Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T17:15:48.077Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - Syntactic processing

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 May 2016

Vivian Cook
Affiliation:
University of Newcastle upon Tyne
Li Wei
Affiliation:
Institute of Education, University College London
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baddeley, A. 2003. ‘Working memory and language: an overview’, Journal of Communication Disorders 36, 189208.Google Scholar
Blumenfeld, H. K. and Marian, V. 2013. ‘Cognitive control and parallel language activation during word recognition in bilinguals’, Journal of Cognitive Psychology 25 (5), 547567.Google Scholar
Boland, J. E., Tanenhaus, M. K., Garnsey, S. M. and Carlson, G. N. 1995. ‘Verb argument structure in parsing and interpretation: evidence from wh-questions’, Journal of Memory and Language 34, 774806.Google Scholar
Chafe, W. L. 1976. ‘Givenness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics, and points of view’, in Li, C. N. (ed.), Subject and Topic, pp. 2555. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1973. ‘Conditions on transformations’, in Anderson, S. R. and Kiparsky, P. (eds), A Festschrift for Morris Halle, pp. 232286. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. and Felser, C. 2006. ‘Grammatical processing in language learners’, Applied Psycholinguistics 27, 342.Google Scholar
Clifton, C. Jr 1993. ‘Thematic roles in sentence parsing’, Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 47, 222246.Google Scholar
Costa, A. Caramazza, A. and Sebastián-Gallés, N. 2000. ‘The cognate facilitation effect: implications for models of lexical access’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 26, 12831296.Google Scholar
Cunnings, I., Batterham, C., Felser, C. and Clahsen, H. 2010. ‘Constraints on L2 learners’ processing of wh-dependencies: evidence from eye movements’, in VanPatten, B. and Jegerski, J. (eds), Research in Second Language Processing and Parsing, pp. 87110. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
De Bot, K. 2004. ‘The multilingual lexicon: modeling selection and control’, International Journal of Multilingualism 1, 1732.Google Scholar
De Groot, A. M. B. 2011. Language and Cognition in Bilinguals and Multilinguals: An Introduction. New York and Hove, East Sussex: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
De Vincenzi, M. 1991. Syntactic Parsing Strategies in Italian. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Dekydtspotter, L., Schwartz, B. and Sprouse, R. A. 2006. ‘The comparative fallacy in L2 processing research’, in O’Brien, M. G., Shea, C. and Archibald, J. (eds), Proceedings from the 8th Generative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition Conference (GASLA 2006), pp. 3340. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Dell, G. S., McKoon, G. and Ratcliff, R. 1983. ‘The activation of antecedent information during the processing of anaphoric reference in reading’, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 22, 121132.Google Scholar
Dijkstra, T. and van Heuven, W. J. B. 1998. ‘The BIA-model and bilingual word recognition’, in Grainger, J. and Jacobs, A. M. (eds), Localist Connectionist Approaches to Human Cognition, pp. 189225. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dornič, S. 1980. ‘Language dominance, spare capacity and perceived effort in bilinguals’, Ergonomics 23, 369377.Google Scholar
Dussias, P. E. and Cramer Scaltz, T. R. 2008. ‘SpanishEnglish L2 speakers’ use of subcategorization bias information in the resolution of temporary ambiguity during second language reading’, Acta Psychologica 128, 501513.Google Scholar
Dussias, P. E. and Piñar, P. 2010. ‘Effects of reading span and plausibility in the reanalysis of wh-gaps by ChineseEnglish L2 speakers’, Second Language Research 26, 443472.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C. 1996. ‘Phonological memory, chunking and points of order’, Studies in Second Language Acquisition 18, 91126.Google Scholar
Felser, C. and Roberts, L. 2007. ‘Processing wh-dependencies in a second language: a cross-modal priming study’, Second Language Research 23, 936.Google Scholar
Felser, C., Sato, M. and Bertenshaw, N. 2009. ‘The on-line application of binding Principle A in English as a second language’, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 12, 485502.Google Scholar
Fender, M. 2001. ‘A review of L1 and L2/ESL word integration skills and the nature of L2/ESL word integration development involved in lower-level text processing’, Language Learning 51, 319396.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. 1983. The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Frazier, L. 1978. On Comprehending Sentences: Syntactic Parsing Strategies. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Connecticut.Google Scholar
