Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-42gr6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T17:05:29.626Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - Introduction

Developmental Dyslexia – A Cross-Linguistic Perspective

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 September 2019

Ludo Verhoeven
Affiliation:
Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen
Charles Perfetti
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
Kenneth Pugh
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

Reading involves decoding written language in order to understand it. In learning to read, children implicitly learn how their writing system encodes their spoken language and how they can decode printed words into spoken words to derive meaning (see Verhoeven & Perfetti, 2017). However, many children around the world encounter problems learning to read, fail to develop fluent decoding, and are thus diagnosed as dyslexic.

A large body of research supports the conclusion that a phonological deficit underlies most developmental dyslexia. Much of the existing evidence, however, is based on studies of children learning to read in English. It is important to note that English has an opaque orthography that creates challenges beyond those facing children who read more transparent orthographies. In recent years, the research base for developmental dyslexia has broadened across languages, allowing the question of differences and similarities across languages and writing systems to receive attention.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Balota, D. A., FloresD’Arcais, G. B., & Rayner, K. (1990). Comprehension processes in reading. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different? Psychological Bulletin, 130, 858888.Google Scholar
Blachman, B. A. (2000). Phonological awareness. In Kamil, M. L., Mosenthal, P. B., Pearson, P. D., & Barr, R. (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. III; pp. 483502). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Blomert, L. (2011). The neural signature of orthographic-phonological binding in successful and failing reading development. Neuroimage, 57, 695703.Google Scholar
Boets, B., Op de Beeck, H. P., & Vandermosten, M. et al. (2013). Intact but less accessible phonetic representations in adults with dyslexia. Science, 342, 12511254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosse, M., & Valdois, S. (2009). Influence of the visual attention span on child reading performance: A cross-sectional study. Journal of Research in Reading, 32, 230253.Google Scholar
Bowers, P. N., Kirby, J. R., & Deacon, S. H. (2010). The effects of morphological instruction on literacy skills: A systematic review of the literature. Review of Educational Research, 80, 144179.Google Scholar
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read – a causal connection. Nature, 301, 419421.Google Scholar
Bus, A. G., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1999). Phonological awareness and early reading: A meta-analysis of experimental training studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 403414.Google Scholar
Byrne, B., Coventry, W. L., Olson, R. K. et al. (2008). A behavioral-genetic analysis of orthographic learning, spelling, and decoding. Journal of Research in Reading, 3, 821.Google Scholar
Caravolas, M., Lervåg, A., Defior, S., Málková, G. S., & Hulme, C. (2013). Different patterns, but equivalent predictors, of growth in reading in consistent and inconsistent orthographies. Psychological Science, 24, 13981407.Google Scholar
Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2009). Ventral and dorsal contribution to word reading. In Gazzaniga, M. S. (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience (pp. 789804). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204256.Google Scholar
Daniels, P. T., & Share, D. L. (2018). Writing system variation and its consequences for reading and dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 22, 101116.Google Scholar
Das, T., Padakannaya, P., Pugh, K. R., & Singh, N. C. (2011). Neuroimaging reveals dual routes to reading in simultaneous proficient readers of two orthographies. NeuroImage, 54, 14761487.Google Scholar
de Graaff, S., Bosman, A. M. T., Hasselman, F., & Verhoeven, L. (2009). Benefits of systematic phonics instruction. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 318333.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain. London: Penguin Viking.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (2011). The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 254262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dufor, O., Serniclaes, W., Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Démonet, J.-F. (2009). Left pre-motor cortex and allophonic speech perception in dyslexia: A PET study, NeuroImage, 46, 241248.Google Scholar
Eden, G., & Moats, L. (2002). The role of neuroscience in the remediation of students with dyslexia. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 10801084.Google Scholar
Ehri, L., Nunes, R. S., Willows, D., Schuster, B. V., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., & Shanahan, T. (2001). Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read: Evidence from the National Reading Panel’s meta-analysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 250287.Google Scholar
