Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-2lccl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T14:53:58.536Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part VII - Intelligence and Its Role in Society

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 December 2019

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Alexandra, V. (2018). Predicting CQ development in the context of experiential cross-cultural training: The role of social dominance orientation and the propensity to change stereotypes. Academy of Management Learning and Education, 17(1), 6278. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amle.2015.0096Google Scholar
Alhashemi, S. E. (2014). Measuring emotional intelligence of university students: A comparison between China and Bahrain. International Journal of Social and Organizational Dynamics in IT, 3(4), 5976.Google Scholar
Ang, S., & Van Dyne, L. (2008). Conceptualization of cultural intelligence: Definition, distinctiveness, and nomological network. In Ang, S., & Van Dyne, L. (Eds.), Handbook of cultural intelligence: Theory, measurement, and applications (pp. 315). New York: Sharpe.Google Scholar
Ang, S., Van Dyne, L., & Koh, C. (2006). Personality correlates of the four-factor model of cultural intelligence. Group and Organization Management, 31(1), 100123.Google Scholar
Azuma, H., & Kashiwagi, K. (1987). Descriptions for an intelligent person: A Japanese study. Japanese Psychological Research, 29, 1726.Google Scholar
Bai, X., Liu, H., & Hu, X. (2007). Development of high school teachers and students’ view on implicit theories of intelligence. [Chinese]. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 5(2), 8185.Google Scholar
Beller, M., & Gafni, N. (1995). Equating and validating translated scholastic aptitude tests: The Israeli case. In Ben-Shakhar, G. & Lieblich, A. (Eds.), Studies in psychology in honor of Solomon Kugelmass (pp. 202219). Jerusalem: Magnes Press.Google Scholar
Brackett, M. A., & Mayer, J. D. (2003). Convergent, discriminate, and incremental validity of competing measures of EI. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(9), 11471158.Google Scholar
Brody, N. (1992). Intelligence (2nd ed.). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S., Shapiro, D., & Tagiuri, R. (1958). The meaning of traits in isolation and in combination. In Tagiuri, R. & Petrullo, L. (Eds.), Person perception and interpersonal behavior (pp.278288). Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Cai, X., & Jiang, L. (1995). A cross-cultural study of the intellectual concepts of junior and senior middle school students from five nationalities in Southwest China. [Chinese]. The Science of Psychology, 18, 346350.Google Scholar
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Dissou, G., Furnham, A., & Bales, A. (2010). Personality traits and lay conceptions of intelligence. In Villanueva, J. P. (Ed.), Personality traits: Classifications, effects and changes (psychology of emotions, motivations, and actions) UK ed Edition (pp. 109123). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Chen, X. P., Liu, D., & Portnoy, R. (2012). A multilevel investigation of motivational cultural intelligence, organizational diversity climate, and cultural sales: Evidence from US real estate firms. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97(1), 93106.Google Scholar
Chua, R. Y., & Ng, K. Y. (2017). Not just how much you know: Interactional effect of cultural knowledge and metacognition on creativity in a global context. Management and Organization Review, 13(2), 281300.Google Scholar
Chen, M. J. (1994). Chinese and Australian concepts of intelligence. Psychology and Developing Societies, 6, 101117.Google Scholar
Chen, C. S., Lee, S. Y., & Stevenson, H. W. (1996). Academic achievement and motivation of Chinese students: A cross-national perspective. In Lau, S. (Ed.). Growing up the Chinese way: Chinese child and adolescent development (pp. 6991). Hong Kong: Chinese University Press.Google Scholar
Chen, W. W., & Wong, Y. L. (2014). What my parents make me believe in learning: The role of filial piety in Hong Kong students’ motivation and academic achievement. International Journal of Psychology, 49(4), 249256.Google Scholar
Cole, M., Gay, J., & Glick, J. (1967). A cross-cultural study of clustering in free recall. Psychonomic Bulletin, 1(2), 18.Google Scholar
Confucius, (2010). The doctrine of the mean (trans. J. Leggs). Whitefish, MT: Kessinger Publishing. (Original translation published 1893.)Google Scholar
Crowne, K. A. (2008). What leads to cultural intelligence? Business Horizons, 51(5), 391399. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bushor.2008.03.010Google Scholar
Crowne, K. A. (2009). The relationships among social intelligence, emotional intelligence and cultural intelligence. Organization Management Journal, 6, 148163. https://doi.org/10.1057/omj.2009.20Google Scholar
Crowne, K. A. (2013). An empirical analysis of three intelligences. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue canadienne des sciences du comportement, 45(2), 105114. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029110.Google Scholar
Depaula, P. D., Azzollini, S. C., Cosentino, A. C., & Castillo, S. E. (2016). Personality, character strengths and cultural intelligence: “Extraversion” or “openness” as further factors associated to the cultural skills. Avances en Psicología Latinoamericana, 34(2), 415436.Google Scholar
Detterman, D. K. (1986). Qualitative integration: The last word? In Sternberg, R. J. & Detterman, D. K. (Eds.), What is intelligence: Contemporary viewpoints on its nature and definition (pp.163166). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing.Google Scholar
Earley, P. C. (2002). Redefining interactions across cultures and organizations: Moving forward with cultural intelligence. Research in Organizational Behavior, 24, 271299. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-3085(02)24008-3Google Scholar
Earley, P. C., & Mosakowski, E. (2004). Cultural intelligence. Harvard Business Review, 82(10), 139146.Google Scholar
Ekermans, G., & Privaatsak, X. (2009). Emotional intelligence across cultures: Theoretical and methodological considerations. In Stough, C., Saklofske, D. H., & Parker, J. (Eds.), Assessing emotional intelligence: Theory, research, and applications (pp. 259290). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Fang, F., & Keats, D. (1987). A cross-cultural study on the conception of intelligence. [Chinese.] Acta Psychologica Sinica, 19(3), 255262.Google Scholar
Fang, F., Schei, V., & Selart, M. (2018). Hype or hope? A new look at the research on cultural intelligence. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 66, 148171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2018.04.002Google Scholar
Frey, M. C., & Detterman, D. K. (2004). Scholastic assessment or g? The relationship between the scholastic assessment test and general cognitive ability. Psychological Science, 15(6), 373378.Google Scholar
Furnham, A., & Mkhize, N. (2004). Indian and Isi-Zulu-speaking South African parents’ estimates on their own and their children’s intelligence. South African Journal of Psychology, 34, 363384.Google Scholar
Fwu, B., Chen, S., Wei, C., & Wang, H. H. (2017). The mediating role of self-exertion on the effects of effort on learning virtues and emotional distress in academic failure in a Confucian context. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 10331089.Google Scholar
Gabel-Shemueli, R., Dolan, S., & Cerdin, J. (2005). Emotional Intelligence as predictor of cultural adjustment for success in global assignments. Career Development International, 10, 375395.Google Scholar
Garcia-Cepero, M. C., & McCoach, D. B. (2009). Educators’ implicit theories of intelligence and beliefs about the identification of gifted students. Universitas Psychologica, 8(2), 295310.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1993). Multiple intelligences: The theory in practice. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1995). Reflections on multiple intelligences: Myths and messages. Phi Delta Kappan, 77, 200203, 206209.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1998). A multiplicity of intelligences. Scientific American Presents, 9, 1823.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (2002). On the three faces of intelligence. Daedalus, 131(1), 139142.Google Scholar
Georgas, J., Van de Vijver, F. J. R., Weiss, L. G., & Saklofske, D. H. (2003). In Georgas, J., Weiss, L. G., Van de Vijver, F. J., & Saflofske, D. H. (Eds.), Culture and children’s intelligence: Cross-cultural analysis of the WISC-III (pp. 227240). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gill, R., & Keats, D. (1980). Elements of intellectual competence: Judgments by Australian and Malay university students. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 11, 233243.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (1997). You can’t take it with you: Why ability assessments don’t cross cultures. American Psychologist, 52, 11151124.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (1998). The cultural evolution of IQ. In Neisser, U. (Ed.), The rising curve: Long term gains in IQ and related measures (pp. 81123). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Grigorenko, E. L., Geissler, P. W., Prince, R., Okatcha, F. Nokes, C. et al. (2001). The organisation of Luo conceptions of intelligence: A study of implicit theories of Kenya village. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25(4), 367378.Google Scholar
Gunkel, M., Schlaegel, C., & Teras, V. (2016). Cultural values, emotional intelligence, and conflict handling styles: A global study. Journal of World Business, 51(4), 568585.)Google Scholar
Harrison, N. (2012). Investigating the impact of personality and early life experiences on intercultural interaction in internationalised universities. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 36(2), 224237. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2011.03.007Google Scholar
Hau, K. T., & Ho, I. T. (2012). Chinese students’ motivation and achievement. In Bond, M. H. (Ed.). The Oxford handbook of Chinese psychology (pp. 187204). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Holloway, S. D. (1988). Concepts of ability and effort in Japan and the United States. Review of Educational Research, 58(3), 327345.Google Scholar
