Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
  • Print publication year: 2014
  • Online publication date: July 2014

2 - Evolution of behavioral and neural complexity: learning and memory in ChamberedNautilus

from Part I - Cognition, brain and evolution

References

Agin, V., Chichery, R. and Chichery, M.-P. (2001). Effects of learning on cytochrome oxidase activity in cuttlefish brain. Neuroreport, 12: 1–4.
Agin, V., Chichery, R., Dickel, L. and Chichery, M.-P. (2006). The “prawn-in-the-tube” procedure in the cuttlefish: Habituation or passive avoidance learning?Learning and Memory, 13: 97–101.
Agin, V., Chichery, R., Maubert, E. and Chichery, M.-P. (2003). Time-dependent effects of cycloheximide on long-term memory in the cuttlefish. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 75: 141–146.
Agin, V., Dickel, L., Chichery, R. and Chichery, M.-P. (1998). Evidence for a specific short-term memory in the cuttlefish, Sepia. Behavioural Processes, 43: 329–334.
Agin, V., Poirier, R., Chichery, R., Dickel, L. and Chichery, M.-P. (2006). Developmental study of multiple memory stages in the cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 86: 264–269.
Alkon, D. L., Ikeno, H., Dworkin, J., McPhie, D. L., Olds, J. L., Lederhendler, I., Matzel, L., Schreurs, B. G., Kuzirian, A. and Collin, C. (1990). Contraction of neuronal branching volume: An anatomic correlate of Pavlovian conditioning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 87: 1611–1614.
Alves, C., Boal, J. G. and Dickel, L. (2008). Short-distance navigation in cephalopods: A review and synthesis. Cognitive Processing, 9: 239–247.
Alves, C., Chichery, R., Boal, J. G. and Dickel, L. (2007). Orientation in the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis: Response versus place learning. Animal Cognition, 10: 29–36.
Aronson, R. B. (1991). Ecology, paleobiology and evolutionary constraint in the octopus. Bulletin of Marine Science, 49: 1–2.
Barber, V. C. and Wright, D. E. (1969). The fine structure of the sense organs of the cephalopod mollusc Nautilus. Cell and Tissue Research, 102: 293–312.
Basil, J. A., Bahctinova, I., Kuroiwa, K., Lee, N., Mims, D., Preis, M. and Soucier, C. (2005). The function of the rhinophore and the tentacles of Nautilus pompilius L. (Cephalopoda, Nautiloidea) in orientation to odor. Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology, 38: 209–221.
Basil, J. A., Hanlon, R. T., Sheikh, S. I. and Atema, J. (2000). Three-dimensional odor tracking by Nautilus pompilius. Journal of Experimental Biology, 203: 1409–1414.
Basil, J. A., Lazenby, G. B., Nakanuku, L. and Hanlon, R. T. (2002). Female nautilus are attracted to male conspecific odor. Bulletin of Marine Science, 70: 217–225.
Basil, J. A. and Sandeman, D. (2000). Crayfish (Cherax destructor) use tactile cues to detect and learn topographical changes in their environment. Ethology, 106: 247–259.
Baxter, D. A. and Byrne, J. H. (2006). Feeding behavior of Aplysia: A model system for comparing cellular mechanisms of classical and operant conditioning. Learning and Memory, 13: 669–80.
Benjamin, P. R. (2000). A systems approach to the cellular analysis of associative learning in the pond snail Lymnaea. Learning and Memory, 7: 124–131.
Bidder, A. M. (1962). Use of the tentacles, swimming and buoyancy control in the pearly nautilus. Nature, 196: 451–454.
Bitterman, M. E., Menzel, R., Fietz, A. and Schäfer, S. (1983). Classical conditioning of proboscis extension in honeybees (Apis mellifera). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 97: 107–119.
Blackwell, K. T. (2006). Subcellular, cellular, and circuit mechanisms underlying classical conditioning in Hermissenda crassicornis. Anatomical Record. Part B, New Anatomist, 289: 25–37.
Boal, J. G. (1991). Complex learning in Octopus bimaculoides. American Malacological Bulletin, 9: 75–80.
Boal, J. G. (1996). A review of simultaneous visual discrimination as a method of training octopuses. Biological Reviews, 71: 157–190.
Boal, J. G., Dunham, A. W., Williams, K. T. and Hanlon, R. T. (2000). Experimental evidence for spatial learning in octopuses (Octopus bimaculoides). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 114: 246–252.