Frazier, L. and Flores d’Arcais, G. B. 1989. ‘Filler-driven parsing: a study of gap filling in Dutch’, Journal of Memory and Language 28, 331344.Google Scholar
Frenck-Mestre, C. 2002. ‘An on-line look at sentence processing in the second language’, in Heredia, R. and Altarriba, J. (eds), Bilingual Sentence Processing, pp. 217236. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Frenck-Mestre, C. 2005. ‘Ambiguities and anomalies: what can eye movements and event-related potentials reveal about second language processing?’, in Kroll, J. F. and de Groot, A. M. B. (eds), Handbook of Bilingualism, pp. 268284. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Frenck-Mestre, C. and Pynte, J. 1997. ‘Syntactic ambiguity resolution while reading in second and native languages’, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, section A: Human Experimental Psychology 50A, 119148.Google Scholar
Gibson, E., Pearlmutter, N., Canseco-Gonzalez, E. and Hickok, G. 1996. ‘Recency preference in the human sentence processing mechanism’, Cognition 59, 2139Google Scholar
Gibson, E. and Warren, T. 2004. ‘Reading-time evidence for intermediate linguistic structure in long-distance dependencies’, Syntax 7, 5578.Google Scholar
Gordon, P. C., Grosz, B. J. and Gilliom, L. A. 1993. ‘Pronouns, names and the centering of attention in discourse’, Cognitive Science 17 (3), 311348.Google Scholar
Gorrell, P. 1995. Syntax and Parsing. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Harrington, M. and Sawyer, M. 1992. ‘L2 working memory and L2 reading skill’, Studies in Second Language Acquisition 14, 2538.Google Scholar
Havik, E., Roberts, L., van Hout, R., Schreuder, R. and Haverkort, M. 2009. ‘Processing subject–object ambiguities in the L2: a self-paced reading study with German L2 learners of Dutch’, Language Learning 59, 73112.Google Scholar
Herdina, P. and Jessner, U. 2002. A Dynamic Model of Multilingualism: Perspectives of Change in Psycholinguistics. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Hoover, M. L. and Dwivedi, V. D. 1998. ‘Syntactic processing in skilled bilinguals’, Language Learning 48, 129.Google Scholar
Hopp, H. 2006. ‘Syntactic features and reanalysis in near-native processing’, Second Language Research 22, 369397.Google Scholar
Hopp, H. 2010. ‘Ultimate attainment in L2 inflectional morphology: performance similarities between non-native and native speakers’, Lingua 120, 901931.Google Scholar
Jackson, C. N. 2008. ‘Proficiency level and the interaction of lexical and morphosyntactic information during L2 sentence processing’, Language Learning 58, 875909.Google Scholar
Jackson, C. N. and Roberts, L. 2010. ‘Animacy affects the processing of subject–object ambiguities in L2 processing: evidence from self-paced reading with German L2 learners of Dutch’, Applied Psycholinguistics 31, 671691.Google Scholar
Jackson, C. N. and van Hell, J. G. 2011. ‘The effects of L2 proficiency level on the processing of wh-questions among Dutch second language speakers of English‘, International Review of Applied Linguistics 49, 195219.Google Scholar
Jiang, N. 2004. ‘Morphological insensitivity in second language processing’, Applied Psycholinguistics 25, 603634.Google Scholar
Jiang, N. 2007. ‘Selective integration of linguistic knowledge in adult second language learning’, Language Learning 57, 133.Google Scholar
Juffs, A. 2004. ‘Representation, processing, and working memory in a second language’, Transactions of the Philological Society 102, 199225.Google Scholar
Juffs, A. 2005. ‘The influence of first language on the processing of wh-movement in English as a second language’, Second Language Research 21, 121151.Google Scholar
Juffs, A. and Harrington, M. 1995. ‘Parsing effects in second language sentence processing: Subject and object asymmetries in wh-extraction’, Studies in Second Language Acquisition 17, 483516.Google Scholar
Juffs, A. and Harrington, M. 1996. ‘Garden path sentences and error data in second language processing research’, Language Learning 46, 286324.Google Scholar
Juffs, A. and Harrington, M. 2011. ‘Aspects of working memory in second language learning and teaching’, Language Teaching 44, 137166.Google Scholar
Kidd, E. 2004. ‘Grammars, parsers, and language acquisition’, Journal of Child Language 31, 480483.Google Scholar
Kilborn, K. 1992. ‘On-line integration of grammatical information in a second language’, in Harris, R. (ed.), Cognitive Processing in Bilinguals, pp. 337350. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Kluender, R. 2004. ‘Are subject islands subject to a processing account?’, in Chand, V., Kelleher, A., Rodríguez, A. J. and Schmeiser, B. (eds), Proceedings of the 23rd WCCFL. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Lenneberg, E. 1967. Biological Foundations of Language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Marian, V. and Spivey, M. 2003. ‘Competing activation in bilingual language processing: within- and between-language competition’, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 6, 97115.Google Scholar
