungerer, J. (1986). Social interactions of autistic, mentally retarded and normal children and their caregivers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 647–655.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Speltz, M. L. (1990). The treatment of preschool conduct problems: An integration of behavioral and attachment concepts. In Greenberg, M., Cicchetti, D., & Cummings, E. M. (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research and intervention (pp. 399–426). Chicago, IL: University Press.Google Scholar
Theall-Honey, L. A., & Schmidt, L. A. (2006). Do temperamentally shy children process emotion differently than non-shy children? Behavioral, psychophysiological, and gender differences in reticent preschoolers. Developmental Psychobiology, 48, 187–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New York: Brunner/Mazel.Google Scholar
Thomas, A., Chess, S., Birch, H. G., Hertzig, M. E., & Korn, S. (1963). Behavioral inhibition in early childhood. New York: New York University Press.Google Scholar
Ijzendoorn, M. H., Rutgers, A. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Swinkels, S. H., Daalen, E., Dietz, C., et al. (2007). Parental sensitivity and attachment in children with autism spectrum disorder: comparison with children with mental retardation, with language delays, and with typical development. Child Development, 78, 597–608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weaver, I. C. G., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F. A., D'Alessio, A. C., Sharma, S., Seckl, J. R., et al. (2004). Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 847–854.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weaver, I. C., Plante, P., Weaver, S., Parent, A., Sharma, S., Diorio, J., et al. (2001). Early environmental regulation of hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor gene expression: Characterization of intracellular mediators and potential genomic target sites. Molecular and Cellular Endocrinology, 185, 205–218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinberg, M. K., & Tronick, E. Z. (1998a). Emotional characteristics of infants associated with maternal depression and anxiety. Pediatrics, 102, 1298–1304.Google ScholarPubMed
Weinberg, M. K., & Tronick, E. Z. (1998b). The impact of maternal psychiatric illness on infant development. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59, 53–61.Google ScholarPubMed
Worobey, J., & Blajda, V. M. (1989). Temperament ratings at 2 weeks, 2 months, and 1 year: Differential stability of activity and emotionality. Developmental Psychology, 25, 257–263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×