Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vvkck Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T12:52:23.057Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - Spatial integration of unusually high numbers of immigrant females into the South Group: further support for the bisexually bonded model in Taï chimpanzees

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 November 2019

Christophe Boesch
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Evolutionäre Anthropologie, Germany
Roman Wittig
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Evolutionäre Anthropologie, Germany
Catherine Crockford
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Evolutionäre Anthropologie, Germany
Linda Vigilant
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Evolutionäre Anthropologie, Germany
Tobias Deschner
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Evolutionäre Anthropologie, Germany
Fabian Leendertz
Affiliation:
Robert Koch-Institut, Germany
Get access

Summary

Chimpanzee populations differ in ranging patterns and social systems. Taï chimpanzees are considered bisexually bonded, with males and females ranging similarly. East African female chimpanzees often range alone and occupy distinct neighbourhoods. Little attention has been paid to the process of spatial integration of immigrant females within the different social systems. In Taï South Group, a large-scale female transfer of 12 immigrants occurred between 2012 and 2015. We analyse the extent of spatial overlap and association between males and resident and immigrant females. Resident and immigrant females exhibited high levels of spatial overlap, which increased over time, without forming distinct neighbourhoods. All females spent little time alone, and immigrant females spent more time in mixed-sex parties than resident females, especially when they presented maximal sexual swellings. Our findings support the social passport hypothesis and a Bisexually Bonded Community Model for Taï chimpanzees, in which eventually immigrant females occupy a similar range as the community and resident females adapt their ranging patterns towards the initial range of immigrant females.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest
40 Years of Research
, pp. 159 - 177
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behaviour, 49, 227266. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853974X00534CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, D. P., Nordheim, E. V., Boesch, C. & Moermond, T. C. (2002). Factors influencing fission–fusion grouping in chimpanzees in the Taï Forest, Cote d’Ivoire. In Boesch, C., Hohmann, G. & Marchant, L. F. (eds.), Chimpanzee Behavioural Diversity (pp. 90101). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing Linguistic Data: A Practical Introduction to Statistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C. & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. (2012). lme4: Linear Mixed-effects Models Using Eigen and S4. R package version, 1(7), 123.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. (1996). Social grouping in Taï chimpanzees. In McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F. & Nishida, T. (eds.), Great Ape Societies. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. & Boesch-Achermann, H. (2000). The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: Behavioural Ecology and Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Chapman, C. A. & Wrangham, R. W. (1993). Range use of the forest chimpanzees of Kibale: Implications for the understanding of chimpanzee social organization. American Journal of Primatology, 31, 263273. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350310403CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deschner, T. & Boesch, C. (2007). Can the patterns of sexual swelling cycles in female Taï chimpanzees be explained by the cost-of-sexual-attraction hypothesis? International Journal of Primatology, 28, 389406. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-007–9120–1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dobson, A. J. & Barnett, A. (2008). An Introduction to Generalized Linear Models. Boca Raton: CRC Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fieberg, J. & Kochanny, C. O. (2005). Quantifying home-range overlap: The importance of the utilization distribution. Journal of Wildlife Management, 69, 13461359.Google Scholar
Field, A. (2005). Discovering Statistics using SPSS. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Foerster, S., McLellan, K., Schroepfer-Walker, K., Murray, C. M., Krupenye, C., Gilby, I. C., et al. (2015). Social bonds in the dispersing sex: partner preferences among adult female chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 105, 139152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.04.012Google Scholar
Forstmeier, W. & Schielzeth, H. (2011). Cryptic multiple hypotheses testing in linear models: overestimated effect sizes and the winner’s curse. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 65, 4755. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-010–1038–5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, J. & Weisberg, S. (2010). An R Companion to Applied Regression. New York: Sage.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hashimoto, C. & Furuichi, T. (2015). Sex differences in ranging and association patterns in chimpanzees in comparison with bonobos. In Furuichi, T., Yamagiwa, J. & Aureli, F. (eds.), Dispersing Primate Females (pp. 105126). Tokyo: Springer Japan, https://doi.org/10.1007/978–4-431–55480-6_5Google Scholar
Herbinger, I., Boesch, C. & Rothe, H. (2001). Territory characteristics among three neighboring chimpanzee communities in the Taï National Park, Côte d’Ivoire. International Journal of Primatology, 22, 143167. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005663212997Google Scholar
Kahlenberg, S. M., Thompson, M. E., Muller, M. N. & Wrangham, R. W. (2008a). Immigration costs for female chimpanzees and male protection as an immigrant counterstrategy to intrasexual aggression. Animal Behaviour, 76, 14971509. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.05.029Google Scholar