Elbro, C., & Scarborough, H. S. (2004). Early intervention. In Bryant, P. & Nunes, T., (Eds.), Handbook of children’s literacy (pp. 361381). Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Elliott, J. G., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2014). The dyslexia debate. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Frost, R. (2012). Towards a universal model of reading. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(5), 263279. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11001841.Google Scholar
Frost, R., Katz, L., & Bentin, S. (1987). Strategies for visual word recognition and orthographic depth: A multilingual comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 104115.Google Scholar
Furnes, B., & Samuelsson, S. (2011). Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming predicting early development in reading and spelling results from a cross-linguistic longitudinal study. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 8595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galuschka, K., Ise, E., Krick, K., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2014). Effectiveness of treatment approaches for children and adolescents with reading disabilities: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. PLoS One, 9(2), e89900.Google Scholar
Goswami, U. (2000). Phonological and lexical processes. In Kamil, M. L., Rosenthal, P. B., Pearson, P. D., & Barr, R. (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. III; pp. 251268). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goswami, U. (2001). Early phonological development and the acquisition of literacy. In Neuman, S. & Dickinson, D. (Eds.), Handbook of research in early literacy for the 21st century (pp. 111125). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Goswami, U., Wang, H. L., & Cruz, A. et al. (2011). Language-universal sensory deficits in developmental dyslexia: English, Spanish, and Chinese. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 325337.Google Scholar
Hagoort, P. (2005). On Broca, brain, and binding: A new framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 416423.Google Scholar
Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2004). Computing the meanings of words in reading: Cooperative division of labor between visual and phonological processes. Psychological Review, 111, 662720.Google Scholar
Høien, T., & Sundberg, P. (2000). Dyslexia: From theory to intervention. Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar
Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2, 127160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hu, W., Lee, H. L., & Zhang, Q. et al. (2010). Developmental dyslexia in Chinese and English populations: Dissociating the effect of dyslexia from language differences. Brain, 133, 16941706.Google Scholar
Jorm, A. F., & Share, D. L. (1983). Phonological recoding and reading acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 4, 103147.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1988). The use of knowledge in discourse processing: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95, 163182.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kirby, J. R., Deacon, S. H., & Bowers, P. N. et al. (2012). Children’s morphological awareness and reading ability. Reading and Writing, 25, 389410.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Andruski, J. E., & Chistovich, I. A. et al. (1997). Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. Science, 277, 684686.Google Scholar
Lallier, M., Molinaro, N., Lizarazu, M., Bourguignon, M., & Carreiras, M. (2017). Amodal atypical neural oscillatory activity in dyslexia: A cross-linguistic perspective. Clinical Psychological Science, 5, 379401.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. B. (2014). Specific language impairment across languages. Child Development Perspectives, 8, 15.Google Scholar
Liberman, A. M. (1992). The relation of speech to reading and writing. Advances in Psychology, 94, 167178.Google Scholar
Liberman, I. Y., & Shankweiler, D. (1985). Phonology and the problems of learning to read and write. Remedial and Special Education, 6(6), 817.Google Scholar
Marshall, C. M., Snowling, M. J., & Bailey, P. J. (2001). Rapid auditory processing and phonological ability in normal readers and readers with dyslexia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 925940.Google Scholar
McQueen, J. M., & Cutler, A. (1997). Cognitive processes in spoken-word recognition. In Hardcastle, W. J. & Laver, J. D. M. H. (Eds.), The handbook of phonetic sciences (pp. 566585). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Moll, K., Ramus, F., & Bartling, J. et al. (2014). Cognitive mechanisms underlying reading and spelling development in five European orthographies: Is English an outlier orthography? Learning and Instruction, 29, 6577.Google Scholar
National Reading Panel. (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel: Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction: Reports of the subgroups. Washington, DC: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health.Google Scholar
Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (2007). Procedural learning difficulties: Reuniting the developmental disorders? Trends in Neurosciences, 30, 135141.Google Scholar