Jensen, A. (1998). The suppressed relationship between IQ and the reaction time slope parameter of the Hick function. Intelligence, 26(1), 4352.Google Scholar
Kim, Y. J., & Van Dyne, L. (2012). Cultural intelligence and international leadership potential: The importance of contact for members of the majority. Applied Psychology, 61(2), 272294. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2011.00468.xGoogle Scholar
Ko, B., Boswell, B., & Yoon, S. (2015). Developing intercultural competence through global link experiences in physical education. Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy, 20(4), 366380. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17408989.2013.837441Google Scholar
Kopic, K., Vranic, A., & Zarevski, P. (2009). Implicit theories of intelligence in elementary school eighth-grade pupils. Drustvena Istrazivanja, 18(3), 503521.Google Scholar
Korzilius, H., Bücker, J. J., & Beerlage, S. (2017). Multiculturalism and innovative work behavior: The mediating role of cultural intelligence. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 56, 1324. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2016.11.001Google Scholar
Kwak, K. (2003). South Korea. In Georgas, J., Weiss, L. G., Van de Vijver, F. J., & Saflofske, D. H. (Eds.), Culture and children’s intelligence: Cross-cultural analysis of the WISC-III (227240). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lautrey, J., & de Ribaupierre, A. (2004). Psychology of human intelligence in France and French-Speaking Switzerland. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), International handbook of intelligence (pp. 104134). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lie, D., Suyasa, P. T., & Wijaya, E. (2016). The mediating role of cultural intelligence in the relationship between the openness to experience personality trait and job satisfaction among expatriates. Makara Human Behaviour Studies in Asia, 20(1), 4656. http://dx.doi.org/10.7454/mssh.v20i1.3486Google Scholar
Lim, W., Plucker, J. A., & Im, K. (2002). We are more alike than we think we are: Implicit theories of intelligence with a Korean sample, Intelligence, 30(2),185208.Google Scholar
MacNab, B. R., & Worthley, R. (2012). Individual characteristics as predictors of cultural intelligence development: The relevance of self-efficacy. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 36(1), 6271. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2010.12.001.Google Scholar
Mayer, J. D., DiPaolo, M., & Salovey, P. (1990). Perceiving the affective content in ambiguous visual stimuli: A component of emotional intelligence. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50, 772781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. (2000). Models of emotional intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Handbook of Intelligence (pp. 396420). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. (2002). Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT), Version 2.0. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. (2004). Emotional intelligence: Theory, findings, and implications. Psychological Inquiry, 15(3), 197215.Google Scholar
Moon, H. K., Choi, B. K., & Jung, J. S. (2012). Previous international experience, cross‐cultural training, and expatriates’ cross‐cultural adjustment: Effects of cultural intelligence and goal orientation. Human Resource Development Quarterly, 23(3), 285330. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hrdq.21131Google Scholar
Moon, H. K., Choi, B. K., & Jung, J. S. (2013). Comprehensive examination on antecedents of cultural intelligence: Case of South Korea. Personnel Review, 42(4), 440465. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/PR-10-2011-0146Google Scholar
Muñiz, J., Bartram, D., Evers, A., Boben, D., Matesic, K., Glabeke, K., et al. (2001). Testing practices in European countries. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 17(3), 201211.Google Scholar
Muñiz, J., Prieto, G., Almeida, L., & Bartram, D. (1999). Test use in Spain, Portugal and Latin American countriesEuropean Journal of Psychological Assessment, 15(2), 151157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027//1015-5759.15.2.151Google Scholar
Neisser, U. (1979). The concept of intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. & Detterman, D. T. (Eds.), Human intelligence: Perspectives on its theory and measurement (pp. 179189). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J., Boykin, A.W., Brody, N., Ceci, S. J. et al. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist, 51(2), 77101.Google Scholar
Niu, W., & Brass, J. (2011). Intelligence in worldwide perspective. In Kaufman, S & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of intelligence (pp. 623646). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Oakland, T. (2009). How universal are test development and use? In Grigorenko, E. (Ed.), Assessment of abilities and competencies in an era of globalization (pp. 140). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Oakland, T., Douglas, S., & Kane, H. (2016). Top ten standardized tests used internationally with children and youth by school psychologists in 64 countries: A 24 year follow-up study. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 34(2), 166176.Google Scholar
Paulhus, D. L., Wehr, P., Harms, P. D., & Strasser, D. I. (2002). Use of exemplar surveys to reveal implicit types of intelligence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 10511062.Google Scholar
Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. American Psychologist, 54(9), 741754.Google Scholar
Plato, (1992). The Republic (trans. G. M. A. Grube) (2nd ed.). Indianapolis: Hackett Publishing.Google Scholar
Rosenblatt, V., Worthley, R., & MacNab, B. (2013). From contact to development in experiential cultural intelligence education: The mediating influence of expectancy disconfirmation. Academy of Management Learning and Education, 12(3), 356379. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amle.2012.0199Google Scholar
Ruzgis, P. M., & Grigorenko, E. L. (1994). Cultural meaning systems, intelligence and personality. In Sternberg, R. J. & Ruzgis, P. (Eds.), Personality and intelligence (pp. 248270). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 9, 185211.Google Scholar
Salovey, P., & Pizarro, D. A. (2003). The value of emotional intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J., Lautrey, J., & Lubart, T. I. (Eds.), Models of intelligence: International perspectives (pp. 263278). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Saklofske, D. H., Van de Vijver, F. J. R., Oakland, T., Mpofu, E., & Suzuki, L. A. (2015). Intelligence and culture: History and assessment. In Goldstein, S, Princiotta, D, Naglieri, J (Eds.), Handbook of intelligence: Evolutionary theory, historical perspective, and current concepts (pp. 341365). New York: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-1562-0_22Google Scholar
Sato, T., Namiki, H., Ando, J., & Hatano, G. (2004). Japanese conception of and research on human intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), International handbook of intelligence (pp. 302324). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Serpell, R. (1974). Aspects of intelligence in a developing country. African Social Research, 17, 576596.Google Scholar
Serpell, R. (2000). Intelligence and culture. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 549580). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Serpell, R., & Jere-Folotiya, J. (2008). Developmental assessment, cultural context, gender, and schooling in Zambia. International Journal of Psychology, 43(2), 8896.Google Scholar
Srivastava, A. K., & Misra, G. (2001). Lay people’s understanding and use of intelligence: An Indian perspective. Psychology and Developing Societies, 13(1), 2549. https://doi.org/10.1177/097133360101300102Google Scholar
Stemler, S. E., & Sternberg, R. J. (2006). Using situational judgment tests to measure practical intelligence. In Weekley, J. A. & Ployhart, R. E. (Eds.), Situational judgment tests: Theory, measurement, and application (pp. 107131). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Implicit theories of intelligence, creativity, and wisdom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 607627.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1997). Successful intelligence. New York: Plume.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2003). Construct validity of the theory of successful intelligence. In Sternbger, R. J., Lautrey, J., & Lubart, T. I. (Eds): Models of intelligence: International perspectives (pp. 5577). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2004). Culture and intelligence. American Psychologist, 59, 325338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sternberg, R. J., Conway, B. E., Ketron, J. L., & Bernstein, M. (1981). People’s conceptions of intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41(1), 3755.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2006). Cultural intelligence and successful intelligence. Group and Organization Management, 31(1), 2739.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., Grigorenko, E. L., Ngorosho, D., Tantufuye, E., Mbise, A., Nokes, C, et al. (2002). Assessing intellectual potential in rural Tanzanian school children. Intelligence, 30(2), 141162.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., Nokes, C., Geissler, P. W., Prince, R., Okatcha, F., Bundy, D. A, et al. (2001). The relationship between academic and practical intelligence: A case study in Kenya. Intelligence, 29 (5), 401418.Google Scholar
Swami, V., Furnham, A., Maakip, I., Ahmad, M. S., Naw, N.H.M., Voo, P. S. K. et al. (2008). Beliefs about the meaning and measurement of intelligence: A cross-cultural comparison of American, British, and Malaysian undergraduates. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22(2), 235246.Google Scholar
Thorndike, R. L., & Stein, S. (1937). An evaluation of the attempts to measure social intelligence. Psychological Bulletin, 34, 275285. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0053850Google Scholar