Kahlenberg, S. M., Thompson, E. M. & Wrangham, R. W. (2008b). Female competition over core areas in Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii, Kibale National Park, Uganda. International Journal of Primatology, 29, 931. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-008–9276–3Google Scholar
Lehmann, J. (2003). Social influences on ranging patterns among chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) in the Taï National Park, Cote d’Ivoire. Behavioral Ecology, 14, 642649. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arg047Google Scholar
Lehmann, J. & Boesch, C. (2005). Bisexually bonded ranging in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 57, 525535. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-004–0891–5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehmann, J. & Boesch, C. (2009). Sociality of the dispersing sex: The nature of social bonds in West African female chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Animal Behaviour, 77, 377387. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.09.038CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, C. M., Mane, S. V. & Pusey, A. E. (2007). Dominance rank influences female space use in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes: Towards an ideal despotic distribution. Animal Behaviour, 74, 17951804. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.03.024Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1989). Social interactions between resident and immigrant female chimpanzees. In Heltne, P. & Marquardt, L. (eds.), Understanding Chimpanzees (pp. 6889). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Nishida, T., Corp, N., Hamai, M., Hasegawa, T., Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M., Hosaka, K., et al. (2003). Demography, female life history, and reproductive profiles among the chimpanzees of Mahale. American Journal of Primatology, 59, 99121. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.10068Google Scholar
Nishida, T., Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M., Hasegawa, T. & Takahata, Y. (1985). Group extinction and female transfer in wild chimpanzees in the Mahale National Park, Tanzania. Ethology, 67, 284301.Google Scholar
Otali, E. & Gilchrist, J. S. (2006). Why chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) mothers are less gregarious than nonmothers and males: The infant safety hypothesis. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 59, 561570. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-005–0081–0Google Scholar
Pepper, J. W., Mitani, J. C. & Watts, D. P. (1999). General gregariousness and specific social preferences among wild chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology, 20, 613632. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1020760616641CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pusey, A. E. (1980). Inbreeding avoidance in chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 28, 543552. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003–3472(80)80063–7Google Scholar
Pusey, A. E. & Schroepfer-Walker, K. (2013). Female competition in chimpanzees. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 368(1631), 2013007720130077. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0077CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pusey, A. E., Williams, J. & Goodall, J. (1997). The influence of dominance rank on the reproductive success of female chimpanzees. Science, 277(5327), 828831. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.277.5327.828Google Scholar
Sakura, O. (1994). Factors affecting party size and composition of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) Bossou, Guinea. International Journal of Primatology, 15, 167183. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02735272Google Scholar
Schielzeth, H. & Forstmeier, W. (2009). Conclusions beyond support: Overconfident estimates in mixed models. Behavioral Ecology, 20, 416420. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arn145Google Scholar
Smuts, B.B. & Smuts, R.W. (1993). Male aggression and sexual coercion of females in nonhuman primates and other mammals: evidence and theoretical implications. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 22(22), 163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, M. E., Kahlenberg, S. M., Gilby, I. C. & Wrangham, R. W. (2007). Core area quality is associated with variance in reproductive success among female chimpanzees at Kibale National Park. Animal Behaviour, 73, 501512. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.09.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wakefield, M. L. (2013). Social dynamics among females and their influence on social structure in an East African chimpanzee community. Animal Behaviour, 85, 13031313. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.03.019Google Scholar
Wallis, J. (1992). Chimpanzee genital swelling and its role in the pattern of sociosexual behavior. American Journal of Primatology, 28, 101113. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350280203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warton, D. I. & Hui, F. K. C. (2011). The arcsine is asinine: the analysis of proportions in ecology. Ecology, 92, 310. https://doi.org/10.1890/10–0340.1Google Scholar
Williams, J. M., Pusey, A. E., Carlis, J. V., Farm, B. P. & Goodall, J. (2002). Female competition and male territorial behaviour influence female chimpanzees’ ranging patterns. Animal Behaviour, 63, 347360. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2001.1916Google Scholar
Wittig, R. M. (2018). Taï chimpanzees. In Vonk, J. & Shackelford, T. K. (eds.), Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior. Heidelberg: Springer.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. (1980). An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour, 75, 262300. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853980X00447Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. (2000). Why are male chimpanzees more gregarious than mothers? A scramble competition hypothesis. In Kappeler, P. (ed.), Primate Males (pp. 248258). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W., Clark, A. P. & Isabirye-Basuta, G. (1992). Female social relationships and social organization of the Kibale Forest chimpanzees. In Nishida, T., McGrew, W. C., Marler, P., Pickford, M. & de Waal, F. D. M. (eds.), Topics in Primatology (pp. 8198). Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×