Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T09:27:23.683Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Secret Language Use at Female Initiation: Bounding Gossiping Communities

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 November 2009

Chris Knight
Affiliation:
University of East London
Michael Studdert-Kennedy
Affiliation:
Haskins Laboratories
James Hurford
Affiliation:
University of Edinburgh
Get access

Summary

The idea that ‘gossip’ or vocal exchange of social information was a vital mechanism for bonding early human groups appears plausible and concretely testable (Dunbar 1996, 1998; Dunbar, Duncan and Nettle 1995). The relatively rapid encephalisation seen in archaic-grade Homo sapiens is presumed to reflect the increasing size and complexity of these hominids' social groups (Aiello and Dunbar 1993). Vocal grooming in the first place and ultimately gossip offered alternative mechanisms for servicing such extensive social networks, because they saved valuable time compared with the traditional primate means of manual grooming.

This ‘time-saving’ argument leads to a serious problem for the gossip hypothesis of language origins, however. As our ancestors maximised brain size in response to the pressure for larger groups, they maximised their ‘Machiavellian intelligence’ (Byrne and Whiten 1988; Whiten and Byrne 1997). Humans appear to be selected for a capacity involving both social cooperation and alliance formation, but also manipulation and exploitation of their relationships. We cannot consider gossip as a mechanism of social bonding without factoring in this Machiavellian aspect of manipulating information for selfish purposes (cf. Kemmerer 1997). In the case of primate grooming, time becomes a currency (Byrne 1995: 200–202). The time an individual spends grooming an ally reliably quantifies its commitment to their relationship. Correspondingly, if vocal grooming and gossip mechanisms led to a reduction in time spent grooming per individual groomed, this implies a reduction in the level of commitment signalled to each individual (not necessarily equally distributed).

Type
Chapter
Information
The Evolutionary Emergence of Language
Social Function and the Origins of Linguistic Form
, pp. 81 - 98
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2000

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×