Van de Vijver, F. J. R. (2003). Principles of adaptation of intelligence tests to other cultures. In Georgas, J., Weiss, L. G., Van de Vijver, F. J., & Saflofske, D. H. (Eds.), Culture and children’s intelligence: Cross-cultural analysis of the WISC-III (pp. 255263). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ueda, N. (1989). Japanese children’s personal theories of intelligence: A developmental study. Doctoral dissertation, Cambridge: Harvard University.Google Scholar
Wan, M., Li, N., & Jing, Q. (1997). A cross-cultural study on middle school and high school students’ implicit theories of intelligence: Comparison among Han, Tabitan, and Dongxiang Chinese students. [Chinese.] Psychological Development and Education. 2, 16.Google Scholar
Watkins, M. W., Glutting, J. J., & Lei, P.-W. (2007). Validity of the full-scale IQ when there is significant variability among WISC-III and WISC-IV factor scores. Applied Neuropsychology, 14(1), 1320.Google Scholar
White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297333.Google Scholar
Willmann, E., Feldt, K., & Amelang, M. (1997). Prototypical behavior patterns of social intelligences: An intercultural comparison between Chinese and German subjects. International Journal of Psychology, 32(5), 329346.Google Scholar
Wober, M. (1974). Towards an understanding of the Kigranda concept of intelligence. In Berry, J. W. & Dasen, P. R. (Eds.), Culture and cognition (pp. 261280). London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Yang, S. -Y., & Sternberg, R. J. (1997a). Conceptions of intelligence in ancient Chinese philosophy. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 17, 101119.Google Scholar
Yang, S. -Y., & Sternberg, R. J. (1997b). Taiwanese Chinese people’s conceptions of intelligence, Intelligence, 25, 2136.Google Scholar
Zhang, H., & Wu, Z. (1994). People’s conceptions of intelligence: A study of Beijing residents’ conceptions of intelligence. [Chinese.] The Science of Psychology, 17(2), 6569, 81.Google Scholar

References

Berry, J. W. (1974). Radical cultural relativism and the concept of intelligence. In Berry, J. W. & Dasen, P. R. (Eds.), Culture and cognition: Readings in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 225229). London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, J. J., & Austin, G. A. (1956). A study of thinking. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1941). Some theoretical issues in adult intelligence testing. Psychological Bulletin, 38, 592.Google Scholar
Cole, M., Gay, J., Glick, J. A., & Sharp, D. W. (1971). The cultural context of thinking and learning. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Dasen, P. R. (1984). The cross-cultural study of intelligence: Piaget and the Baoulé. International Journal of Psychology, 19, 407434.Google Scholar
Dasen, P. R. (2011). Culture, cognition and learning. In Nsamenang, A. B. & Tchombe, T. M. S. (Eds.), Handbook of African educational theories and practices (pp. 159174). Bamenda: Human Development Resource Center.Google Scholar
Glick, J. (1968). Cognitive style among the Kpelle. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Chicago.Google Scholar
Glick, J. (1974). Cognitive development in cross-cultural perspective. In Hetherington, E. M. (Ed.), Review of child development research (pp. 595654). Chicago: SRCD.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (1966). On culture and conservation. In Bruner, J. S., Olver, R. R., Greenfield, P. M., et al., Studies in cognitive growth (pp. 225256). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (1974). Cross-cultural research and Piagetian theory: Paradox and progress. Dossiers Pedagogiques, 34–39. English version in Riegel, K. F. & Meacham, J. A. (Eds.) (1976). The developing individual in a changing world, Vol. 1, Historical and cultural issues (pp. 322333). Paris: Mouton.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (1993). Representational competence in shared symbol systems: Electronic media from radio to video games. In Cocking, R. R. & Renninger, K. A. (Eds.), The development and meaning of psychological distance (pp. 161183). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (1997). You can’t take it with you: Why ability assessments don’t cross cultures. American Psychologist, 52, 11151124.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (1998). The cultural evolution of IQ. In Neisser, U. (Ed.), The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures (pp. 81123). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (1999). Cultural change and human development. In Turiel, E. (Ed.), Development and cultural change: Reciprocal processes. New Directions in Child Development, No. 83 (pp. 3760). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (2009a). Technology and informal education: What is taught, what is learned. Science, 323, 6971.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (2009b). Linking social change and developmental change: Shifting pathways of human development. Developmental Psychology, 45, 401418.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (2016) Social change, cultural evolution, and human development. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 8492.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (2018). Studying social change, culture, and human development: A theoretical framework and methodological guidelines. Developmental Review.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (2019). Communication technologies and social transformation: Their impact on human development. In Parke, R. & Elder, G. (Eds.), Children in changing worlds: Sociocultural and temporal perspectives (pp. 235–273). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M., & Childs, C. P. (1977). Weaving, color terms and pattern representation: Cultural influences and cognitive development among the Zinacantecos of Southern Mexico. Inter-American Journal of Psychology, 11, 2348.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M., Maynard, A. E., & Childs, C. P. (2003). Historical change, cultural learning, and cognitive representation in Zinacantec Maya children. Cognitive Development, 18, 455487.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M., & Quiroz, B. (2013). Context and culture in the socialization and development of personal achievement values: Comparing Latino immigrant, families, European American families, and elementary school teachers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 34,108118.Google Scholar
Grigorenko, E. L. et al. (2001). The organization of Luo conceptions of intelligence: A study of implicit theories in a Kenyan village. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 367378.Google Scholar
Hansen, N., Koudenburg, N., Hiersemann, R., Tellegen, P. J., Kocsev, M., & Postmes, T. (2012). Laptop usage affects abstract reasoning of children in the developing world. Computers in Education, 59, 9891000.Google Scholar
Huang, V. B., Greenfield, P. M., Zhou, C., & Wu, M. (in prep.). Intergenerational differences in child behaviors and parent socialization in United States.Google Scholar
Laughlin, R. (1975). The great Tzotzil dictionary of San Lorenzo Zinacantán. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution PressGoogle Scholar
Lave, J., Murtaugh, M., & de la Rocha, O. (1984). The dialectic of arithmetic in grocery shopping. In Rogoff, B. & Lave, J. (Eds.), Everyday cognition: Its development in social context (pp. 6794). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1976). Cognitive development: Its cultural and social foundations. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Manago, A. M., & Pacheco, P. (2019). Globalization and the transition to adulthood in a Maya community in Mexico: Communication technologies, social networks, and views on gender. New Directions in Child and Adolescent Development, 164, 115.Google Scholar
Maynard, A. E., & Greenfield, P. M. (2008). Women’s schooling and other ecocultural shifts: A longitudinal study of historical change among the Zinacantec Maya. Mind, Culture, and Activity,15, 165175.Google Scholar
Maynard, A. E., Greenfield, P. M., & Childs, C. P. (2015). Developmental effects of economic and educational change: Cognitive representation across 43 years in a Maya community. International Journal of Psychology, 50, 1219.Google Scholar
Maynard, A. E., Subrahmanyam, K., & Greenfield, P. M. (2005). Technology and the development of intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. & Preiss, D. (Eds.), Intelligence and technology: The impact of tools on the nature and development of human abilities (pp. 5497). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Mundy-Castle, A. C. (1974). Social and technological intelligence in Western and non-Western cultures. Universitas, 4, 4652. Reprinted in S. Pilowsky (Ed.) (1975). Cultures in collision. Adelaide: Australian Association for Mental Health.Google Scholar
Nsamenang, A. B. (2003). Conceptualizing human development and education in subSaharan Africa at the interface of endogenous and exogenous influences. In Sarasthwathi, T. S. (Ed.), Cross-cultural perspectives in human development: Theory, research, and applications (pp. 213235). New Delhi: Sage.Google Scholar
Nsamenang, A. B. (2006). Human ontogenesis: An indigenous African view on development and intelligence. International Journal of Psychology, 41, 293297.Google Scholar
Pearson. (2010). WISC-IV – Frequently asked questions. Pearson Education. http://images.pearsonclinical.com/images/Products/WISC-IV/4326-10_WISC_FAQ_pdf_f.pdfGoogle Scholar
Santos, H. I., Varnum, M. E. W., & Grossmann, I. (2017). Global increases in individualism. Psychological Science, 28, 12281239.Google Scholar
Saxe, G. (1999). Cognition, development, and cultural practices. In Turiel, E. (Ed.), Development and cultural change: Reciprocal processes. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, no. 83 (pp. 1935). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Scheibel, A. (1996). Introduction to symposium on the evolution of intelligence. Center for the Study of Evolution and the Origin of Life, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Scribner, S. (1984). Studying working intelligence. In Rogoff, B. & Lave, J. (Eds.), Everyday cognition: Its development in social context (pp. 940). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Serpell, R. (2011). Social responsibility as a dimension of intelligence, and as an educational goal: Insights from programmatic research in an African society. Child Development Perspectives, 5, 126133.Google Scholar
