Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-9pm4c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T09:44:36.230Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - The Case Study Perspective on Psychological Research

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Randi Martin
Affiliation:
Rice University
Rachel Hull
Affiliation:
Rice University
Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Henry L. Roediger III
Affiliation:
Washington University, St Louis
Diane F. Halpern
Affiliation:
Claremont McKenna College, California
Get access

Summary

The case study approach has a rich history in psychology as a method for observing the ways in which individuals may demonstrate abnormal thinking and behavior, for collecting evidence concerning the circumstances and consequences surrounding such disorders, and for providing data to generate and test models of human behavior (see Yin, 1998, for an overview). Nevertheless, the most typical methods for scientifically studying human cognition involve testing groups of healthy people – typically, college undergraduates. In their statistics and research methods courses, psychology students are trained to study the effects of manipulations that are significant across groups of participants despite considerable variation at the level of the individual. They are trained to be skeptical of reasoning from an individual case that goes against the general trend, and to be suspicious of the compelling anecdote that may be introduced to defend some position about how cognition or social interactions might work. Given this state of affairs, are the practitioners of the case study approach misguided, or can valid conclusions be drawn from findings with one patient? Can case reports that detail a client's symptoms and reactions to psychotherapy constitute scientific data? What about case studies that investigate how brain damage affects particular cognitive processes? The goal of this chapter is to demonstrate how single-case-study approaches in clinical psychology and cognitive neuropsychology have contributed to the advancement of theories and models of human cognition and to address the common concerns that researchers often have about case study methodology.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Badecker, W., & Caramazza, A. (1985). On consideration of method and theory governing the use of clinical categories in neurolinguistics and cognitive neuropsychology: The case against agrammatism. Cognition, 20, 97–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berndt, R. S., Mitchum, C., & Haendiges, A. (1996). Comprehension of reversible sentences in “agrammatism”: A meta-analysis. Cognition, 58, 289–308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berndt, R. S., Mitchum, C., & Haendiges, A. (1996). Comprehension of reversible sentences in “agrammatism”: A meta-analysis. Cognition, 58, 289–308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowman, C. G., & Mertz, E. (1996). A dangerous direction: Legal interventions in sexual abuse survivor therapy. Harvard Law Review, 109, 549–639.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briere, J., & Conte, J. (1993). Self-reported amnesia for abuse in adults molested as children. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 6, 21–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewin, C. R. (1989). Cognitive change processes in psychotherapy. Psychological Review, 96, 379–394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, D., Scheflin, A. W., & Hammond, D. C. (1998). Memory, trauma treatment, and the law. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Butterworth, B., Campbell, R., & Howard, D. (1986). The uses of short-term memory: A case study. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 38, 705–737.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, D. T., Stanley, J. C., & Gage, N. L. (1963). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for research. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Caramazza, A., & McCloskey, M. (1988). The case for single-patient studies. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5, 517–528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caramazza, A., & Hillis, A. (1989). The disruption of sentence production: Some dissociations. Brain and Language, 36, 628–650.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caramazza, A., & Zurif, E. (1976). Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in language comprehension: Evidence from aphasia. Brain and Language, 3, 572–582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ceci, S. J., & Loftus, E. F. (1994). ‘Memory work’: A royal road to false memories?Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 351–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clancy, S. A. (2001). Memory distortion in individuals reporting recovered memories of trauma. Dissertation Abstracts International, 62, 2098B.Google Scholar
