Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T02:01:41.790Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - Reading and the brain: A cross-language approach

from Part III - Brain, language, and mathematics

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Usha Goswami
Affiliation:
Faculty of Education University of Cambridge
Antonio M. Battro
Affiliation:
National Academy of Education, Argentina
Kurt W. Fischer
Affiliation:
Harvard University, Massachusetts
Pierre J. Léna
Affiliation:
Université de Paris VII (Denis Diderot)
Get access

Summary

Overview

The learning of reading skills is causally related to some properties of the language itself. Transparent or consistent alphabetic languages, such as Spanish, map most easily from orthography to sounds, with one-to-one correspondence from letters to phonemes. Inconsistent orthographies, such as English, show instead a greater mismatch between orthography and phonology. A pre-reading child detects small units within syllables in his spoken vocabulary, units comprised of onset and rime. Analyzing speech into these components is central to phonological awareness, the ability to analyze speech into its component sounds. Reading in an alphabetic language requires relating letters to sound units that are smaller than onset and rime, units called phonemes. Goswami argues that children learn phonemes through mastering the orthography (letters in combination) of their language. To master the code relating written letters to spoken sounds, children need more time for languages with inconsistent orthography, even when they do not have dyslexia. Dyslexic children of all languages have greater trouble learning letter-sound relationships, typically because of poor representation of sounds, which can be investigated in both behavior and brain imaging. Electrical potentials from cortical activity evoked by sounds and other stimuli indicate that the auditory systems of dyslexic children seem to be more immature than those of normal children. This kind of research combining both behavior and brain activity to analyze reading is one of the areas that is most likely to produce important knowledge relevant for schooling in the near future.

The Editors

Type
Chapter
Information
The Educated Brain
Essays in Neuroeducation
, pp. 198 - 212
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baldeweg, T., Richardson, A., Watkins, S., Foale, C., and Gruzelier, J. (1999). Ann. Neurology, 45 (4), 495–503.3.0.CO;2-M>CrossRef
Bradley, L. and Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorising sounds and learning to read: A causal connection. Nature, 310, 419–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bregman, A. S. (1993). Auditory scene analysis: hearing in complex environments. In McAdams, S. and Bigand, E. (eds.), Thinking in Sound: The Cognitive Psychology of Human Audition, (pp. 10–36). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, G. D. A. and Ellis, N. C. (1994). Issues in spelling research. In Brown, G. D. A. and Ellis, N. C. (eds.), Handbook of Spelling: Theory, Process and Intervention (pp. 3–25). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bruck, M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexics' phonological awareness deficits. Developmental Psychology, 28, 874–886.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruneau, N., Roux, S., Guérin, P., Barthélémy, C., and Lelord, G. (1997). Temporal prominence of auditory evoked potentials (N1 wave) in 4- 8-year-old children. Psychophysiology, 34, 32–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cossu, G., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I. Y., Katz, L., and Tola, G. (1988). Awareness of phonological segments and reading ability in Italian children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goswami, U. (2000a). Phonological representations, reading development and dyslexia: Towards a cross-linguistic theoretical framework. Dyslexia, 6, 133–151.3.0.CO;2-A>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goswami, U.(2000b). The potential of a neuro-constructivist framework for developmental dyslexia: The abnormal development of phonological representations? Developmental Science, 3, 27–29.Google Scholar
Goswami, U.(2002). In the beginning was the rhyme? A reflection on Hulme, Hatcher, Nation, Brown, Adams & Stuart, 2002. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 47–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goswami, U.(2003a). Phonology, learning to read and dyslexia: A cross-linguistic analysis. In Csepe, V. (ed.), Dyslexia: Different Brain, Different Behaviour, (pp. 1–40). NL: Kluwer Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goswami, U.(2003b). Why theories about developmental dyslexia require developmental designs. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 534–540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goswami, U., Thomson, J., Richardson, U., Stainthorp, R., Hughes, D., Rosen, S., and Scott, S. K. (2002). Amplitude envelope onsets and developmental dyslexia: A new hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99 (16), 10911–10916.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goswami, U., Ziegler, J., Dalton, L., and Schneider, W. (2003). Nonword reading across orthographies: How flexible is the choice of reading units? Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 235–247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoien, T., Lundberg, L., Stanovich, K. E., and Bjaalid, I. K. (1995). Components of phonological awareness. Reading & Writing, 7, 171–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., Fischer, F. W., and Carter, B. (1974). Explicit syllable and phoneme segmentation in the young child. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, 201–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McArthur, G. M. and Bishop, D. V. M. (2001). Auditory perceptual processing in people with reading and oral language impairments: Current issues and recommendations. Dyslexia, 7, 150–170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porpodas, C. D. (1999). Patterns of phonological and memory processing in beginning readers and spellers of Greek. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 406–416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rack, J. P., Snowling, M. J., and Olson, R. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 29–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siok, W. T. and Fletcher, P. (2001). The role of phonological awareness and visual-orthographic skills in Chinese reading acquisition. Developmental Psychology, 37, 886–899.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snowling, M. J. (2000). Dyslexia. 2nd edition. Oxford: Blackwells.Google ScholarPubMed
Tallal, P. (1980). Auditory temporal perception, phonics and reading disabilities in children. Brain & Language, 9, 182–198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomson, J., Baldeweg, T., and Goswami, U. (2004). Amplitude envelope onsets and dyslexia: a behavioural and electrophysiological study. Poster presented at the Annual Conference of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, Amsterdam, June 2004.
Wimmer, H. (1993). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 1–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wimmer, H.(1996). The early manifestation of developmental dyslexia: Evidence from German children. Reading & Writing, 8, 171–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, B. A., Bowen, R. W., and Zecker, S. G. (2000). Nonlinguistic perceptual deficits associated with reading and language disorders. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 10, 482–486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×