Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 9
  • Print publication year: 2008
  • Online publication date: June 2012

Chapter 12 - Spatial Cognition

from Part III - Empirical Developments

References

Alibali, M. W., Flevares, L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1997). Assessing knowledge conveyed in gesture: Do teachers have the upper hand? Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 183–193.
Andersen, E. S. (1978). Lexical universals of body-part terminology. In J. H. Greenberg (Ed.), Universals of human language (pp. 335–368). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Aziz-Zadeh, L., Maeda, F., Zaidel, E., Mazziotta, J., & Iacoboni, M. (2002). Lateralization in motor facilitation during action observation: A TMS study. Experimental Brain Research, 144, 127–131.
Beniger, J. R., & Robyn, D. L. (1978). Quantitative graphics in statistics. American Statistician, 32, 1–11.
Bower, G. H., Black, J. B., & Turner, T. J. (1979). Scripts in memory for text. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 179–220.
Brown, C. H. (1976). General principles of human anatomical partonomy and speculations on the growth of partonomic nomenclature. American Ethnologist, 3, 400–424.
Bryant, D. J., & Tversky, B. (1999). Mental representations of spatial relations from diagrams and models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 25, 137–156.
Bryant, D. J., Tversky, B., & Franklin, N. (1992). Internal and external spatial frameworks for representing described scenes. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 74–98.
Bryant, D. J., Tversky, B., & Lanca, M. (2001). Retrieving spatial relations from observation and memory. In E. van der Zee & U. Nikanne (Eds.), Conceptual structure and its interfaces with other modules of representation (pp. 116–139). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Carswell, C. M., & Wickens, C. D. (1988). Comparative graphics: History and applications of perceptual integrality theory and the proximity compatibility hypothesis (Tech. Rep.). Urbana: Institute of Aviation, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.
Chatterjee, S. H., Freyd, J. J., & Shiffrar, M. (1996). Configural processing in the perception of apparent biological motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 916–929.
Clark, H. H. (1973). Space, time, semantics, and the child. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (pp. 27–63). New York: Academic Press.
Cross, E. S., Hamilton, A. F., & Grafton, S. T. (2006). Building a motor simulation de novo: Observation of dance by dancers. Neuroimage, 31(3), 1257–1267.
Decety, J., & Grèzes, J. (2006). The power of simulation: Imagining one's own and other's behavior. Brain Research, 1079, 4–14.
Emmorey, K., Tversky, B., & Taylor, H. A. (2000). Using space to describe space: Perspective in speech, sign, and gesture. Journal of Spatial Cognition and Computation, 2, 157–180.
Engelkamp, J. (1998). Memory for actions. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Franklin, N., & Tversky, B. (1990). Searching imagined environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 63–76.
Franklin, N., Tversky, B., & Coon, V. (1992). Switching points of view in spatial mental models acquired from text. Memory and Cognition, 20, 507–518.
Gallese, V. (2005). “Being like me”: Self-other identity, mirror neurons, and empathy. In S. Hurley & N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectives on imitation: From neuroscience to social science (Vol. 1, pp. 108–118). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gelb, I. (1963). A study of writing (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). Hearing gesture: How our hands help us think. Cambridge, MA: Bel-knap Press.
Goldman, A. (2005). Imitation, mindreading, and simulation. In S. Hurley & N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectives on imitation: From neuroscience to social science (Vol. 2, pp. 79–93). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Goodnow, J. (1977). Children's drawing. London: Open Books.
Gross, C. G., & Graziano, M. S. A. (1995). Multiple representations of space in the brain. Neuroscientist, 1, 43–50.
Hegarty, M., Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (1990). Diagrams in the comprehension of scientific text. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research. New York: Longman.
Heiser, J., & Tversky, B. (2006). Arrows in comprehending and producing mechanical diagrams. Cognitive Science, 30, 581–592.
Hirtle, S., & Jonides, J. (1985). Hierarachies in cognitive maps. Memory and Cognition, 13, 208–217.
Hoffman, D. D., & Richards, W. A. (1984). Parts of recognition. Cognition, 18, 65–96.
Holyoak, K. J., & Mah, W. A. (1982). Cognitive reference points in judgements of symbolic magnitude. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 328–352.
Iverson, J., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1997). What's communication got to do with it? Gesture in children blind from birth. Developmental Psychology, 33, 453–467.
Kellogg, R. (1969). Analyzing children's art. Palo Alto, CA: National Press.
Kelly, S. D., & Church, B. (1998). A comparison between children's and adults’ ability to detect conceptual information conveyed through representational gestures. Child Development, 69, 85–93.
