Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-zzh7m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T17:38:16.232Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

II.G.10 - Game

from II.G - Important Foods from Animal Sources

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 March 2008

Kenneth F. Kiple
Affiliation:
Bowling Green State University, Ohio
Get access

Summary

Evolutionary Underpinnings

Game (defined here as the meat of wild mammals, birds, and terrestrial reptiles) has been an important component of the human diet since earliest times. A signal difference between the digestive system of humans and those of their closest primate relatives is the modification of the guts of the former for the processing of a diet containing substantial quantities of meat (Chivers 1992). Until the last few thousand years, all meat-producing animals were wild. The continuing cultural significance of hunting as an activity, and game as an element of the diet, must be seen in the light of the age-old importance of game animals in human biological and cultural evolution.

Although little doubt exists that early hominids consumed appreciable quantities of game, lively debate lingers over whether our remote ancestors were primarily hunters or scavengers (e.g., Shipman 1986; Binford 1988). Nevertheless, there is abundant evidence that by the time of the appearance of early modern humans – between 50,000 and 100,000 years ago – hunting was one of humankind’s major activities. It may have been so for uncounted previous millennia.

A striking sexual division of subsistence labor characterizes most contemporary hunting and gathering peoples, with men hunting and women gathering (but see later discussion).Typically, gathered foods more or less reliably provide the bulk of the calories in the diet, mainly as carbohydrates, whereas game (and/or fish) contributes the undependable and sometimes limiting protein fraction. It has been proposed (Isaac 1978; Isaac and Crader 1981) that this bifurcation in foraging tasks and products is ancient and is implicated in the coevolution of food sharing, tool use, paternal investment in offspring, and related features of the unique human constellation of social behaviors.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2000

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Altman, J. 1987. Hunter-gatherers today: An aboriginal economy in north Australia. Canberra, Australia.Google Scholar
Binford, L. R. 1988. The hunting hypothesis, archaeological methods, and the past. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 30.Google Scholar
Chivers, D. J. 1992. Diets and guts. In The Cambridge encyclopedia of human evolution, ed. Jones, Steve, Martin, Robert, and Pilbeam, David. Cambridge.Google Scholar
Estioko-Griffin, A., and Griffin, P. B.. 1981. Woman the hunter: The Agta. In Woman the gatherer, ed. Dahlberg, Frances. New Haven.Google Scholar
Griffin, P. B., and Griffin, M. B.. 1992. Fathers and childcare among the Cagayan Agta. In Father–child relations: Cultural and biosocial relations, ed. Hewlett, Barry. Hawthorne, N.Y.Google Scholar
Halleux, G. 1988. Charlemagne à la chasse. Plaisirs de la chasse 437.Google Scholar
Hawkes, K. 1990. Why do men hunt? Some benefits for risky choices. In Risk and uncertainty in tribal and peasant economies, ed. Cashdan, E.. Boulder, Colo.Google Scholar
Hawkes, K., O'Connell, J., and Blurton-Jones, N.. 1991. Hunting income patterns among the Hadza: Big game, common goods, foraging goals and the evolution of the human diet. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B334.Google Scholar
Headland, T. 1987. The wild yam question: How well could independent hunter-gatherers live in a tropical rain forest ecosystem?Human Ecology 15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Headland, T., and Bailey, R.. 1991. Introduction: Have hunter-gatherers ever lived in tropical rain forest independently of agriculture?Human Ecology 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, K., and Hawkes, K.. 1983. Neotropical hunting among the Aché of Eastern Paraguay. In Adaptive responses of native Amazonians, ed. Hames, Ray and Vickers, William. New York.Google Scholar
Hurber, P. 1980. The Anggor bowman: Ritual and society in Melanesia. American Ethnologist 7.Google Scholar
Isaac, G. L. 1978. The food sharing behavior of protohuman hominids. Scientific American 238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isaac, G. L., and Crader, D. C.. 1981. To what extent were early hominids carnivorous? An archaeological perspective. In Omnivorous primates: Gathering and hunting in human evolution, ed. Harding, R. S. O. and Teleki, G.. New York.Google Scholar
Jochim, M. 1981. Strategies for survival: Cultural behavior in an ecological context. New York.Google Scholar
Kaplan, H., and Hill, K.. 1992. The evolutionary ecology of food acquisition. In Evolutionary ecology and human behavior, ed. Smith, Eric and Winterhalder, Bruce. Hawthorne, N.Y.Google Scholar
Kent, S. 1989. Cross-cultural conceptions of farmers as hunters and the value of meat. In Farmers as hunters: The implications of sedentism, ed. Kent, S.. Cambridge and New York.Google Scholar
Kuchikura, Y. 1988. Efficiency and focus of blowpipe hunting among Semaq Beri hunter-gatherers of peninsular Malaysia. Human Ecology 16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, R. B. 1968. What hunters do for a living, or, how to make out on scarce resources. In Man the hunter, ed. Lee, R. B. and DeVore, I.. Chicago.Google Scholar
Marshall, L. 1965. The !Kung Bushmen of the Kalahari Desert. In Peoples of Africa, ed. Gibbs, J. L. Jr.. Prospect Heights, Ill.Google Scholar
Polo, M. 1927. The Book of Ser Marco Polo the Venetian, concerning the kingdoms and marvels of the East, ed. Parks, G.. New York.Google Scholar
Roaf, M. 1990. Cultural atlas of Mesopotamia and the ancient Near East. Oxford.Google Scholar
Schalk, R. 1977. The structure of an anadromous fish resource. In For theory building in archaeology, ed. Binford, L. R.. New York.Google Scholar
Shipman, P. 1986. Scavenging or hunting in early hominids: Theoretical framework and tests. American Anthropologist 88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephens, D. W. and Krebs, J. R.. 1986. Foraging theory. Princeton, N.J.Google Scholar
Trigger, B. 1965. History and settlement in lower Nubia. Yale University Publications in Anthropology, No. 69. New Haven, Conn.Google Scholar
Welch, P. 1991. Moundville’s economy. Tuscaloosa, Ala.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×