Marinis, T., Roberts, L., Felser, C. and Clahsen, H. 2005. ‘Gaps in second language processing’, Studies in Second Language Acquisition 27, 5378.Google Scholar
McRae, K. and Matsuki, K. 2013. ‘Constraint-based models of sentence processing’, in van Gompel, R. (ed.), pp. 5177.Google Scholar
Miyake, A. and Friedman, N. 1998. ‘Individual differences in second language proficiency: working memory as language aptitude’, in Healy, A. and Bourne, L. (eds), Foreign Language Learning, pp. 339364. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Omaki, A. and Schulz, B. 2011. ‘Filler-gap dependencies and island constraints in second language sentence processing’, Studies in Second Language Acquisition 33, 563588.Google Scholar
Osaka, M. and Osaka, N. 1992. ‘Language-independent working memory as measured by Japanese and English reading span tests’, Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30, 287289.Google Scholar
Papadopoulou, D. and Clahsen, H. 2003. ‘Parsing strategies in L1 and L2 sentence processing: a study of relative clause attachment in Greek’, Studies in Second Language Acquisition 25, 501528.Google Scholar
Pickering, M. J. and Garrod, S. 2013. ‘An integrated theory of language production and comprehension’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36, 329347.Google Scholar
Pickering, M. J. and Traxler, M. J. 1998. ‘Plausibility and recovery from garden-paths: an eye-tracking study’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 24, 940961.Google Scholar
Pritchett, B. 1992. Grammatical Competence and Parsing Performance. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Roberts, L., in preparation. Pronoun resolution in Turkish–Dutch bilinguals.Google Scholar
Roberts, L. and Felser, C. 2011. ‘Plausibility and recovery from garden paths in second language sentence processing’, Applied Psycholinguistics 32, 299331.Google Scholar
Roberts, L., Gullberg, M. and Indefrey, P. 2008. ‘Online pronoun resolution in L2 discourse: L1 influence and general learner effects’, Studies in Second Language Acquisition 30, 333357.Google Scholar
Roberts, L. and Liszka, S. A. 2013Processing tense/aspect-agreement violations on-line in the second language: a self-paced reading study with French and German L2 learners of English’, Second Language Research 29 (4), 413439.Google Scholar
Service, E., Simola, M., Metsanheimo, O. and Maury, S. 2002. ‘Bilingual working memory span is affected by language skill’, European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 14, 383408.Google Scholar
Slabakova, R. 2009. ‘The Fundamental Difference Hypothesis twenty years later’, Studies in Second Language Acquisition 31 (2), 155173.Google Scholar
Stowe, L. 1989. ‘Thematic structures and sentence comprehension’, in Carlson, G. and Tanenhaus, M. (eds), Linguistic Structure in Language Processing. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Townsend, D. J. and Bever, T. G. 2001. Sentence Comprehension. The Integration of Habits and Rules. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Traxler, M. J., Williams, R. S., Blozis, S. A. and Morris, R. K. 2005. ‘Working memory, animacy, and verb class in the processing of relative clauses’, Journal of Memory and Language 53, 204224.Google Scholar
Van Gompel, R. 2013. Sentence Processing. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Waters, G. S. and Caplan, D. 1996. ‘The measurement of verbal working memory capacity and its relation to reading comprehension’, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology 49, 5179.Google Scholar
Weinberg, A. 1999. ‘A minimalist theory of human sentence processing’, in Epstein, S. and Hornstein, N. (eds), Working Minimalism, pp. 283315. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Williams, J. 2006. ‘Incremental interpretation in second language sentence processing’, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 9, 7181.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Syntactic processing
  • Edited by Vivian Cook, University of Newcastle upon Tyne, Li Wei
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Linguistic Multi-Competence
  • Online publication: 05 May 2016
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107425965.011
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Syntactic processing
  • Edited by Vivian Cook, University of Newcastle upon Tyne, Li Wei
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Linguistic Multi-Competence
  • Online publication: 05 May 2016
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107425965.011
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Syntactic processing
  • Edited by Vivian Cook, University of Newcastle upon Tyne, Li Wei
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Linguistic Multi-Competence
  • Online publication: 05 May 2016
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107425965.011
Available formats
×