Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-gtxcr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T20:35:16.576Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Building bridges in neuroeducation

from Part I - The mind, brain, and education triad

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

John T. Bruer
Affiliation:
The James McDonnell Foundation St. Louis
Antonio M. Battro
Affiliation:
National Academy of Education, Argentina
Kurt W. Fischer
Affiliation:
Harvard University, Massachusetts
Pierre J. Léna
Affiliation:
Université de Paris VII (Denis Diderot)
Get access

Summary

Overview

Mind, brain, and education initiatives should build bridges between educators and behavioral, cognitive, and neurobiological scientists. Some bridges are robust, and others are problematic. Links of cognitive development to education can be straightforward and useful. For example, children with low socioeconomic status typically show delays in the normal acquisition of arithmetic skills and concepts. When these children have access to intensive training, such as the program “Right Start,” they overcome the obstacles and improve their level of performance. Some other bridges are not so well founded. In particular, the over-emphasis on sensitive periods for learning connected with brain maturation has led to a restrictive concept of “windows of opportunity” for learning, which is not supported by research on learning. In fact, some research invalidates the common view that high synaptic density is needed for learning. The link from neuroscience to education needs to include assessment of the target behaviors, such as learning arithmetic and reading, and not assume that brain findings link in obvious ways. Other chapters in this book highlight areas where links between brain research and educationally relevant behaviors are being made fruitfully and with appropriate scientific caution, especially for language and arithmetic.

The Editors

In Education and the Brain: A Bridge Too Far (Bruer, 1997) I expressed concerns about supposed implications of developmental neuroscience for teaching and learning. I also argued positively that currently cognitive psychology is a better source for educationally relevant basic research than is developmental neuroscience.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Educated Brain
Essays in Neuroeducation
, pp. 43 - 58
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bachevalier, J. and Mishkin, M. (1984). An early and a late developing system for learning and retention in infant monkeys. Behavioral Neuroscience, 98(5), 770–778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bailey, D. B., Bruer, J. T., Symons, F. J., and Lichtman, J. W. (2001). Critical Thinking about Critical Periods. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Bruer, J. T. (1994). Schools for Thought: A Science of Learning in the Classroom. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bruer, J. T.(1997). Education and the Brain: A Bridge Too Far. Educational Researcher, 26(8), 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruer, J. T.(1999). The Myth of the First Three Years: A New Understanding of Early Brain Development and Life Long Learning. New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Bruer, J. T.(2002). Avoiding the pediatrician's error: how neuroscientists can help educators (and themselves). Nature Neuroscience, Nov. 5, Suppl., 1031–1033.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chugani, H. T., Phelps, M. E., and Mazziota, J. C. (1987). Positron emission tomography study of human brain function development. Annals of Neurology, 22, 487–497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chugani, H. (1998). A critical period of brain development: studies of cerebral glucose utilization with PET. Preventive Medicine 27, 184–188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cynader, M. (2000). Perspectives: neuroscience. Strengthening visual connections. Science, 287(5460), 1943–1944.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 44(1–2), 1–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S.(1996). The organization of brain activations in number comparison: event related potentials and the additive-factors method. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 47–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiez, J. A., Balota, D. A., Raichle, M. E., and Petersen, S. E. (1999). Effects of lexicality, frequency, and spelling-to-consistency on the functional anatomy of reading. Neuron, 25, 205–218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., Paus, T., Evans, A. C., and Rapoport, J. L. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience, 2(10), 861–863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenough, W., Black, J. and Wallace, C. (1987). Experience and brain development. Child Development, 58(3), 539–559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffin, S. A., Case, R., and Siegler, R. S. (1994). Rightstart: Providing the central conceptual prerequisites for first formal learning of arithmetic to students at risk for school failure, In McGilly, K. (ed.). Classroom Lessons, (pp. 25–50). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, P. R. (1979). Synaptic density in human frontal cortex – developmental changes of ageing. Brain Research, 163, 195–209.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, P. R.(2003). Basic neuroscience research has important implications for child developmen. Nature Neuroscience, 6(6), 541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kandell, E. and Schwartz, J. (1991). Principles of Neural Science. New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Lund, J. S., Boothe, R. G., and Lund, R. D. (1977). Development of neurons in the visual cortex (area 17) of the monkey (Macaca nemestrina): A Golgi study from fetal day 127 to postnatal maturity. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 176, 149–188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Means, M. and Voss, J. (1985). Star Wars: A developmental study of expert and novice knowledge structure. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 746–757.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nathan, M. J. and Koedinger, K. R. (2000). Teachers' and researchers' beliefs about the development of algebraic reasoning. Journal of Research on Mathematics Education, 31(2), 168–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
NIH Publication No. 01-4929 NIMH Press release Child and adolescent mental health information at www.nimh.nih.gov/publicat/childmenu.cfm.
Overman, W. H., Bachevalier, J., Schuhmann, E., and Ryan, P. (1996a). Cognitive gender differences in very young children parallel biologically based cognitive differences in monkeys. Behavioral Neuroscience, 110(4), 673–684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Overman, W. H., Bachevalier, J., Miller, M., and Moore, K. (1996b). Children's performance on “animal tests” of oddity: Implications for cognitive processes required for tests of oddity and delayed nonmatch to sample. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 62(2), 223–242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Overman, W. H., Pate, B. J., Moore, K., and Peuster, A. (1996c). Ontogeny of place learning in children measured in the radial arm maze, Morris search task, and open field task. Behavioral Neuroscience, 110(6), 1205–1228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Overman, W. H. (1990). Performance on traditional matching to sample, non-matching to sample, and object discrimination tasks by 12- to 32-month-old children. A developmental progression. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 608, 365–385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinel, P., Dehaene, S., Riviere, D., and LeBihan, D. (2001). Modulations of parietal activation by semantic distance in a number comparison task. Neuroimage, 14(5), 1013–1026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pressley, M. and McCormick, C. B. (1995). Advanced Educational Psychology for Educators, Researchers, and Policymakers. New York: Harper Collins College Publishers.Google Scholar
Rakic, P., Bourgeois, J. P., Eckenhoff, M. F., Zecevic, N., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1986). Concurrent overproduction of synapses in diverse regions of the primate cerebral cortex. Science, 232(4747), 232–235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramus, F. (2001). Outstanding questions about phonological processing in dyslexia. Dyslexia, 7(4), 197–216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Resnick, L. B. (1982). Syntax and semantics in learning to subtract. In Carpenter, T. P., Moser, J. M., and Romberg, T. A. (eds). Addition and Subtraction: A Cognitive Perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×