Kessell, A., & Tversky, B. (2005). Gestures for thinking and explaining. In B. G. Bara, L. W. Barsalou, & M. Bucciarelli (Eds.), Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society Meetings (p. 2498). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Koski, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M.-C., Woods, R. P., & Mazziotta, J. C. (2003). Modulation of cortical activity during different imitative behaviours. Journal of Neurophysiology, 89, 460–471.
Kosslyn, S. M. (1980). Image and mind. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Krauss, R. M., Chen, Y., & Chawla, P. (1996). Nonverbal behavior and nonverbal communication: What do conversational hand gestures tell us? In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 389–450). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Krauss, R. M., Chen, Y., & Gottesman, R. (2000). Lexical gestures and lexical access: A process model. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 261–284). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Larkin, J. H., & Simon, H. A. (1987). Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth ten thousand words. Cognitive Science, 11, 65–99.
McNeill, D. (2005). Gesture and thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
McNeill, D., Cassell, J., & McCullough, K. E. (1994). Communicative effects of speech-mismatched gestures. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 27, 223–237.
Morrison, J. B., & Tversky, B. (2005). Bodies and their parts. Memory and Cognition, 33, 696–709.
Nickerson, J., Tversky, B., Corter, J., Zahner, D., & Rho, Y.-J. (in press). Diagrams as a tool in the design of information systems. In J. S. Gero & A. Goel (Eds.), Design Computing and Cognition ’08. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.
Parsons, L. M. (1987). Imagined spatial transformation of one's body. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116, 172–191.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1981) The imagery debate: Analogue media versus tacit knowledge. Psychological Review, 88, 16–45.
Quattrone, G. A. (1986). On the perception of a group's variability. In S. Worchel & W. Austin (Eds.), The psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 25–48). New York: Nelson-Hall.
Reed, C. L. (2002). Body schemas. In A. Meltzoff & W. Prinz (Eds.), The imitative mind (pp. 233–243). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Reed, C. L., & Farah, M. J. (1995). The psychological reality of the body schema: A test with normal participants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 334–343.
Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (1999). Resonance behaviors and mirror neurons. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 137, 85–100.
Rock, I. (1973). Orientation and form. New York: Academic Press.
Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 27–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Ruby, P., & Decety, J. (2001). Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: A PET investigation of agency. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 546–550.
Sadalla, E. K., Burroughs, W. J., & Staplin, L. I. (1980). Reference points in spatial cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 516–528.
Sadalla, E. K., & Staplin, L. J. (1980a). An information storage model for distance cognition. Environment and Behavior, 12, 183–193.
Sadalla, E. K., & Staplin, L. J. (1980b). The perception of traversed distance: Intersections. Environment and Behavior, 12, 167–182.
Schiano, D., & Tversky, B. (1992). Structure and strategy in viewing simple graphs. Memory and Cognition, 20, 12–20.
Shepard, R. N. (1978). Externalization of mental images and the act of creation. In B. S. Randhawa & B. F. Coffman (Eds.), Visual learning, thinking, and communication (pp. 133–189). London: Academic Press.
Shepard, R. N. (1984). Ecological constraints on internal representations: Resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. Psychological Review, 91, 417–447.
Shepard, R. N., & Cooper, L. (1982). Mental images and their transformation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Shepard, R. N., & Podgorny, P. (1978). Cognitive processes that resemble perceptual processes. In W. K. Estes (Ed.), Handbook of learning and cognitive processes (Vol. 5, pp. 189–237). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Shiffrar, M., & Freyd, J. J. (1993). Timing and apparent motion path choice with human body photographs. Psychological Science, 4, 379–384.
Talmy, L. (1983). How language structures space. In H. L. Pick, Jr. & L. P. Acredolo (Eds.), Spatial orientation: Theory, research, and application (pp. 225–282). New York: Plenum Press.
Taylor, H. A., & Tversky, B. (1992). Descriptions and depictions of environments. Memory and Cognition, 20, 483–496.
Taylor, H. A., & Tversky, B. (1996). Perspective in spatial descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 371–391.
Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological Review, 110, 403–421.
Tufte, E. R. (1983). The visual display of quantitative information. Cheshire, CT: Graphics Press.
Tversky, B. (1981). Distortions in memory for maps. Cognitive Psychology, 13, 407–433.
Tversky, B. (1989). Parts, partonomies, and taxonomies. Developmental Psychology, 25, 983–995.