Clancy, S. A., McNally, R. J., & Schacter, D. L. (1999). Effects of guided imagery on memory distortion in women reporting recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 12, 559–569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, N. J., & Squire, L. R. (1980). Preserved learning and retention of pattern analyzing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of “knowing how” and “knowing that.” Science, 210, 207–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M. (2001). Assumptions and methods in cognitive neuropsychology. In B. Rapp (Ed.), The handbook of neuropsychology (pp. 3–22). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Crawford, J. R., & Howell, D. C. (1998). Comparing an individual's test score against norms derived from small samples. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12, 482–486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowder, R. (1982). The demise of short-term memory. Acta Psychologica, 50, 291–323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cupples, L., & Inglis, A. L. (1993). When task demands induce “asyntactic” comprehension: A study of sentence interpretation in aphasia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 10, 201–234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S., & O' Seaghdha, P. G. (1992). Stages of lexical access in language production. Cognition, 42, 287–314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dell, G. S., Schwartz, M. F., Martin, N., Saffran, E. M., & Gagnon, D. A. (1997). Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. Psychological Review, 104, 801–838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodson, C. S., & Johnson, M. K. (1993). Rate of false source attributions depends on how questions are asked. Journal of Psychology, 106, 541–557.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eyre, D. P. (1991). Therapy with a sexually abused woman. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 72, 403–415.Google ScholarPubMed
Faust, M., & Chiarello, C. (1998). Constraints on sentence priming in the cerebral hemispheres: Effects of intervening words in sentences and lists. Brain and Language, 63, 219–236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gleaves, D. H., Smith, S. M., Butler, L. D., & Spiegel, D. (2004). False and recovered memories in the laboratory and clinic: A review of experimental and clinical evidence. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 3–28.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1935; reprinted in 1960). A general introduction to psychoanalysis. New York: Washington Square Press.Google Scholar
Green, D. & Price, C. 2001. Functional imaging in the study of recovery patterns in bilingual aphasia. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4, 191–201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanten, G., & Martin, R. (2001). A developmental phonological short-term memory deficit: A case study. Brain and Cognition, 45, 164–188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hickok, G. (2001). Functional anatomy of speech perception and speech production: Psycholinguistic implications. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30, 225–235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hitch, G. (1980). Developing the concept of working memory. In G. Claxton (Ed.), Cognitive psychology: New directions. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Howard, D. (1985). Introduction to “On agrammatism” (Ueber Agrammatismus), by Max Isserlin, 1922. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 2, 303–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. K., Hashroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, R., & Manicavasagar, V. (2001). Is there a false memory syndrome? A review of three cases. Comprehensive psychiatry, 42, 342–348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, J., Lesser, R., & Coltheart, M. (1992). Psycholinguistic assessments of language processing in aphasia. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Kolb, L. C. (1988). Recovery of memory and repressed fantasy in combat-induced post-traumatic stress disorder of Vietnam veterans. In H. M. Pettinati (Ed.), Hypnosis and memory (pp. 265–274). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kolk, H., van Grunsven, M., & Keyser, A. (1985). On parallelism between production and comprehension in agrammatism. In M. L. Kean (Ed.), Agrammatism (pp. 347–384). New York: Academic Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Lief, H. I. (2003). Questions raised by the controversy over recovered memories of incest. Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis & Dynamic Psychiatry, 31, 381–395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linebarger, M. C., Schwartz, M. F., Romania, J. R., Kohn, S. E., & Stephens, D. L. (2000). Grammatical encoding in aphasia: Evidence from a “processing prosthesis.” Brain and Language, 75, 416–427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linebarger, M., Schwartz, M., & Saffran, E. (1983). Sensitivity to grammatical structure in so-called agrammatic aphasics. Cognition, 13, 361–392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loftus, E. F. (1993). The reality of repressed memories. American Psychologist, 48, 518–537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loftus, E. F., Polonsky, S., & Fullilove, M. T. (1994). Memories of childhood sexual abuse: Remembering and repressing. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 67–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, R. C. (2001). Sentence comprehension deficits. In B. Rapp (Ed.), Handbook of cognitive neuropsychology (pp. 349–374). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Martin, R. C. (1993). Short-term memory and sentence processing: Evidence from neuropsychology. Memory and Cognition, 12, 176–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, R. C., & Breedin, S. (1992). Dissociations between speech perception and phonological short-term memory. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 9, 509–534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, R. C., & Freedman, M. L. (2001). Short-term retention of lexical-semantic representations: Implications for speech production. Memory, 9, 261–280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, R. C., Lesch, M., & Bartha, M. (1999). Independence of input and output phonology in word processing and short-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 3–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, R. C., Miller, M., & Vu, H. (2004) Lexical-semantic retention and speech production: Further evidence from normal and brain-damaged participants for a phrasal scope of planning. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21, 625–644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, R. C., Shelton, J. R., & Yaffee, L. S. (1994). Language processing and working memory: Neuropsychological evidence for separate phonological and semantic capacities. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 83–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, R. C., Wetzel, F., Blossom-Stach, C. & Feher, E. (1989). Syntactic loss versus processing deficit: An assessment of two theories of agrammatism and syntactic comprehension deficits. Cognition, 32, 157–191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDermott, K. B. (1996). The persistence of false memories in list recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 212–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McHugh, P. R., Lief, H. J., Freyd, P. P., & Fetkewicz, J. M. (2003). From refusal to reconciliation: Family relationships after an accusation based on recovered memories. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 192, 525–531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNally, R. J. (2003). Recovering memories of trauma: A view from the laboratory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 32–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNally, R. J., Metzger, L. J., Lasko, N. B., Clancy, S. A., & Pitman, R. K. (1998). Directed forgetting of trauma cues in adult survivors of childhood sexual abuse with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 596–601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miceli, G., Mazzucchi, A., Menn, L., & Goodglass, H. (1983). Contrasting cases of Italian agrammatic aphasia without comprehension disorder. Brain and Language, 19, 65–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ofshe, R., & Watters, E. (1993, March/April). Making monsters. Society, 4–16.Google Scholar
Poeppel, D., & Hickok, G. (2004). Towards a new functional anatomy of language. Cognition, 92, 1–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perrotto, R. S., & Culkin, J. (1993). Exploring abnormal psychology. New York: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Price, C. (1998). The functional anatomy of word comprehension and production. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 281–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rapp, B. (2001). Handbook of cognitive neuropsychology. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Rapp, B., & Goldrick, M. (2000). Discreteness and interactivity in spoken word production. Psychological Review, 107, 460–499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, K. J., & Roediger, H. L., III. (1997). Associative processes in false recall and false recognition, Psychological Science, 8, 231–237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roelofs, A. (1992). A spreading activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking. Cognition, 42, 107–142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romani, C. (1992). Are there distinct input and output buffers? Evidence from an aphasic patient with an impaired output buffer. Language and Cognitive Processes, 7, 131–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saffran, E. M., Berndt, R. S., & Schwartz, M.F. (1989). The quantitative analysis of agrammatic production: Procedure and data. Brain and Language, 37, 440–479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sargant, W., & Slater, E. (1941). Amnestic syndromes of war. Proceeding of the Royal Society of Medicine, 34, 757–764.Google Scholar
Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 19, 252–259.Google Scholar
Searleman, A., & Herrmann, D. (1994). Memory from a broader perspective. New York: Mcgraw Hill.Google Scholar
Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, S. M., Gleaves, D. H., Pierce, B. H., Williams, T. L., Gilliland, T. R., & Gerkens, D. R. (2003). Eliciting and comparing false and recovered memories: An experimental approach. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 251–279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Squire, L. R., Knowlton, B., & Musen, G. (1993). The structure and organization of memory. In L. W. Porter & M. R. Rosenzweig (Eds.), Annual review of psychology (Vol. 44, pp. 463–495). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stein, J. (1970). Neurosis in contemporary society. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Taylor, S. (2004). Amnesia, folklore and folks: Recovered memories in clinical practice. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 33, 105–108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ullman, M. (2001). The neural basis of lexicon and grammar in first and second language: The declarative/procedural model. Bilingualism, 4, 105–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaid, J., & Hull, R. (2002). Re-envisioning the bilingual brain using functional neuroimaging: Methodological and interpretive issues. In F. Fabbro (Ed.), Advances in the neurolinguistics of bilingualism: A festschrift for Michel Paradis (pp. 315–355). Udine, Italy: Forum.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale Revised.San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Yin, R. (1998). The abridged version of case study research: Design and method. In Bickman, L. and Rog, D. J. (Eds.), Handbook of applied social research methods (pp. 229–259). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×