Serpell, R. (2017). How the study of cognitive growth can benefit from a cultural lens. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 889899.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2018). Why real-world problems go unresolved and what we can do about it: Inferences from a limited resource model of successful intelligence. Journal of Intelligence.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2000). Practical intelligence and its development. In Bar-On, R. & Parker, J. D. A. (Eds.), The handbook of emotional intelligence: Theory, development, assessment, and application at home, school, and in the workplace (pp. 215243). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., Nokes, C., Geissler, P. W., Prince, R., Okatcha, F., Bundy, D. A., Grigorenko, E. L. (2001). The relationship between academic and practical intelligence: A case study in Kenya. Intelligence, 29, 401418.Google Scholar
Tönnies, F. (1887/1957). Community and society (Gemeinschaft und Gesellschaft) (trans. C. P. Loomis). East Lansing: Michigan State University Press.Google Scholar
Vasquez-Salgado, Y., Greenfield, P. M., & Burgos-Cienfuegos, R. (2014). Exploring home-school value conflicts: Implications for academic achievement and well-being among Latino first-generation college students. Journal of Adolescent Research, 30, 135.Google Scholar
Wober, M. (1974). Towards an understanding of the Kiganda concept of intelligence. In Berry, J. W. & Dasen, P. R. (Eds.), Culture and cognition: Readings in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 261280). London: Methuen.Google Scholar
World Bank. (2018a). Data (wealth). World Bank. https://data.worldbank.org/country/kenyaGoogle Scholar
World Bank. (2018b). Data (secondary education). World Bank. https://data.worldbank.org/indicator/SE.SEC.ENRR?locationsKEGoogle Scholar
Zambrano, I., & Greenfield, P. (2004). Ethnoepistemologies at home and at school. In Sternberg, R. J. & Grigorenko, E. L. (Eds.), Culture and competence: Contexts of life success (pp. 251272). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar

References

Batterjee, A. A. (2011). Intelligence and education: The Saudi case. Mankind Quarterly 52, 133187.Google Scholar
Baxendale, S. (2010). The Flynn effect and memory function. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32, 699703.Google Scholar
Blair, C., Gamson, D., Thorne, S., & Baker, D. (2005). Rising mean IQ: Cognitive demand of mathematics education for young children, population exposure to formal schooling, and the neurobiology of the prefrontal cortex. Intelligence, 33, 93106.Google Scholar
Brand, C. R. (1990). A “gross” underestimate of a “massive” IQ rise? A rejoinder to Flynn. The Irish Journal of Psychology, 11, 5256.Google Scholar
Brand, C. R., Freshwater, S., & Dockrell, W. B. (1989). Has there been a “massive” rise in IQ levels in the West? Evidence for Scottish children. The Irish Journal of Psychology, 11, 4151.Google Scholar
Bratsberg, R., & Rogeberg, O. (2018). Flynn effect and its reversal are both environmentally caused. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115, 66746678.Google Scholar
Carey, J. (1992). The intellectuals and the masses: Pride and prejudice among the literary intelligentsia, 1880–1939. London: Faber.Google Scholar
Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., & Shell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures: A theoretical account of processing in the Raven progressive matrices test. Psychological Review, 97, 404431.Google Scholar
Ceci, S. J. (1991). How much does schooling influence general intelligence and its cognitive components? A reassessment of the evidence. Developmental Psychology, 27, 703722.Google Scholar
Colom, R., & Flores-Mendoz, C. E. (2007). Intelligence predicts scholastic achievement irrespective of SES factors: Evidence from Brazil. Intelligence, 35, 243251.Google Scholar
Cotton, S., Kiely, P., Crewther, D., Thompson, B., Laycock, R., & Crewther, S. (2005). A normative and reliability study of the Raven’s Coloured Matrices for primary school aged children from Victoria, Australia. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 647659.Google Scholar
Daley, T. C., Whaley, S. E., Sigman, M. D., Espinosa, M. P., & Neumann, C. (2003). IQ on the rise: The Flynn Effect in rural Kenyan children. Psychological Science, 14, 215219.Google Scholar
Dutton, E., Bakhiet, S. F. A., Osman, H. A., Becker, D., Essa, Y. A. S., Blahmar, T. A. M., Lynn, R., & Hakami, S. A. (2018). A Flynn effect in Khartoum, the Sudanese capital, 2004–2016. Intelligence, 68, 8286.Google Scholar
Dutton, E., & Lynn, R. (2013). A negative Flynn effect in Finland, 1997–2009. Intelligence, 41, 817820.Google Scholar
Dutton, E., & Lynn, R. (2015). A negative Flynn effect in France, 1999 to 2008–9. Intelligence, 51, 6770.Google Scholar
Dutton, E., Van der Linden, D., & Lynn, R. (2016). The negative Flynn effect: A systematic literature review. Intelligence, 59, 163169.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (1984). The mean IQ of Americans: Massive gains 1932 to 1978. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 2951.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (1985). Wechsler intelligence tests: Do we really have a criterion of mental retardation? American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 90, 236244.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (1987). Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 171191.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (1990). Massive IQ gains on the Scottish WISC: Evidence against Brand et al.’s hypothesis. The Irish Journal of Psychology, 11, 4151.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (1993). Skodak and Skeels: The inflated mother-child IQ gap. Intelligence, 17, 557561.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (1998). The schools: IQ tests, labels, and the word “intelligence.” In Carlson, J. S., Kingma, J., & Tomic, W. (Eds.), Advances in cognition and educational practice, Vol. 5, Conceptual issues in research on intelligence (pp. 1342). London: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (2009a). The WAIS-III and WAIS-IV: Daubert motions favor the certainly false over the approximately true. Applied Neuropsychology, 16, 17.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (2009b). What is intelligence? Beyond the Flynn effect. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (2012a). Are we getting smarter: Rising IQ in the twenty-first century. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (2012b). How to improve your mind: Twenty keys to unlock the modern world. London: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (2013). The “Flynn effect” and Flynn’s paradox. Intelligence, 41, 851857.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (2016a). Does your family make you smarter? Nature, nurture, and human autonomy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (2016b). The new torchlight list: In search of the best modern authors. Wellington, New Zealand: AWA Press.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (2017). No place to hide: Climate change – a short introduction for New Zealanders. Nelson, New Zealand: Potton & Burton.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R., & Rossi-Casé, L. (2011). Modern women match men on Raven’s Progressive Matrices. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 799803.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R., & Shayer, M. (2017). IQ decline and Piaget: Does the rot start at the top? Intelligence 66, 112121.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R., & Widaman, K. (2007). The Flynn effect and the shadow of the past: MR and the indefensible and indispensable role of IQ. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 35, 123151.Google Scholar
Florida, R. (2014). Where the good and bad jobs will be, 10 years from now. Vol. 25, City Lab.Google Scholar
Fox, M. C., & Mitchum, A. L. (2012). A knowledge based theory of rising scores on “culture-free” tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 9791000.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garber, H. L. (1988). The Milwaukee Project: Preventing mental retardation in children at risk. Washington, DC: American Association on Mental Retardation.Google Scholar
Genovese, J. E. (2002). Cognitive skills valued by educators: Historic content analysis of testing in Ohio. Journal of Educational Research, 96, 101114.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. A. (1998). The cultural evolution of IQ. In Neisser, U. (Ed.), The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures (pp. 81124). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Gresham, F. M., & Reschly, D. J. (2011). Standard of practice and Flynn effect testimony in death penalty cases. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 49, 131140.Google Scholar
Husén, T., & Tuijnman, A. (1991). The contribution of formal schooling to the increase in intellectual capital. Educational Researcher, 20, 1725.Google Scholar
Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g Factor: The science of mental ability. Westport, CT: PraegerGoogle Scholar
Johnson, S. (2005). Everything bad is good for you: How today’s popular culture is actually making us smarter. New York: Riverhead Books.Google Scholar
Kagitcibasi, C., & Biricik, D. (2011). Generational gains on the Draw-a-Person IQ scores: A three-decade comparison from Turkey. Intelligence, 39, 351356.Google Scholar
Kanaya, T., Scullin, M. H., & Ceci, S. J. (2003). The Flynn effect and U.S. policies: The impact of rising IQ scores on American society via mental retardation diagnoses. American Psychologist, 58, 778790.Google Scholar
Khaleefa, O., Sulman, A. & Lynn, R. (2009). An increase of intelligence in Sudan,1987–2007. Journal of Biosocial Science, 41, 279283.Google Scholar
Korgesaar, M. (2013). Flynni Efekti esinemine ee sti abiturientide seas Raveni Testi põhjal [Presence of Flynn Effect among Estonian school-leavers on Raven Matrices]. Tartu: University of Tartu, Institute of Psychology.Google Scholar
Kvarnöf, G. (2018). Interview with Martin Ingvar. Svenska Dagbladet, January 30.Google Scholar