Bonnaud, L., Lu, C. C. and Boucher-Rodoni, R. (2006). Morphological character evolution and molecular trees in sepiids (Mollusca: Cephalopoda): Is the cuttlebone a robust phylogenetic marker? Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 89: 139–150.
Bonnaud, L., Ozouf-Costaz, C. and Boucher-Rodoni, R. (2004). A molecular and karyological approach to the taxonomy of Nautilus. Comptes Rendus de Biologie, 327: 133–138.
Boycott, B. B. (1961). The functional organization of the brain of the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis. Proceedings of the Royal Society. B: Biological Sciences, 153: 503–534.
Boycott, B. B. and Young, J. Z. (1955). A memory system in Octopus vulgaris Lamarck. Proceedings of the Royal Society. B: Biological Sciences, 143: 449–480.
Boyle, P. R. (1986). Neural control of cephalopod behavior. The Mollusca, 9: 1–97.
Brandon, S. E. and Wagner, A. R. (1998). Occasion setting: Influences of conditioned emotional responses and configured ural cues. In Occasion setting: Associative learning and cognition in animals. Schmajuk, N. A. and Holland, P. C. (Eds), pp. 343–382. Washington, DC, USA: American Psychological Association.
Brembs, B., Baxter, D. A. and Byrne, J. H. (2004). Extending in vitro conditioning in Aplysia to analyze operant and classical processes in the same preparation. Learning and Memory, 11: 412–20.
Budelmann, B. U. (1995). Cephalopod sense organs, nerves and the brain: Adaptations for high performance and life style. Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology, 25: 13–33.
Burrell, B. D. and Sahley, C. L. (2001). Learning in simple systems. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11: 757–764.
Carew, T. J. and Sahley, C. L. (1986). Invertebrate learning and memory: From behavior to molecules. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 9: 435–487.
Carlini, D. B., Young, R. E. and Vecchione, M. (2001). A molecular phylogeny of the Octopoda (Mollusca: Cephalopoda) evaluated in light of morphological evidence. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution, 21: 388–397.
Carlson, B. A., McKibben, J. N. and De Gruy, M. V. (1984). Telemetric investigation of vertical migration of Nautilus belauensis in Palau. Pacific Science, 38: 183–188.
Cartron, L., Darmaillacq, A. S. and Dickel, L. (2013). The “prawn-in-the-tube” procedure: What do cuttlefish learn and memorize?Behavioural Brain Research, 240: 29–32.
Chamberlain, J. A. (1990). Jet propulsion of Nautilus: A surviving example of early paleozoic cephalopod locomotor design. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 68: 806–814.
Chamberlain, J. A. (1993). Locomotion in ancient seas: Constraint and opportunity in cephalopod adaptive design. Geobios, 26: 49–61.
Chandra, S. B. C., Hosler, J. S. and Smith, B. H. (2000). Heritable variation for latent inhibition and its correlation with reversal learning in honeybees (Apis mellifera). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 114: 86–97.
Cheng, K., Shettleworth, S. J., Huttenlocher, J. and Rieser, J. J. (2007). Bayesian integration of spatial information. Psychological Bulletin, 133: 625–637.
Clarke, M. R. and Trueman, E. R. (1988). Evolution of recent cephalopods. A brief review. The Mollusca, 12: 11–79.
Cole, P. D. and Adamo, S. A. (2005). Cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis: Cephalopoda) hunting behavior and associative learning. Animal Cognition, 8: 27–30.
Collett, T. S., Dillmann, E., Giger, A. and Wehner, R. (1992). Visual landmarks and route following in desert ants. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 170: 435–442.
Crook, R. J. (2008). Behavioral correlates of learning and memory in Chambered Nautilus, Nautilus pompilius. New York, NY, USA: City University of New York.
Crook, R. J. and Basil, J. A. (2008a). A biphasic memory curve in the chambered nautilus, Nautilus pompilius L. (Cephalopoda: Nautiloidea). Journal of Experimental Biology, 211: 1992–1998.
Crook, R. J. and Basil, J. A. (2008b). A role for nautilus in studies of the evolution of brain and behavior. Communicative and Integrative Biology, 1: 18–19.
Crook, R. J. and Basil, J. A. (2013). Flexible spatial orientation and navigational strategies in chambered Nautilus. Ethology, 119: 77–85.