Norton, E. S., Black, J. M., & Stanley, L. M. et al. (2014). Functional neuroanatomical evidence for the double-deficit hypothesis of developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 61, 235246.Google Scholar
Pennington, B. F. (2006). From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition, 101, 385413.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In Gough, P. B., Ehri, L. C., & Treiman, R. (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 145174). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A. (1997). The psycholinguistics of spelling and reading. In Perfetti, C. A., Rieben, L., & Fayol, M. (Eds.), Learning to spell: Research, theory, and practice across languages (pp. 2138). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A. (2003). The universal grammar of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 324.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 357383.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., & Harris, L. N. (2013). Universal reading processes are modulated by language and writing system. Language Learning and Development, 9(4), 296-316.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2001). The lexical quality hypothesis. In Verhoeven, L., Elbro, C., & Reitsma, P. (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 189214). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., Liu, Y., & Fiez, J. et al. (2007). Reading in two writing systems: Accommodation and assimilation of the brain’s reading network. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10, 131146.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., Liu, Y., & Tan, L. H. (2005). The Lexical Constituency Model: Some implications of research on Chinese for general theories of reading. Psychological Review, 12(11), 4359.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., Zhang, S., & Berent, I. (1992). Reading in English and Chinese: Evidence for a “universal” phonological principle. In Frost, R. & Katz, L. (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (pp. 227248). Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A. & Verhoeven, L. (2017). Epilogue: Universals and particulars in learning to read across seventeen orthographies. In Verhoeven, L. & Perfetti, C. A. (Eds.), Learning to read across languages and writing systems (pp. 455480). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Perry, C., Zorzi, M., & Ziegler, J. C. (2019). Understanding dyslexia through personalized large-scale computational models. Psychological Science, 30(3), 386395.Google Scholar
Plaut, D. C., & Gonnerman, L. M. (2000). Are non-semantic morphological effects incompatible with a distributed connectionist approach to lexical processing? Language and Cognitive Processing, 15, 445485.Google Scholar
Pugh, K. R., Landi, N., & Preston, J. L. et al. (2013). The relationship between phonological and auditory processing and brain organization in beginning readers. Brain and Language, 125, 173183.Google Scholar
Pugh, K. R., & Verhoeven, L. (2018). Introduction to this special issue: Dyslexia across languages and writing systems. Scientific Studies of Reading, 22, 16.Google Scholar
Ramus, F. (2014). Neuroimaging sheds new light on the phonological deficit in dyslexia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 274275.Google Scholar
Richlan, F. (2012). Developmental dyslexia: Dysfunction of a left hemisphere reading network. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 120.Google Scholar
Richlan, F. (2014). Functional neuroanatomy of developmental dyslexia: The role of orthographic depth. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 347.Google Scholar
Rueckl, J. G., Paz-Alonso, P. M., & Molfese, P. J. et al. (2015). Universal brain signature of proficient reading: Evidence from four contrasting languages. Proceedings National Academy of Sciences, 112, 1551015515.Google Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S., & Gonnerman, L. M. (2000). Explaining derivational morphology as the convergence of codes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 353361.Google Scholar
Serniclaes, W., Van Heghe, S., Mousty, P., Carré, R., & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2004). Allophonic mode of speech perception in dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 336361.Google Scholar
Shankweiler, D., & Liberman, I. Y. (Eds.). (1989). Phonology and reading disability: Solving the reading puzzle. Ann Arbor, MI: The University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151218.Google Scholar
Share, D. L. (2004). Orthographic learning at a glance: On the time course and developmental onset of reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 267298.Google Scholar
Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2008). Paying attention to reading: The neurobiology of reading and dyslexia. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 13291349.Google Scholar
Shu, H., Peng, H., & McBride-Chang, C. (2008). Phonological awareness in young Chinese children. Developmental Science, 11, 171181.Google Scholar
Siok, W. T., Niu, Z. Jin, Z., Perfetti, C. A., & Tan, L. H. (2008). A structural-functional basis for dyslexia in the cortex of Chinese readers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 55615566.Google Scholar
Siok, W. T., Perfetti, C. A., Jin, Z., & Tan, L. H. (2004). Biological abnormality of impaired reading constrained by culture: Evidence from Chinese. Nature, 431, 7176.Google Scholar