Laird, C., & Whitaker, S. (2011). The use of IQ and descriptions of people with intellectual disabilities in the scientific literature. British Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 57, 175183.Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1976). Cognitive development: Its cultural and social foundations. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lynn, R. (1982). IQ in Japan and the United States shows a growing disparity. Nature, 297, 222223.Google Scholar
Lynn, R. (1998). In support of nutrition theory. In Neisser, U. (Ed.), The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures (pp. 2566). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Lynn, R., & Vanhanen, T. (2006). IQ and global inequality. Augusta, GA: Washington Summit Publishers.Google Scholar
Mahdi, W. (2011). Oil is no gold mine for jobs in Saudi. The National, March 2.Google Scholar
Martorell, R. (1998). Nutrition and the worldwide rise in IQ scores. In Neisser, U. (Ed.), The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures (pp. 183206). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Meisenberg, G., Lawless, E., Lambert, E., & Newton, A. (2005). The Flynn effect in the Caribbean: Generational change of cognitive test performance in Dominica. Mankind Quarterly, 46, 2969.Google Scholar
Mingroni, M. A. (2007). Resolving the IQ paradox: Heterosis as a cause of the Flynn Effect and other trends. Psychological Review, 114, 806829.Google Scholar
Must, O., & Must, A. (2013). Changes in test-taking patterns over time. Intelligence, 41, 780790.Google Scholar
NEA (National Endowment for the Arts). (2016). Results from the Annual Arts Basic Survey, August 2016. National Endowment for the Arts.Google Scholar
Pietschnig, J., & Gittler, G. (2015). A reversal of the Flynn effect for spatial perception in German-speaking countries: Evidence from a cross-temporal IRT-based meta-analysis. PloS One, 5, e14406.Google Scholar
Pietsching, J., Tran, U.S., & Voracek, M. (2013). Item-response theory modeling of IQ gains (the Flynn effect) on crystallized intelligence: Rodgers’ hypothesis yes, Brand’s hypothesis perhaps. Intelligence, 41, 791801.Google Scholar
Pietschnig, J., Voracek, M., & Formann, A.K. (2010). Pervasiveness of the IQ rise: A cross-temporal meta-analysis (1977–2014). Intelligence, 53, 145153.Google Scholar
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1989). Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales (Research Supplement No. 4). London: H. K. Lewis.Google Scholar
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1993). Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales (Section 1). Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Rönnlunda, M., & Nilsson, L.-G. (2009). Flynn effects on sub-factors of episodic and semantic memory: Parallel gains over time and the same set of determining factors. Neuropsychologia, 47, 21742180.Google Scholar
Rutter, J. M. (2000). Comments in discussion on James R. Flynn. In Bock, G. R., Goode, J. A., & Webb, K. (Eds.), The nature of intelligence (Novartis Foundation Symposium 233). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Schalock, R. L. (2012). User’s guide. Intellectual Disability: Definition, Classification, and Systems of Support (11th ed.). Washington, DC: AAIDD Books.Google Scholar
Schooler, C. (1998). Environmental complexity and the Flynn effect. In Neisser, U. (Ed.), The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures (pp. 6779). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Shiu, W., Beaujean, A. A., Must, O., te Nijenhuis, J., & Must, A. (2013). An item-level examination of the Flynn effect on the National Intelligence Test in Estonia. Intelligence, 41, 770779.Google Scholar
Skodak, M., & Skeels, H. M. (1949). A final follow-up study of 100 adopted children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 75, 85125.Google Scholar
Staff, R. T., Hogan, M. J., & Whalley, L. J. (2014). Aging trajectories of fluid intelligence in late life: The influence of age, practice and childhood IQ on Raven’s Progressive Matrices. Intelligence, 47, 194201.Google Scholar
Statista (2018). Percentage of the U.S. population who have completed four years of college or more from 1940 to 2017, by gender. Statisca Portal.Google Scholar
Steen, R. G. (2009). Human intelligence and medical illness: Assessing the Flynn effect. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Storfer, M. D. (1990). Intelligence and giftedness: The contributions of heredity and early environment. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Sundet, J. M., Barlaug, D. G., & Torjussen, T. M. (2004). The end of the Flynn effect? A study of secular trends in mean intelligence test scores of Norwegian conscripts during half a century. Intelligence, 32, 349362.Google Scholar
Teasdale, T. W, & Owen, D. R. (2008). Secular declines in cognitive test scores: A reversal of the Flynn Effect. Intelligence, 36, 121126.Google Scholar
te Nijenhuis, J., Cho, S. H., Murphy, R., & Lee, K H. (2012). The Flynn effect in Korea: Large gains. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 147151.Google Scholar
Tuddenham, R. (1948). Soldier intelligence in World Wars I and II. American Psychologist, 89, 5456.Google Scholar
Verguts, T., De, Boeck., & Maris, E. (1999). Generation speed in Raven’s Progressive Matrices Test. Intelligence, 27, 329345.Google Scholar
Weiss, L. G., Gregoire, J.,& Zhu, J. (2016). Flaws in Flynn effect research with the Wechsler Scales. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 34, 411420.Google Scholar
Wicherts, J. W., Dolan, C. V., Oosterveld, P., van Baal, G. C. V., Boomsma, D. I., & Span, M. M. (2014). Are intelligence tests measurement invariant over time? Investigating the nature of the Flynn effect. Intelligence, 32, 509537.Google Scholar
Woodley, M. A. (2011). Heterosis doesn’t cause the Flynn effect: A critical examination of Mingroni (2007). Psychological Review, 118, 689693.Google Scholar
Woodley, M. A., & Meisenberg, G. (2013). In the Netherlands the anti-Flynn effect is a Jensen effect. Personality and Individual Differences, 54, 872876.Google Scholar
Woodley, M. A., te Nijenhuis, J., & Murphy, R. (2013). Were the Victorians cleverer than us? The decline in general intelligence estimated from a meta-analysis of the slowing of simple reaction time. Intelligence, 41, 843850.Google Scholar
Young, G. W. (2012). Understanding Flynn and honoring Atkins: The Flynn effect should apply in capital determinations of mental retardation or intellectual disability. Vanderbilt Law Review, March.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B., & Mullally, P. R. (1997). Birth order: Reconciling conflicting effects. American Psychologist, 52, 685699.Google Scholar

References

Alderman, H., Behrman, J. R., Ross, D., & Sabot, R. (1996). The returns to endogenous human capital in Pakistan’s rural wage labor market. Oxford Bulletin of Economics and Statistics, 58, 2955.Google Scholar
Altinok, N., Angrist, N., & Patrinos, H. A. (2018). Global data set on education quality (1965–2015). Policy Research Working Paper No. WPS 8314. Washington, DC: World Bank.Google Scholar
Angrist, J., & Krueger, A. B. (1991). Does compulsory schooling affect schooling and earnings? Quarterly Journal of Economics, 106, 9791014.Google Scholar
Barnett, S. M., & Ceci, S. J. (2002). When and where do we apply what we learn? A taxonomy for far transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 612637.Google Scholar
Barnett, S. M., & Williams, W. M. (2004). National intelligence and the emperor’s new clothes. Contemporary Psychology, 49, 389396.Google Scholar
Barnett, S. M., & Williams, W. M. (2005). IQ-income data do not prove poor countries must remain poor. PsycCRITIQUES, 50(13).Google Scholar
Barnett, W. S., & Boocock, S. S. (Eds.). (1998). Early care and education for children in poverty. Promises, programs, and long-term results. Albany: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Becker, D., & Rindermann, H. (2016). The relationship between cross-national genetic distances and IQ-differences. Personality and Individual Differences, 98, 300310.Google Scholar
Becker, M., Lüdtke, O., Trautwein, U., Köller, O., & Baumert, J. (2007). The effect of schooling on psychometric intelligence: Does school quality make a difference? Unpublished paper. Berlin: Max-Planck-Institute for Human Development.Google Scholar
Bond, R., & Saunders, P. (1999). Routes of success: Influences on the occupational attainment of young British males. British Journal of Sociology, 50, 217249.Google Scholar
Burhan, N. A. S., Kurniawan, Y., Sidek, A. H., & Mohamad, M. R. (2014). Crimes and the Bell curve: The role of people with high, average, and low intelligence. Intelligence, 47, 1222.Google Scholar
Cahan, S., & Cohen, N. (1989). Age versus schooling effects on intelligence development. Child Development, 60, 12391249.Google Scholar
Canfield, R. L.Henderson, C. R., Jr., Cory-Slechta, D. A.Cox, C.Jusko, T. A., & Lanphear, B. P. (2003). Intellectual impairment in children with blood lead concentrations below 10 microg per deciliter. New England Journal of Medicine, 348, 15171526.Google Scholar
Caplan, B. (2017). The case against education. Princeton: University of Princeton Press.Google Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1987). Intelligence: Its structure, growth and action. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Ceci, S. J. (1991). How much does schooling influence general intelligence and its cognitive components? A reassessment of the evidence. Developmental Psychology, 27, 703722.Google Scholar
Ceci, S. J., & Williams, W. M. (1997). Schooling, intelligence, and income. American Psychologist, 52, 10511058.Google Scholar
Ceci, S. J., & Williams, W. M. (2009). Should scientists study race and IQ? Yes: The scientific truth must be pursued. Nature, 457, 788789.Google Scholar
Charlton, B. G. (2009). Replacing education with psychometrics. Medical Hypotheses, 73, 273277.Google Scholar
Coyle, T. R., Rindermann, H., & Hancock, D. (2016). Cognitive capitalism: Economic freedom moderates the effects of intellectual and average classes on economic productivity. Psychological Reports, 119, 411427.Google Scholar
Cunha, F., Heckman, J. J., Lochner, L., & Masterov, D. V. (2006). Interpreting the evidence on life cycle skill formation. In Hanushek, E. A. & Welch, F. (Eds.), Handbook of the economics of education, Vol.1 (pp. 697812). Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Deary, I. J., Batty, G. D., & Gale, C. R. (2008). Bright children become enlightened adults. Psychological Science, 19, 16.Google Scholar