Crook, R. J., Hanlon, R. T. and Basil, J. A. (2009). Memory of visual and topographical features suggests spatial learning in nautilus (Nautilus pompilius L.). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123: 264–274.
Crow, T. (2004). Pavlovian conditioning of Hermissenda: Current cellular, molecular, and circuit perspectives. Learning and Memory, 11: 229–238.
Crow, T. J. and Alkon, D. L. (1978). Retention of an associative behavioral change in Hermissenda. Science, 201: 1239–1241.
Darmaillacq, A.-S., Chichery, R. and Dickel, L. (2006). Food imprinting, new evidence from the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis. Biology Letters, 2: 345–347.
Darmaillacq, A.-S., Chichery, R., Shashar, N. and Dickel, L. (2006). Early familiarization overrides innate prey preference in newly hatched Sepia officinalis cuttlefish. Animal Behaviour, 71: 511–514.
Darmaillacq, A. S., Dickel, L., Chichery, M.-P., Agin, V. and Chichery, R. (2004). Rapid taste aversion learning in adult cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis. Animal Behaviour, 68: 1291–1298.
Davis, H. P. and Squire, L. R. (1984). Protein synthesis and memory: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 96: 518–559.
Davis, R. L. (2005). Olfactory memory formation in Drosophila: From molecular to systems neuroscience. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28: 275–302.
Dickel, L., Chichery, M.-P. and Chichery, R. (2001). Increase of learning abilities and maturation of the vertical lobe complex during postembryonic development in the cuttlefish, Sepia. Developmental Psychobiology, 39: 92–98.
Dunstan, A. J., Alanis, O. and Marshall, J. (2010). Nautilus pompilius fishing and population decline in the Philippines: A comparison with an unexploited Australian Nautilus population. Fisheries Research, 106: 239–247.
Dunstan, A. J., Ward, P. D. and Marshall, N. J. (2011). Vertical distribution and migration patterns of Nautilus pompilius. Public Library of Science One, 6: e16311.
Dzik, J. (1981). Origin of the Cephalopoda. Acta Palaeontologica Polonica, 26: 161–191.
Emery, D. G. (1975). The histology and fine structure of the olfactory organ of the squid Lolliguncula brevis blainville. Tissue and Cell, 7: 357–367.
Fanselow, M. S. and Poulos, A. M. (2005). The neuroscience of mammalian associative learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 56: 207–234.
Fiorito, G. and Chichery, R. (1995). Lesions of the vertical lobe impair visual discrimination learning by observation in Octopus vulgaris. Neuroscience Letters, 192: 117–120.
Fiorito, G. and Scotto, P. (1992). Observational learning in Octopus vulgaris. Science, 256: 545–547.
Fukuda, Y. (1980). Observations by SEM, Nautilus macromphalus in captivity. Tokyo, Japan: Tokai University Press.
Gallistel, C. R. (1990). The organization of learning. Cambridge, MA, USA: MIT Press.
Gelperin, A. and Culligan, N. (1984). In vitro expression of in vivo learning by an isolated molluscan CNS. Brain Research, 304: 207–213.
Gibson, B. M. and Shettleworth, S. J. (2005). Place versus response learning revisited: Tests of blocking on the radial maze. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119: 567–586.
Gilly, W. F. and Lucero, M. T. (1992). Behavioral responses to chemical stimulation of the olfactory organ in the squid Loligo opalescens. Journal of Experimental Biology, 162: 209–229.
Graindorge, N., Alves, C., Darmaillacq, A.-S., Chichery, R., Dickel, L. and Bellanger, C. (2006). Effects of dorsal and ventral vertical lobe electrolytic lesions on spatial learning and locomotor activity in Sepia officinalis. Behavioral Neuroscience, 120: 1151–1158.
Grasso, F. W. and Basil, J. A. (2009). The evolution of flexible behavioral repertoires in cephalopod molluscs. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 74: 231–245.
Graziadei, P. (1964). Receptors in the sucker of the cuttlefish. Nature, 203: 384–386.
Hanlon, R. T. and Messenger, J. B. (1996). Cephalopod behaviour. Cambridge, MA, USA: Cambridge University Press.
Hochner, B., Shomrat, T. and Fiorito, G. (2006). The octopus: A model for a comparative analysis of the evolution of learning and memory mechanisms. The Biological Bulletin, 210: 308–317.