Snowling, M. J. (2000). Language and literacy skills: Who is at risk and why? In Bishop, D. V. M. & Leonard, L. B. (Eds.), Speech and language impairment in children: Causes, characteristics, interventions and outcome (pp. 245260). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Snowling, M. J. (2013). Early identification and interventions for dyslexia: A contemporary view. Journal of Research in Special Educational Needs, 13, 714.Google Scholar
Snowling, M. J., Gallagher, A., & Frith, U. (2003). Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: Individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Development, 74, 358373.Google Scholar
Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2011). Evidence-based interventions for reading and language difficulties: Creating a virtuous circle. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 123.Google Scholar
Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2012). Interventions for children’s language and literacy difficulties. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 47, 2734.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (2000). Progress in understanding reading: Scientific foundations and new frontiers. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. L., Trainin, G., Necoechea, D. M., & Hammill, D. D. (2003). Rapid naming, phonological awareness, and reading: A meta-analysis of the correlation evidence. Review of Educational Research, 73, 407440.Google Scholar
Tallal, P. (1980). Auditory-temporal perception, phonics, and reading disabilities in children. Brain and Language, 9, 182198.Google Scholar
Torgerson, C. J., Brooks, G., & Hall, J. (2006). A systematic review of the research literature on the use of phonics in the teaching of reading and spelling (Research Report No. 711). London: DfES Publications.Google Scholar
Torgesen, J. K. & Hudson, R. (2006). Reading fluency: Critical issues for struggling readers. In Samuels, S. J. & Farstrup, A. (Eds.). Reading fluency: The forgotten dimension of reading success. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.Google Scholar
Torgeson, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Burgess, S., & Hecht, S. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatic naming ability to the growth of word-reading skills in second-to fifth-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 161195.Google Scholar
Treiman, R., & Zukowski, A. (1996). Children’s sensitivity to syllables, onsets, rimes, and phonemes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 61, 193215.Google Scholar
Tunmer, W. E., & Hoover, W. A. (1993). Components of variance models of language-related factors in reading disability: A conceptual overview. In Joshi, R. J. & Leong, C. K. (Eds.), Reading disabilities: Diagnosis and component processes (pp. 135173). Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Verhoeven, L. (2000). Components in early second language reading and spelling. Scientific Studies of Reading, 4, 313330.Google Scholar
Verhoeven, L., & Perfetti, C. (2008). Advances in text comprehension: Model, process, and development. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 293301.Google Scholar
Verhoeven, L., & Perfetti, C. (2011). Introduction to this special issue: Vocabulary growth and reading skill. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15, 17.Google Scholar
Verhoeven, L., & Perfetti, C. (2017). Learning to read across languages and writing systems. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Verhoeven, L., & van Leeuwe, J. (2009). Modeling the growth of word decoding skills: Evidence from Dutch. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 205223.Google Scholar
Verhoeven, L., van Leeuwe, J., & Vermeer, A. (2011). Vocabulary growth and reading development across the elementary school years. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15, 825.Google Scholar
Vicari, S., Finzi, A., & Menghini, L. et al. (2005). Do children with developmental dyslexia have an implicit learning deficit? Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 76, 13921397.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: new evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 7387.Google Scholar
Willcutt, E. G., & Pennington, B. F. (2000). Comorbidity of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Differences by gender and subtype. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 179191.Google Scholar
Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Landerl, K. (2000). The double-deficit hypothesis and difficulties in learning to read a regular orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 668680.Google Scholar
Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415438.Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., Castel, C., & Pech-Georgel, C. et al. (2008). Developmental dyslexia and the dual route model of reading: Simulating individual differences and subtypes. Cognition, 107, 151178.Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 329.Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., & Zorzi, M. (2014). Modelling reading development through phonological decoding and self-teaching: Implications for dyslexia. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences, 369, 20120397.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×