Denny, K., & Doyle, O. (2008). Political interest, cognitive ability and personality: Determinants of voter turnout in Britain. British Journal of Political Science, 38, 291310.Google Scholar
Ellis, L., & Walsh, A. (2003). Crime, delinquency and intelligence: A review of the worldwide literature. In Nyborg, H. (Ed.), The scientific study of general intelligence. Tribute to Arthur R. Jensen (pp. 343365). Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J., & Schoenthaler, S. J. (1997). Raising IQ level by vitamin and mineral supplementation. In Sternberg, R. J. & Grigorenko, E. L. (Eds.), Intelligence, heredity and environment (pp. 363392). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (2007). What is intelligence? Beyond the Flynn effect. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. (2018). Reflections about intelligence over 40 years. Intelligence, 70, 7383.Google Scholar
Ganzach, Y. (2017). Cognitive ability and party affiliation: The role of the formative years of political socialization. Intelligence, 61, 5662.Google Scholar
Glaeser, E., Ponzetto, G., & Shleifer, A. (2007). Why does democracy need education? Journal of Economic Growth, 12, 7799.Google Scholar
Glewwe, P., & Kremer, M. (2006). Schools, teachers, and education outcomes in developing countries. In Hanushek, E. A. & Welch, F. (Eds.), Handbook of the economics of education (Vol. 2, pp. 9451017). Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Goodenough, F. L. (1926). Measurement of intelligence by drawings. Chicago: World Book.Google Scholar
Gottfredson, L., & Deary, I. J. (2004). Intelligence predicts health and longevity, but why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 14.Google Scholar
Hallpike, C. R. (1980). The foundations of primitive thought. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hansen, K. T., Heckman, J. J., & Mullen, K. J. (2004). The effect of schooling and ability on achievement test scores. Journal of Econometrics, 121, 3998.Google Scholar
Hanushek, E. A., & Woessmann, L. (2008). The role of cognitive skills in economic development. Journal of Economic Literature, 46, 607668.Google Scholar
Hanushek, E. A., & Woessmann, L. (2015). The knowledge capital of nations. Education and the economics of growth. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hanushek, E. A., Schwerdt, G., Wiederhold, S., & Woessmann, L. (2015). Returns to skills around the world: Evidence from PIAAC. European Economic Review, 73, 103130.Google Scholar
Harris, D. B. (1963). Children’s drawings as measures of intellectual maturity. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.Google Scholar
Hart, M. (2007). Understanding human history: An analysis including the effects of geography and differential evolution. Augusta, GA: Washington Summit.Google Scholar
Heckman, J. J., & Masterov, D. V. (2007). The productivity argument for investing in young children. NBER Working Paper No. 13016. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Educational Research.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The WEIRDEST people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183.Google Scholar
Holland, P. W., & Wainer, H. (Eds.). (1993). Differential item functioning. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Hunt, E. (2012). What makes nations intelligent? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(3), 284306.Google Scholar
Hunt, E., & Carlson, J. (2007). Considerations relating to the study of group differences in intelligence. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 194213.Google Scholar
Jones, G. (2016). Hive mind: How your nation’s IQ matters so much more than your own. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Klauer, K. J., & Phye, G. D. (2008). Inductive reasoning: A training approach. Review of Educational Research, 78, 85123.Google Scholar
Kura, K. (2013). Japanese north-south gradient in IQ predicts differences in stature, skin color, income, and homicide rate. Intelligence, 41, 512516.Google Scholar
Lakhanpal, M., & Ram, R. (2008). Educational attainment and HIV/AIDS prevalence: A cross-country study. Economics of Education Review, 27, 1421.Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1976). Cognitive development. Its cultural and social foundations. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lynn, R. (2009). What has caused the Flynn effect? Secular increases in the development quotients of infants. Intelligence, 37, 1624.Google Scholar
Lynn, R. (2010). National IQs updated for 41 nations. Mankind Quarterly, 50, 275296.Google Scholar
Lynn, R., & Meisenberg, G. (2010). The average IQ of sub-Saharan Africans: Comments on Wicherts, Dolan, and van der Maas. Intelligence, 28, 2129.Google Scholar
Lynn, R., & Vanhanen, T. (2001). National IQ and economic development: A study of eighty-one nations. Mankind Quarterly, 41, 415435.Google Scholar
Lynn, R., & Vanhanen, T. (2002). IQ and the wealth of nations. Westport, CT: Praeger.Google Scholar
Lynn, R., & Vanhanen, T. (2006). IQ and global inequality. Augusta GA: Washington Summit Publishers.Google Scholar
Lynn, R., & Vanhanen, T. (2012). Intelligence. A unifying construct for the social sciences. London: Ulster Institute for Social Research.Google Scholar
Meisenberg, G. (2012). National IQ and economic outcomes. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 103107.Google Scholar
Meisenberg, G. (2015). Verbal ability as a predictor of political preferences in the United States, 1974–2012. Intelligence, 50, 135143.Google Scholar
Melhuish, E. C., Phan, M. B., Sylva, K., Sam-mons, P., Siraj-Blatchford, I., & Taggart, B. (2008). Effects of the home learning environment and preschool center experience upon literacy and numeracy development in early primary school. Journal of Social Issues, 64, 95114.Google Scholar
Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Foy, P., & Hooper, M. (2016). TIMSS 2015: International results in mathematics. Chestnut Hill, MA: International Study Center.Google Scholar
Murnane, R. J., Willet, J. B., & Levy, F. (1995). The growing importance of reading comprehension cognitive skills in wage determination. Review of Economics and Statistics, 77, 251266.Google Scholar
Murray, Ch. (2008). Real education: Four simple truths for bringing America’s schools back to reality. New York: Crown Forum.Google Scholar
Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N., Ceci, S. J., et al. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist, 51, 77101.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E. (2009). Intelligence and how to get it. Why schools and cultures count. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Oesterdiekhoff, G. W. (2014). Evolution of law and justice from ancient to modern times. Journal on European History of Law, 5, 5464.Google Scholar
Piffer, D. (2015). A review of intelligence GWAS hits: Their relationship to country IQ and the issue of spatial autocorrelation. Intelligence, 53, 4350.Google Scholar
Psacharopoulos, G., & Patrinos, H. A. (2004). Returns to investment in education: A further update. Education Economics, 12, 111134.Google Scholar
Radding, C. (1985). A world made by men: Cognition and society, 400–1200. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina Press.Google Scholar
Rindermann, H. (2018). Cognitive capitalism: Human capital and the wellbeing of nations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rindermann, H., & Baumeister, A. E. E. (2015). Validating the interpretations of PISA and TIMSS tasks: A rating study. International Journal of Testing, 15, 122.Google Scholar
Rindermann, H., Becker, D., & Coyle, T. R. (2016). Survey of expert opinion on intelligence: Causes of international differences in cognitive ability tests. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 19.Google Scholar
Rindermann, H., & Carl, N. (2018). Human rights: Why countries differ. Comparative Sociology, 17, 2969.Google Scholar
Rindermann, H., & Ceci, S. J. (2009). Educational policy and country outcomes in international cognitive competence studies. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 551577.Google Scholar
Rindermann, H., & Ceci, S. J. (2018). Parents’ education is more important than their wealth in shaping their children’s intelligence: Results of 19 samples in seven countries at different developmental levels. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 41, 298326.Google Scholar
Rindermann, H., Sailer, M., & Thompson, J. (2009). The impact of smart fractions, cognitive ability of politicians and average competence of peoples on social development. Talent Development and Excellence, 1, 325.Google Scholar
Roivainen, W. (2010). European and American WAIS III norms: Cross-national differences in performance subtest scores. Intelligence, 38, 187192.Google Scholar
Rushton, J. P., Bons, T. A., Vernon, P. A., & Cvorovic, J. (2007). Genetic and environmental contributions to population group differences on the Raven’s Progressive Matrices estimated from twins reared together and apart. Proceedings of the Royal Society, 274, 17731777.Google Scholar
Rushton, J. P., & Templer, D. I. (2009). National differences in intelligence, crime, income, and skin color. Intelligence, 37, 341346.Google Scholar
Sandefur, J. (2016). Internationally comparable mathematics scores for fourteen African countries. Working Paper No. 444. Center for Global Development.Google Scholar
Spence, M. (1973). Job market signaling. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 87, 355374.Google Scholar
Stelzl, I., Merz, F., Remer, H., & Ehlers, T. (1995). The effect of schooling on the development of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A quasi-experimental study. Intelligence, 21, 279296.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2004). Culture and intelligence. American Psychologist, 59, 325338.Google Scholar
Strauss, J., & Thomas, D. (1998). Health, nutrition, and economic development. Journal of Economic Literature 36, 766817.Google Scholar
Thomson, G. H. (1937). Intelligence and civilisation: A Ludwig Mond lecture delivered at the University of Manchester on October 23, 1936. Journal of the University of Manchester, 1, 1838.Google Scholar
Warne, R. T., & Burningham, C. (2019). Spearman’s g found in 31 non-Western nations: Strong evidence that g is a universal phenomenon. Psychological Bulletin, 145, 237272.Google Scholar