Hvorecny, L. M., Grudowski, J. L., Blakeslee, C. J., Simmons, T. L., Roy, P. R., Brooks, J. A., Hanner, R. M., Beigel, M. E., Karson, M. A. and Nichols, R. H. (2007). Octopuses (Octopus bimaculoides) and cuttlefishes (Sepia pharaonis, S. officinalis) can conditionally discriminate. Animal Cognition, 10: 449–459.
Kamil, A. C. (1994). A synthetic approach to the study of animal intelligence. Behavioural Mechanisms in Evolutionary Ecology, 11–45.
Kamil, A. C. and Cheng, K. (2001). Way-finding and landmarks: The multiple-bearings hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Biology, 204: 103–113.
Kamil, A. C. and Jones, J. E. (1997). The seed-storing corvid Clark’s nutcracker learns geometric relationships among landmarks. Nature, 390: 276–279.
Kandel, E. R., 1976. Cellular basis of behavior: An introduction to behavioral neurobiology. New York, NY, USA: W. H. Freeman.
Kandel, E. R. and Schwartz, J. H. (1982). Molecular biology of learning: Modulation of transmitter release. Science, 218: 433–443.
Kandel, E. R. and Tauc, L. (1965). Heterosynaptic facilitation in neurones of the abdominal ganglion of Aplysia depilans. Journal of Physiology, 181: 1–27.
Karson, M. A., Boal, J. G. and Hanlon, R. T. (2003). Experimental evidence for spatial learning in cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117: 149–155.
Kier, W. M. (1987). The functional morphology of the tentacle musculature of Nautilus pompilius. In Nautilus: The biology and paleobiology of a living fossil. Saunders, W. B. and Landman, N. (Eds), pp. 257–269. New York, NY, USA: Springer.
Landman, N. H. and Cochran, J. K. (2010). Growth and longevity of Nautilus. In Nautilus: The biology and paleobiology of a living fossil. Saunders, W. B. and Landman, N. (Eds), pp. 401–420. New York, NY, USA: Springer.
Lucero, M. T., Horrigan, F. T. and Gilly, W. M. F. (1992). Electrical responses to chemical stimulation of squid olfactory receptor cells. Journal of Experimental Biology, 162: 231–249.
Lukowiak, K., Adatia, N., Krygier, D. and Syed, N. (2000). Operant conditioning in Lymnaea: Evidence for intermediate-and long-term memory. Learning and Memory, 7: 140–150.
Maddock, L. and Young, J. Z. (1987). Quantitative differences among the brains of cephalopods. Journal of Zoology, 212: 739–767.
Maldonado, H. (1965). The positive and negative learning process in Octopus vulgaris Lamarck. Influence of the vertical and median superior frontal lobes. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 51: 185–203.
Mather, J. A. (1991). Navigation by spatial memory and use of visual landmarks in octopuses. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 168: 491–497.
Mather, J. A. (1995). Cognition in cephalopods. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 24: 317–353.
Mather, J. A. (2008). To boldly go where no mollusc has gone before: Personality, play, thinking, and consciousness in cephalopods. American Malacological Bulletin, 24: 51–58.
McGaugh, J. L. (2000). Memory – a century of consolidation. Science, 287: 248–251.
Menzel, R., Leboulle, G. and Eisenhardt, D. (2006). Small brains, bright minds. Cell, 124: 237–239.
Messenger, J. B. (1973). Learning in the cuttlefish, Sepia. Animal Behaviour, 21: 801–826.
Messenger, J. B. (1991). Photoreception and vision in molluscs. Vision and Visual Dysfunction, 2: 364–397.
Milner, B., Squire, L. R. and Kandel, E. R. (1998). Cognitive neuroscience and the study of memory. Neuron, 20: 445–468.
Muntz, W. R. A. (1986). Short communications: The spectral sensitivity of Nautilus pompilius. Journal of Experimental Biology, 126: 513–517.
Muntz, W. R. A. (2010a). A possible function of the iris groove of Nautilus. In Nautilus: The biology and paleobiology of a living fossil, 2nd edn. Saunders, W. B. and Landman, N. (Eds), pp. 245–247. New York, NY, USA: Springer.
Muntz, W. R. A. (2010b). Visual behavior and visual sensitivity of Nautilus pompilius. In Nautilus: The biology and paleobiology of a living fossil, 2nd edn. Saunders, W. B. and Landman, N. (Eds), pp. 231–244. New York, NY, USA: Springer.
