Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-ph5wq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T06:11:28.024Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

13 - Distinguishing Two Processes of Self-reflection

from Part III - The Agent Rises a Reflective Self: Education and Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 June 2018

Alberto Rosa
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid
Jaan Valsiner
Affiliation:
Aalborg University, Denmark
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Bain, R. (1936). The self-and-other words of a child. The American Journal of Sociology, 41(6), 767775.Google Scholar
Bakhtin, M. (1923/1990). Author and hero in aesthetic activity (trans. by V. Liapunov). In Holquist, M. & Liapunov, V. (Eds.), Art and Answerability: Early Philosophical Essays by M. M. Bakhtin (vol. 9, pp. 4256). Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1906). Mental Development in the Child and the Race. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Bauer, M. W. & Gaskell, G. (1999). Towards a paradigm for research on social representations. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 29, 163186.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1986). Actual Minds, Possible Worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Cole, M. (1996). Cultural Psychology: A Once and Future Discipline. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human Nature and the Social Order. New York: Charles Scribner's Sons.Google Scholar
Cooley, C. H. (1908). A study of the early use of the self words by a child. Psychological Review, 15, 339357.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1896). The reflex arc concept in psychology. Psychological Review, 3(July), 357370.Google Scholar
Doise, W. & Mugny, G. (1984). The Social Development of the Intellect. Oxford: Pergammon.Google Scholar
Engeström, Y. (1987). Learning by Expanding: An Activity-Theoretical Approach to Developmental Research. Helsinki: Orienta Konsultit.Google Scholar
Engeström, Y., Brown, K., Christopher, L. C., & Gregory, J. (1997). Coordination, cooperation and communication in the courts: Expansive transitions in legal work. In Cole, M., Engestrom, Y., & Vasquez, O. (Eds.), Mind, Culture and Activity: Seminal Papers from the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition (pp. 369385). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Farr, R. M. (1997). The significance of the skin as a natural boundary in the sub-division of psychology. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 27, 305323.Google Scholar
Gergely, G. & Watson, J. S. (1996). The social biofeedback theory of parental affect-mirroring: The development of emotional self-awareness and self-control in infancy. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 77, 11811212.Google Scholar
Gillespie, A. (2003). Surplus & supplementarity: Moving between the dimensions of otherness. Culture & Psychology, 9, 209220.Google Scholar
Gillespie, A. (2005). G. H. Mead: Theorist of the social act. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 35, 1939.Google Scholar
Gillespie, A. (2006a). Becoming Other to Oneself: A Meadian Study of Culture Tourism in Ladakh. Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Gillespie, A. (2006b). Tourist photography and the reverse gaze. Ethos, 34, 343366.Google Scholar
Gillespie, A. (2007). Collapsing self/other positions: Identification through differentiation. British Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 579595.Google Scholar
Gillespie, A. (2010). The intersubjective nature of symbols. In Wagoner, B. (Ed.), Symbolic Transformations: The Mind in Movement through Culture and Society (pp. 2337). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Gillespie, A., & Martin, J. (2013). Position exchange theory: A socio-material basis for discursive and psychological positioning. New Ideas in Psychology, 32, 7379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillespie, A. & Zittoun, T. (2013). Meaning making in motion: Bodies and minds moving through institutional and semiotic structures. Culture & Psychology, 19, 518532.Google Scholar
Harré, R., & Van Langenhove, L. (1991). Varieties of positioning. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 21(4).Google Scholar
Hermans, H. J. M. (2001). The dialogical self: Toward a theory of personal and cultural positioning. Culture & Psychology, 7, 243281.Google Scholar
Hobson, P. (2002). The Cradle of Thought. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Ichheiser, G. (1949). Misunderstandings in human relations: A study in false social perception. American Journal of Sociology, 55, 172.Google Scholar
Josephs, I. E. (2002). “The Hopi in me”: The construction of a voice in the dialogical self from a cultural psychological perspective. Theory & Psychology, 12, 161173.Google Scholar
Jovchelovitch, S. (1995). Social representations in and of the public sphere: Towards a theoretical articulation. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 25, 81102.Google Scholar
Lacan, J. (1949/1977). The mirror stage as formative of the function of the I as revealed in psychoanalytic theory. In Écrits: A Selection (pp. 19). London: Tavistock.Google Scholar