Weede, E., & Kämpf, S. (2002). The impact of intelligence and institutional improvements on economic growth. Kyklos, 55, 361380.Google Scholar
Whetzel, D. L., & McDaniel, M. A. (2006). Prediction of national wealth. Intelligence, 34, 449458.Google Scholar
Wicherts, J. M., Borsboom, D., & Dolan, C. V. (2010). Evolution, brain size, and the national IQ of people around 3000 years B.C. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 104106.Google Scholar
Wicherts, J. M., & Dolan, C. V. (2010). Measurement invariance in confirmatory factor analysis: An illustration using IQ test performance of minorities. Educational Measurement: Issues and Practice, 29(3), 3947.Google Scholar
Wicherts, J. M., Dolan, C. V., Carlson, J. S., & van der Maas, H. L. J. (2010a). Raven’s Test performance of sub-Saharan Africans: Average performance, psychometric properties, and the Flynn effect. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 135–151.Google Scholar
Wicherts, J. M., Dolan, C. V., Carlson, J. S., & van der Maas, H. L. J. (2010b). Another failure to replicate Lynn’s estimate of the average IQ of sub-Saharan Africans. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 155–157.Google Scholar
Wicherts, J. M., Dolan, C. V., & van der Maas, H. L. J. (2010a). A systematic literature review of the average IQ of sub-Saharan Africans. Intelligence, 38, 120.Google Scholar
Wicherts, J. M., Dolan, C. V., & van der Maas, H. L. J. (2010b). The dangers of unsystematic selection methods and the representativeness of 46 samples of African test-takers. Intelligence, 38, 3037.Google Scholar
Wober, M. (1969). Distinguishing centri-cultural from cross-cultural tests and research. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 28, 488.Google Scholar
Woodworth, K. R., David, J. L., Guha, R., Wang, H., & Lopez-Torkos, A. (2008). San Francisco Bay Area KIPP schools: A study of early implementation and achievement. Final report. Menlo Park, CA: SRI International.Google Scholar

References

Allen, J. L., Oberdorster, G., Morris-Schaffer, K., Wong, C., Klocke, C., Sobolewski, M., et al. (2017). Developmental neurotoxicity of inhaled ambient ultrafine particle air pollution: Parallels with neuropathological and behavioral features of autism and other neurodevelopmental disorders. Neurotoxicology, 59, 140154.Google Scholar
Amato, M. S., Magzamen, S., Imm, P., Havlena, J. A., Anderson, H. A., Kanarek, M. S., & Moore, C. F. (2013). Early lead exposure (<3 years old) prospectively predicts fourth grade school suspension in Milwaukee, Wisconsin (USA). Environmental Research, 126, 6065.Google Scholar
Amato, M. S., Moore, C. F., Magzamen, S., Imm, P., Havlena, J. A., Anderson, H. A., & Kanarek, M. S. (2012). Lead exposure and educational proficiency: Moderate lead exposure and educational proficiency on end-of-grade examinations. Annals of Epidemiology, 22(10), 738743.Google Scholar
American Lung Association. (2019). Key findings: The State of the Air 2019. www.lung.org/our-initiatives/healthy-air/sota/key-findings/Google Scholar
Aylward, L. L., Hays, S. M., Kirman, C. R., Marchitti, S. A., Kenneke, J. F., English, C., et al. (2014). Relationships of chemical concentrations in maternal and cord blood: A review of available data. Journal of Toxicology and Environmental Health B Critical Reviews, 17(3), 175203.Google Scholar
Bellanger, M., Pichery, C., Aerts, D., Berglund, M., Castano, A., Cejchanova, M., et al. (2013). Economic benefits of methylmercury control in Europe: Monetary value of neurotoxicity prevention. Environmental Health, 12, 3.Google Scholar
Bellinger, D. C. (2012). A strategy for comparing the contributions of environmental chemicals and other risk factors to neurodevelopment of children. Environmental Health Perspectives, 120(4), 501507.Google Scholar
Bellinger, D. C. (2018). Applying methods of the Global Burden of Diseases, Injuries, and Risk Factors Study to developmental neurotoxicants: A commentary. Environmental Health, 17, 53.Google Scholar
Bellinger, D. C., Matthews-Bellinger, J. A., & Korkas, K. (2016). A developmental perspective on early-life exposure to neurotoxicants. Environment International, 94, 103112.Google Scholar
Bouchard, M. F., Chevrier, J., Harley, K. G., Kogut, K., Vedar, M., Calderon, N., et al. (2011). Prenatal exposure to organophosphate pesticides and IQ in 7-year-old children. Environmental Health Perspectives, 119(8), 11891195.Google Scholar
Boutwell, B. B., Nelson, E. J., Qian, Z., Vaughn, M. G., Wright, J. P., Beaver, K. M., et al. (2017). Aggregate-level lead exposure, gun violence, homicide, and rape. PLoS One, 12(11), e0187953.Google Scholar
Braun, J. (2017). Early-life exposure to EDCs: Role in childhood obesity and neurodevelopment. Nature Reviews Endocrinology, 13(3), 161173.Google Scholar
Braun, J. M., Kahn, R. S., Froehlich, T., Auinger, P., & Lanphear, B. P. (2006). Exposures to environmental toxicants and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in U.S. children. Environmental Health Perspectives, 114(12), 19041909.Google Scholar
Brubaker, C. J., Schmithorst, V. J., Haynes, E. N., Dietrich, K. N., Egelhoff, J. C., Lindquist, D. et al. (2009). Altered myelination and axonal integrity in adults with childhood lead exposure: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Neurotoxicology, 30(6), 867875.Google Scholar
Byers, R. K., & Lord, E. E. (1943). Late effects of lead poisoning on mental development. American Journal of Diseases of Children, 66(5), 471494.Google Scholar
Calderón-Garcidueñas, L., Gónzalez-Maciel, A., Reynoso-Robles, R., Delgado-Chávez, R., Mukherjee, P. S., Kulesza, R. J., et al. (2018). Hallmarks of Alzheimer disease are evolving relentlessly in Metropolitan Mexico City infants, children and young adults. APOE4 carriers have higher suicide risk and higher odds of reaching NFT stage V at ≤ 40 years of age. Environmental Research, 164, 475487.Google Scholar
Calderón-Garcidueñas, L., Reynoso-Robles, R., Vargas-Martínez, J., Gómez-Maqueo-Chew, A., Pérez-Guillé, B., Mukherjee, P. S., et al. (2016). Prefrontal white matter pathology in air pollution exposed Mexico City young urbanites and their potential impact on neurovascular unit dysfunction and the development of Alzheimer’s disease. Environmental Research, 146, 404417.Google Scholar
Cecil, K. M., Brubaker, C. J., Adler, C. M., Dietrich, K. N., Altaye, M., Egelhoff, J. C., et al. (2008). Decreased brain volume in adults with childhood lead exposure. PLoS Medicine, 5(5), e112.Google Scholar
Cecil, K. M., Dietrich, K. N., Altaye, M., Egelhoff, J. C., Lindquist, D. M., Brubaker, C. J., & Lanphear, B. P. (2011). Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy in adults with childhood lead exposure. Environmental Health Perspectives, 119(3), 403408.Google Scholar
Choi, A. L., Cordier, S., Weihe, P., & Grandjean, P. (2008). Negative confounding in the evaluation of toxicity: The case of methylmercury in fish and seafood. Critical Reviews in Toxicology, 38(10), 877893.Google Scholar
Choi, B. H. (1989). The effects of methylmercury on the developing brain. Progress in Neurobiology, 32(6), 447470.Google Scholar
Choi, W. J., Kwon, H. J., Lim, M. H., Lim, J. A., & Ha, M. (2016). Blood lead, parental marital status and the risk of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in elementary school children: A longitudinal study. Psychiatry Research, 236, 4246.Google Scholar
Clifford, A., Lang, L., Chen, R., Anstey, K. J., & Seaton, A. (2016). Exposure to air pollution and cognitive functioning across the life course: A systematic literature review. Environmental Research, 147, 383398.Google Scholar
David, O., Clark, J., & Voeller, K. (1972). Lead and hyperactivity. Lancet, 2(7783), 900903.Google Scholar
Davidson, P. W., Myers, G. J., Cox, C., Axtell, C., Shamlaye, C., Sloane-Reeves, J., et al. (1998). Effects of prenatal and postnatal methylmercury exposure from fish consumption on neurodevelopment: Outcomes at 66 months of age in the Seychelles Child Development Study. Journal of the American Medical Association, 280(8), 701707.Google Scholar
Delgado, C. F., Ullery, M. A., Jordan, M., Duclos, C., Rajagopalan, S., & Scott, K. (2017). Lead exposure and developmental disabilities in preschool-aged children. Journal of Public Health Management Practice, 24(2), e10e17.Google Scholar
Eskenazi, B., An, S., Rauch, S. A., Coker, E. S., Maphula, A., Obida, M., et al. (2018). Prenatal exposure to DDT and pyrethroids for malaria control and child neurodevelopment: The VHEMBE cohort, South Africa. Environmental Health Perspectives, 126(4), 047004.Google Scholar
Evens, A., Hryhorczuk, D., Lanphear, B. P., Rankin, K. M., Lewis, D. A., Forst, L., & Rosenberg, D. (2015). The impact of low-level lead toxicity on school performance among children in the Chicago public schools: A population-based retrospective cohort study. Environmental Health, 14, 21.Google Scholar
Factor-Litvak, P., Insel, B., Calafat, A. M., Liu, X., Perera, F., Rauh, V. A., & Whyatt, R. M. (2014). Persistent associations between maternal prenatal exposure to phthalates on child IQ at age 7 years. PLoS One, 9(12), e114003.Google Scholar
Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., & Lynskey, M. T. (1997). Early dentine lead levels and educational outcomes at 18 years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 471478.Google Scholar
Froehlich, T. E., Lanphear, B. P., Auinger, P., Hornung, R., Epstein, J. N., Braun, J., & Kahn, R. S. (2009). Association of tobacco and lead exposures with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics, 124(6), e1054e1063.Google Scholar
Gąssowska, M., Baranowska-Bosiacka, I., Moczydłowska, J., Tarnowski, M., Pilutin, A., Gutowska, I., et al. (2016). Perinatalexposure to lead (Pb) promotes Tau phosphorylation in the rat brain in a GSK-3β and CDK5 dependent manner: Relevance to neurological disorders. Toxicology, 347349, 1728.Google Scholar
GBD 2016 Risk Factor Collaborators. (2017). Global, regional, and national comparative risk assessment of 84 behavioural, environmental and occupational, and metabolic risks or clusters of risks, 1990–2016: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet, 390, 13451442.Google Scholar
Gibb, H., & O’Leary, K. G. (2014). Mercury exposure and health impacts among individuals in the artisanal and small-scale goldmining community: A comprehensive review. Environmental Health Perspectives, 122(7), 667672.Google Scholar