Muntz, W. R. A. and Raj, U. (1984). On the visual system of Nautilus pompilius. Journal of Experimental Biology, 109: 253–263.
Nixon, M. and Young, J. Z. (2003). The brains and lives of cephalopods. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
O’Keefe, J. and Nadel, L. (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press.
Packard, A. (1972). Cephalopods and fish: The limits of convergence. Biological Reviews, 47: 241–307.
Pavlov, I. (1927). Conditioned reflexes: An investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Pearce, J. M. (1987). A model for stimulus generalization in Pavlovian conditioning. Psychological Review, 94: 61–73.
Pearce, J. M. (1994). Similarity and discrimination: A selective review and a connectionist model. Psychological Review, 101: 587–607.
Pedreira, M. E. and Maldonado, H. (2003). Protein synthesis subserves reconsolidation or extinction depending on reminder duration. Neuron, 38: 863–869.
Poucet, B., Chapuis, N., Durup, M.and Thinus-Blanc, C. (1986). A study of exploratory behavior as an index of spatial knowledge in hamsters. Learning and Behavior, 14: 93–100.
Rescorla, R. A. and Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory, 2: 64–99.
Ruth, P., Schmidtberg, H., Westermann, B. and Schipp, R. (2002). The sensory epithelium of the tentacles and the rhinophore of Nautilus pompilius L. (Cephalopoda, Nautiloidea). Journal of Morphology, 251: 239–255.
Sanders, G. D. (1970). Long-term memory of a tactile discrimination in Octopus vulgaris and the effect of vertical lobe removal. Brain Research, 20: 59–73.
Sangha, S., Scheibenstock, A., McComb, C. and Lukowiak, K. (2003). Intermediate and long-term memories of associative learning are differentially affected by transcription versus translation blockers in Lymnaea. Journal of Experimental Biology, 206: 1605–1613.
Sasaki, T., Shigeno, S. and Tanabe, K. (2010). Anatomy of living Nautilus: Re-evaluation of primitiveness and comparison with Coleoidea. In Cephalopods – present and past. Tanabe, K., Shigeta, Y., Sasaki, T. and Hirano, H. (Eds), pp. 35–66. Tokyo, Japan: Tokai University Press.
Saunders, W. B. (1985). Studies of living Nautilus in Palau. National Geographic Society Reseach Reports, 18: 669–682.
Saunders, W. B. and Landman, N. H. (1987). Nautilus: The biology and paleobiology of a living fossil, reprint with additions. New York, NY, USA: Springer.
Shettleworth, S. J. and Sutton, J. E. (2005). Multiple systems for spatial learning: Dead reckoning and beacon homing in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 31: 125–141.
Shigeno, S., Sasaki, T., Moritaki, T., Kasugai, T., Vecchione, M. and Agata, K. (2008). Evolution of the cephalopod head complex by assembly of multiple molluscan body parts: Evidence from Nautilus embryonic development. Journal of Morphology, 269: 1–17.
Shigeno, S., Takenori, S. and Boletzky, S. v. (2010). The origins of cephalopod body plans: A geometrical and developmental basis for the evolution of vertebrate-like organ systems. Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory, 1: 23–34.
Shillito, E. E. (1963). Exploratory behaviour in the short-tailed vole Microtus agrestis. Behaviour, 21: 145–154.
Smith, B. H. (1997). An analysis of blocking in odorant mixtures: An increase but not a decrease in intensity of reinforcement produces unblocking. Behavioral Neuroscience, 11: 57–69.
Smith, B. H. and Cobey, S. (1994). The olfactory memory of the honeybee Apis mellifera. II. Blocking between odorants in binary mixtures. Journal of Experimental Biology, 195: 91–108.
Soucier, C. P. and Basil, J. A. (2008). Chambered Nautilus (Nautilus pompilius pompilius) responds to underwater vibrations. American Malacological Bulletin, 24: 3–11.
Stork, O. and Welzl, H. (1999). Memory formation and the regulation of gene expression. Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences, 55: 575–592.
Strugnell, J., Jackson, J., Drummond, A. J. and Cooper, A. (2006). Divergence time estimates for major cephalopod groups: Evidence from multiple genes. Cladistics, 22: 89–96.
Strugnell, J., Norman, M., Jackson, J., Drummond, A. J. and Cooper, A. (2005). Molecular phylogeny of coleoid cephalopods (Mollusca: Cephalopoda) using a multigene approach; the effect of data partitioning on resolving phylogenies in a Bayesian framework. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution, 37: 426–441.