Tversky, B. (1993). Cognitive maps, cognitive collages, and spatial mental models. In A. U. Frank & I. Campari (Eds.), Spatial information theory: A theoretical basis for GIS (pp. 14–24). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Tversky, B. (1995). Cognitive origins of graphic conventions. In F. T. Marchese (Ed.), Understanding images (pp. 29–53). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Tversky, B. (2000). Remembering space. In E. Tulving & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Handbook of memory (pp. 363–378). New York: Oxford University Press.
Tversky, B. (2001). Spatial schemas in depictions. In M. Gattis (Ed.), Spatial schemas and abstract thought (pp. 79–111). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Tversky, B. (2003). Navigating by mind and by body. In C. Freksa, W. Brauer, C. Habel, & K. F. Wender (Eds.), Spatial cognition III: Routes and navigation, human memory and learning, spatial representation and spatial reasoning (pp. 1–10). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Tversky, B. (2005a). Functional significance of visuospatial representations. In P. Shah & A. Miyake (Eds.), Handbook of higher-level visuospatial thinking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Tversky, B. (2005b). How to get around by mind and body: Spatial thought, spatial action. In A. Zilhao (Ed.), Cognition, evolution, and rationality: A cognitive science for the twenty-first century. London: Routledge.
Tversky, B. (2005c). La cognition spatiale: Incarnée et desincarnée. In A. Berthoz & R. Recht (Eds.), Les espaces de l’homme (pp. 161–184). Paris: Odile Jacob.
Tversky, B. (2005d). Visuospatial reasoning. In K. Holyoak & R. Morrison (Eds.), Handbook of reasoning (pp. 209–249). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Tversky, B., & Gati, I. (1978). Studies of similarity. In E. Rosch & B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 79–98). New York: Wiley.
Tversky, B., & Hemenway, K. (1983). Categories of scenes. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 121–149.
Tversky, B., & Hemenway, K. (1984). Objects, parts, and categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 169–193.
Tversky, B., Kim, J., & Cohen, A. (1999). Mental models of spatial relations and transformations from language. In C. Habel & G. Rickheit (Eds.), Mental models in discourse processing and reasoning (pp. 239–258). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Tversky, B., Kugelmass, S., & Winter, A. (1991). Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 515–557.
Tversky, B., & Lee, P. U. (1998). How space structures language. In C. Freksa, C. Habel, & K. F. Wender (Eds.), Spatial cognition: An interdisciplinary approach to representation and processing of spatial knowledge (pp. 157–175). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Tversky, B., & Lee, P. U. (1999). Pictorial and verbal tools for conveying routes. In C. Freksa & D. M. Mark (Eds.), Spatial information theory: Cognitive and computational foundations of geographic information science (pp. 51–64). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Tversky, B., Morrison, J. B., & Zacks, J. (2002). On bodies and events. In A. Meltzoff & W. Prinz (Eds.), The imitative mind: Development, evolution and brain bases (pp. 221–232). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Tversky, B., & Schiano, D. (1989). Perceptual and conceptual factors in distortions in memory for maps and graphs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 387–398.
Tversky, B., Zacks, J. M., & Hard, B. M. (2008). The structure of experience. In T. Shipley & J. M. Zacks (Eds.), Understanding events (pp. 436–464). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Tversky, B., Zacks, J., Lee, P. U., & Heiser, J. (2000). Lines, blobs, crosses, and arrows: Diagrammatic communication with schematic figures. In M. Anderson, P. Cheng, & V. Haarslev (Eds.), Theory and application of diagrams (pp. 221–230). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Tversky, B., Zacks, J. M., Morrison, J. B., & Hard, B. M. (in press). Talking about events. In E. Pederson, J. Bohnemeyer, & R. Tomlin (Eds.), Event representation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Valenzeno, L., Alibali, M. W., & Klatzky, R. (2003). Teachers’ gestures facilitate students’ learning: A lesson in symmetry. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28, 187–204.
Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Wraga, M., Creem, S. H., & Proffitt, D. R. (2000). Updating displays after imagined object and viewer rotations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 151–168.
Zacks, J. M., Mires, J., Tversky, B., & Hazeltine, E. (2000). Mental spatial transformations of objects and perspective. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 2, 315–332.
Zacks, J., & Tversky, B. (1999). Bars and lines: A study of graphic communication. Memory and Cognition, 27, 1073–1079.
Zacks, J. M., & Tversky, B. (2001). Event structure in perception and conception. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 3–21.
Zacks, J., Tversky, B., & Iyer, G. (2001). Perceiving, remembering, and communicating structure in events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 29–58.
Zacks, J. M., Vettel, J. M., & Michelon, P. (2003). Imagined viewer and object rotations dissociated with event-related fMRI. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 1002–1018.