Lawrence, J. A. & Valsiner, J. (1993). Conceptual roots of internalization: From transmission to transformation. Human Development, 36, 150167.Google Scholar
Lundgren, D. C. (2004). Social feedback and self-appraisals: Current status of the Mead-Cooley hypothesis. Symbolic Interaction, 27, 267286.Google Scholar
Marková, I. (1982). Paradigms, Thought and Language. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Matusov, E. (1998). When solo activity is not privileged: Participation and internalization models of development. Human Development, 41, 326354.Google Scholar
Mead, G. H. (1910). Social consciousness and the consciousness of meaning. Psychological Bulletin, 6, 401408.Google Scholar
Mead, G. H. (1912). The mechanism of social consciousness. The Journal of Philosophy, Psychology and Scientific Methods, 9, 401406.Google Scholar
Mead, G. H. (1922). A behavioristic account of the significant symbol. Journal of Philosophy, 19, 157163.Google Scholar
Moscovici, S. (1984). The phenomenon of social representations. In Farr, R. & Moscovici, S. (Eds.), Social Representations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (2000). Narrative, time and the emergence of the encultured self. Culture & Psychology, 6, 183196.Google Scholar
Pavlov, I. P. (1951). Psychopathology and Psychiatry: Selected Works (trans. by D. Myshne & S. Belsky). Moscow: Foreign Languages Publishing House.Google Scholar
Peirce, C. S. (1878/1998). Charles S. Peirce: The Essential Writings (ed. by Moore, Edward C.). New York: Prometheus Books.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget's theory. In Mussen, P. H. (Ed.), Carmichael's Manual of Child Psychology (3rd edn., pp. 703732). New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Potter, J. & Wetherell, M. (1987). Discourse and Social Psychology. London: SAGE.Google Scholar
Prebensen, N. K., Larsen, S., & Abelsen, B. (2003). I'm not a typical tourist: German tourists’ self-perception, activities and motivations. Journal of Travel Research, 41, 416420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Psaltis, C. & Duveen, G. (2006). Social relations and cognitive development: The influence of conversation type and representations of gender. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 407430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rommetveit, R. (1974). On Message Structure: A Framework for the Study of Language and Communication. London: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Shrauger, J. S. & Schoeneman, T. J. (1979). Symbolic interactionist view of self-concept. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 549573.Google Scholar
Sigel, I. E. (2002). The psychological distancing model: A study of the socialization of cognition. Culture & Psychology, 8, 189214.Google Scholar
Smith, A. (1759/1982). The Theory of Moral Sentiments (ed. by Raphael, D. D. & Macfie, A. L.). Indianapolis, IN: Liberty Fund.Google Scholar
Tolman, C. W. & Piekkola, B. (1989). John Dewey and dialectical materialism: Anticipations of activity theory in the critique of the reflex arc concept. Activity Theory, 1, 4346.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675691.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (1998). The Guided Mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2001). Process structure of semiotic mediation in human development. Human Development, 44, 8497.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1925/1999). Consciousness as a problem in the psychology of behavior. In Veresov, N. N. (Ed.), Undiscovered Vygotsky: Etudes on the Pre-History of Cultural-Historical Psychology. Bern: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1989). Concrete human psychology. Soviet Psychology, 27, 5377.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1997). The Collected Works of L. S. Vygotsky (vol. 4, ed. by R. W. Rieber, trans. by M. J. Hall). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. & Luria, A. (1930/1994). Tool and symbol in child development. In Van de Veer, R. & Valsiner, J. (Eds.), The Vygotsky Reader (pp. 99174). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wagner, W., Duveen, G., Themel, M., & Verma, J. (1999). The modernization of tradition: Thinking about madness in Patna, India. Culture & Psychology, 5, 413445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wertsch, J. V. & Stone, A. (1985). The concept of internalization in Vygotsky's account of the genesis of higher mental functions. In Wertsch, J. V. (Ed.), Culture, Communication and Cognition: Vygotskian Perspectives (pp. 162179). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. (2005). The imitative correspondence problem: Solved or sidestepped? In Hurley, S. & Chater, N. (Eds.), Perspectives on Imitation: Mechanisms of Imitation and Imitation in Animals (vol. 1, pp. 220222). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Winnicott, D. W. (1971). Playing and Reality. London: Tavistock.Google Scholar
Zittoun, T., Duveen, G., Gillespie, A., Ivinson, G., & Psaltis, C. (2003). The use of symbolic resources in developmental transitions. Culture & Psychology, 9, 415448.Google Scholar

Further Reading

Perret-Clermont, A. N. (1980). Social Interaction and Cognitive Development in Children. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×