Suboski, M. D., Muir, D. and Hall, D. (1993). Social learning in invertebrates. Science, 259: 1628–1629.
Sutherland, N. S. (1963). The shape-discrimination of stationary shapes by octopuses. American Journal of Psychology, 76: 177–190.
Teichert, C., Clarke, M. R. and Trueman, E. R. (1988). Main features of cephalopod evolution. In The Mollusca, Clarke, M. R. and Trueman, E. R. (Eds), pp. 11–79. San Diego, CA, USA: Academic Press.
Teichert, C. and Matsumoto, T. (2010). The ancestry of the genus Nautilus. In Nautilus: The biology and paleobiology of a living fossil, 2nd edn. Saunders, W. B. and Landman, N. (Eds), pp. 25–32. New York, NY, USA: Springer.
Teyke, T. (1985). Collision with and avoidance of obstacles by blind cave fish Anoptichthys jordani (Characidae). Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 157: 837–843.
Teyke, T. (1988). Flow field, swimming velocity and boundary layer: Parameters which affect the stimulus for the lateral line organ in blind fish. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 163: 53–61.
Teyke, T. (1989). Learning and remembering the environment in the blind cave fish Anoptichthys jordani. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 164, 655–662.
Thein, T., Westbrook, R. F. and Harris, J. A. (2008). How the associative strengths of stimuli combine in compound: Summation and overshadowing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 34: 155–166.
Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20: 410–433.
Walters, E. T. (1991). A functional, cellular, and evolutionary model of nociceptive plasticity in Aplysia. The Biological Bulletin, 180: 241–251.
Walters, E. T., Alizadeh, H. and Castro, G. A. (1991). Similar neuronal alterations induced by axonal injury and learning in Aplysia. Science, 253: 797–799.
Ward, P. D., Carlson, B., Weekly, M. and Brumbaugh, B. (1984). Remote telemetry of daily vertical and horizontal movement of Nautilus in Palau. Nature, 309: 248–250.
Ward, P. D. and Saunders, W. B. (1997). Allonautilus: A new genus of living nautiloid cephalopod and its bearing on phylogeny of the Nautilida. Journal of Paleontology, 71: 1054–1064.
Welker, W. I. and Welker, J. (1958). Reaction of fish (Eucinostomus gula) to environmental changes. Ecology, 39: 283–288.
Wells, M. J. (1964). Detour experiments with octopuses. Journal of Experimental Biology, 41: 621–642.
Wells, M. J. (1978). Octopus: Physiology and behaviour of an advanced invertebrate. London, UK: Chapman and Hall.
Westermann, B. and Beuerlein, K. (2005). Y-maze experiments on the chemotactic behaviour of the tetrabranchiate cephalopod. Marine Biology, 147: 145–151.
Woodhams, P. L. and Messenger, J. B. (1974). A note on the ultrastructure of the Octopus olfactory organ. Cell and Tissue Research, 152: 253–258.
Young, J. Z. (1960). The failures of discrimination learning following the removal of the vertical lobes in Octopus. Proceedings of the Royal Society. B: Biological Sciences, 153: 18–46.
Young, J. Z. (1961). Learning and discrimination in the octopus. Biological Reviews, 36: 32–95.
Young, J. Z. (1964). A model of the brain. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.
Young, J. Z. (1965a). The central nervous system of Nautilus. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, 249: 1–25.
Young, J. Z. (1965b). The Croonian lecture, 1965: The organization of a memory system. Proceedings of the Royal Society. B: Biological Sciences, 163: 285–320.
Young, J. Z. (1988). Evolution of the cephalopod brain. The Mollusca, 12: 215–228.
Young, J. Z. (1991). Computation in the learning system of cephalopods. The Biological Bulletin, 180: 200–208.
Young, J. Z. and Boycott, B. B. (1971). The anatomy of the nervous system of Octopus vulgaris. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.
Young, R. E. and Vecchione, M. (1996). Analysis of morphology to determine primary sister-taxon relationships within coleoid cephalopods. American Malacological Bulletin, 12: 91–112.
Young, R. E., Vecchione, M. and Donovan, D. T. (1998). The evolution of coleoid cephalopods and their present biodiversity and ecology. South African Journal of Marine Science, 20: 393–420.