Goodlad, J. K., Marcus, D. K., & Fulton, J. J. (2013). Lead and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(3), 417425.Google Scholar
Grandjean, P., Weihe, P., White, R. F., Debes, F., Araki, S., Yokoyama, K., et al. (1997). Cognitive deficit in 7-year-old children with prenatal exposure to methylmercury. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 19(6), 417428.Google Scholar
Harada, M. (1995). Minamata disease: Methylmercury poisoning in Japan caused by environmental pollution. Critical Reviews in Toxicology, 25(1), 124.Google Scholar
Health Effects Institute. (2019). State of Global Air 2019 (website). www.stateofglobalair.org/airGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, A. S., Zhou, X., Reynolds, M. R., Kaufman, N. L., Green, G. P., & Weiss, L. G. (2014). The possible societal impact of the decrease in U.S. blood lead levels on adult IQ. Environmental Research, 132, 413420.Google Scholar
Kordas, K., Canfield, R. L., López, P., Rosado, J. L., Vargas, G. G., Cebrián, M. E., et al. (2006). Deficits in cognitive function and achievement in Mexican first-graders with low blood lead concentrations. Environmental Research, 100(3), 371386.Google Scholar
Lam, J., Lanphear, B. P., Bellinger, D., Axelrad, D. A., McPartland, J., Sutton, P., et al. (2017). Developmental PBDE exposure and IQ/ADHD in childhood: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Environmental Health Perspectives, 125(8), 086001.Google Scholar
Lanphear, B. P., Hornung, R., Khoury, J., Yolton, K., Baghurst, P., Bellinger, D. C., et al. (2005). Low-level environmental lead exposure and children’s intellectual function: An international pooled analysis. Environmental Health Perspectives, 113(7), 894899.Google Scholar
Levie, D., Korevaar, T. I. M., Bath, S. C., Dalmau-Bueno, A., Murcia, M., Espada, M., et al. (2018). Thyroid function in early pregnancy, child IQ, and autistic traits: A meta-analysis of individual-participant data. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 103(8), 29672979.Google Scholar
Liew, Z., Goudarzi, H., & Oulhote, Y. (2018). Developmental exposures to perfluoroalkyl substances (PFASs): An update of associated health outcomes. Current Environmental Health Reports, 5(1), 119.Google Scholar
Magzamen, S., Amato, M. S., Imm, P., Havlena, J. A., Coons, M. J., Anderson, H. A., et al. (2015). Quantile regression in environmental health: Early life lead exposure and end-of-grade exams. Environmental Research, 137, 108119.Google Scholar
Magzamen, S., Imm, P., Amato, M. S., Havlena, J. A., Anderson, H. A., Moore, C. F., & Kanarek, M. S. (2013). Moderate lead exposure and elementary school end-of-grade examination performance. Annals of Epidemiology, 23(11), 700707.Google Scholar
Miranda, M. L., Kim, D., Reiter, J., Galeano, M. A. O., & Maxson, P. (2009). Environmental contributors to the achievement gap. Neurotoxicology, 30(6), 10191024.Google Scholar
Nakiwala, D., Peyre, H., Heude, B., Bernard, J. Y., Béranger, R., Slama, R., & Philippat, C. (2018). In-utero exposure to phenols and phthalates and the intelligence quotient of child study group: Boys at 5 years. Environmental Health, 17(1), 17.Google Scholar
Needleman, H. L., Gunnoe, C., Leviton, A., Reed, R., Peresie, H., Maher, C., & Barrett, P. (1979). Deficits in psychologic and classroom performance of children with elevated dentine lead levels. New England Journal of Medicine, 300(13), 689695.Google Scholar
Needleman, H. L., Schell, A., Bellinger, D., Leviton, A., & Allred, E. N. (1990). The long-term effects of exposure to low doses of lead in childhood: An 11-year follow-up report. New England Journal of Medicine, 322(2), 8388.Google Scholar
Nevin, R. (2009). Trends in preschool lead exposure, mental retardation, and scholastic achievement: Association or causation? Environmental Research, 109(3), 301310.Google Scholar
Nigg, J. T., Knottnerus, G. M., Martel, M. M., Nikolas, M., Cavanagh, K., Karmaus, W., & Rappley, M. D. (2008). Low blood lead levels associated with clinically diagnosed attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and mediated by weak cognitive control. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 325331.Google Scholar
Nigg, J. T., Nikolas, M., Knottnerus, G. M., Cavanagh, K., & Friderici, K. (2010). Confirmation and extension of association of blood lead with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and ADHD symptom domains at population-typical exposure levels. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 5865.Google Scholar
Oken, E., Wright, R. O., Kleinman, K. P., Bellinger, D., Amarasiriwardena, C. J., Hu, H., et al. (2005). Maternal fish consumption, hair mercury, and infant cognition in a U.S. Cohort. Environmental Health Perspectives, 113(10), 13761380.Google Scholar
Park, J. H., Seo, J. H., Hong, Y. S., Kim, Y. M., Kang, J. W., Yoo, J. H., et al. (2016). Blood lead concentrations and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in Korean children: A hospital-based case control study. BMC Pediatrics, 16(1), 156.Google Scholar
Peterson, B. S., Rauh, V. A., Bansal, R., Hao, X., Toth, Z., Nati, G., et al. (2015). Effects of prenatal exposure to air pollutants (polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons) on the development of brain white matter, cognition, and behavior in later childhood. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(6), 531540.Google Scholar
Pujol, J., Martínez-Vilavella, G., Macià, D., Fenoll, R., Alvarez-Pedrerol, M., Rivas, I., et al. (2016). Traffic pollution exposure is associated with altered brain connectivity in school children. Neuroimage, 129, 175184.Google Scholar
Rauh, V. A., Perera, F. P., Horton, M. K., Whyatt, R. M., Bansal, R., Hao, X., et al. (2012). Brain anomalies in children exposed prenatally to a common organophosphate pesticide. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(20), 78717876.Google Scholar
Rauh, V., Arunajadai, S., Horton, M., Perera, F., Hoepner, L., Barr, D. B., & Whyatt, R. (2011). Seven-year neurodevelopmental scores and prenatal exposure to chlorpyrifos, a common agricultural pesticide. Environmental Health Perspectives, 119(8), 11961201.Google Scholar
Reuben, A., Caspi, A., Belsky, D. W., Broadbent, J., Harrington, H., Sugden, K., et al. (2017). Association of childhood blood lead levels with cognitive function and socioeconomic status at age 38 years and with IQ change and socioeconomic mobility between childhood and adulthood. Journal of the American Medical Association, 317(12), 12441251.Google Scholar
Romano, M. E., Eliot, M. N., Zoeller, R. T., Hoofnagle, A. N., Calafat, A. M., Karagas, M. R., et al. (2018). Maternal urinary phthalate metabolites during pregnancy and thyroid hormone concentrations in maternal and cord sera: The HOME Study. International Journal of Hygiene and Environmental Health, 221(4), 623631.Google Scholar
Schneider, J. S., & DeKamp, E. (2007). Postnatal lead poisoning impairs behavioral recovery following brain damage. Neurotoxicology, 28(6), 11531157.Google Scholar
Selevan, S. G., Kimmel, C. A., & Mendola, P. (2000). Identifying critical windows of exposure for children’s health. Environmental Health Perspectives, 108(Suppl 3), 451455.Google Scholar
Seo, J., Lee, B. K., Jin, S. U., Jang, K. E., Park, J. W., Kim, Y. T., et al. (2015). Altered executive function in the lead-exposed brain: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurotoxicology, 50, 19.Google Scholar
Skerfving, S., Lofmark, L., Lundh, T., Mikoczy, Z., & Stromberg, U. (2015). Late effects of low blood lead concentrations in children on school performance and cognitive functions. Neurotoxicology, 49, 114120.Google Scholar
Surkan, P. J., Zhang, A., Trachtenberg, F., Daniel, D. B., McKinlay, S., & Bellinger, D. C. (2007). Neuropsychological function in children with blood lead levels <10 microg/dL. Neurotoxicology, 28(6), 11701177.Google Scholar
Swan, S. H., Liu, F., Hines, M., Kruse, R. L., Wang, C., Redmon, J. B., et al. (2010). Prenatal phthalate exposure and reduced masculine play in boys. International Journal of Andrology, 33(2), 259269.Google Scholar
Téllez-Rojo, M. M., Bellinger, D. C., Arroyo-Quiroz, C., Lamadrid-Figueroa, H., Mercado-García, A., Schnaas-Arrieta, L., et al. (2006). Longitudinal associations between blood lead concentrations lower than 10 microg/dL and neurobehavioral development in environmentally exposed children in Mexico City. Pediatrics, 118(2), e323e330.Google Scholar
Wang, H. L., Chen, X. T., Yang, B., Ma, F. L., Wang, S., Tang, M. L., et al. (2008). Case-control study of blood lead levels and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in Chinese children. Environmental Health Perspectives, 116(10), 14011406.Google Scholar
Weiss, B., Clarkson, T. W., & Simon, W. (2002). Silent latency periods in methylmercury poisoning and in neurodegenerative disease. Environmental Health Perspectives, 110(Suppl 5), 851854.Google Scholar
White, R. F, Palumbo, C. L., Yurgelun-Todd, D. A., Heaton, K. J., Weihe, P., Debes, F., & Grandjean, P. (2011). Functional MRI approach to developmental methylmercury and polychlorinated biphenyl neurotoxicity. Neurotoxicology, 32(6), 975980.Google Scholar
WHO (World Health Organization). (2009). Ten chemicals of major public health concern. www.who.int/ipcs/assessment/public_health/chemicals_phc/en/Google Scholar
Woodruff, T. J., Zota, A. R., & Schwartz, J. M. (2011). Environmental chemicals in pregnant women in the United States. Environmental Health Perspectives, 119(6), 878885.Google Scholar
Wright, J. P., Dietrich, K. N., Ris, M. D., Hornung, R. W., Wessel, S. D., Lanphear, B. P., Ho, M., & Rae, M. N. (2008). Association of prenatal and childhood blood lead concentrations with criminal arrests in early adulthood. PLoS Medicine, 5(5), e101.Google Scholar
Yuan, W., Holland, S. K., Cecil, K. M., Dietrich, K. N., Wessel, S. D., Altaye, M., et al. (2006). The impact of early childhood lead exposure on brain organization: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of language function. Pediatrics, 118(3), 971977.Google Scholar
Zheng, W., Aschner, M., & Ghersi-Egea, J. F. (2003). Brain barrier systems: A new frontier in metal neurotoxicological research. Toxicology and Applied Pharmacology, 192(1), 111.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×